Housing, Theory and Society: Publication Details, Including Instructions For Authors and Subscription Information
This article argues that the sociology of housing and family needs a post-humanist approach because homes are now shared not just by humans but companion animals as well. It presents survey data showing companion animals are widely regarded as family members. It also discusses how the presence of companion animals has changed the nature of domestic life, housing design, and human well-being. The article concludes by examining more radical examples of human-animal cohabitation and mutual influence, like people living with house rabbits.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0 ratings0% found this document useful (0 votes)
163 views21 pages
Housing, Theory and Society: Publication Details, Including Instructions For Authors and Subscription Information
This article argues that the sociology of housing and family needs a post-humanist approach because homes are now shared not just by humans but companion animals as well. It presents survey data showing companion animals are widely regarded as family members. It also discusses how the presence of companion animals has changed the nature of domestic life, housing design, and human well-being. The article concludes by examining more radical examples of human-animal cohabitation and mutual influence, like people living with house rabbits.
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 21
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
This article was downloaded by: [Toma, Claudiu]
On: 7 December 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 930761521] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37- 41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Housing, Theory and Society Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t713699832 Be[a]ware of the Dog: A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing Adrian Franklin a a University of Tasmania, Australia To cite this Article Franklin, Adrian(2006) 'Be[a]ware of the Dog: A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing', Housing, Theory and Society, 23: 3, 137 156 To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/14036090600813760 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14036090600813760 Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material. Be[a]ware of the Dog: A Post- Humanist Approach to Housing ADRIAN FRANKLIN University of Tasmania, Australia ABSTRACT Alongside much talk of the dissolution of a nature /culture binary view of the world, there is also, symmetrically, considerable change observed in the performance of relations with non-humans and the proliferation of hybrids (Latour 1993, Haraway 2003). Through an examination of why and how humans and companion species have begun to live with each other in new ways this paper will challenge (at least) two of those sociological disciplines currently governed by humanist ontologies. It suggests that the sociology of the family and the sociology of housing need a new post-humanist makeover, for it is increasingly doubtful whether either are exclusively human domains. This is because neither families, households or housing can be thought of any longer as humans among themselves. Companion animals are now found not only in the vast majority of human households / families but their position, role, agency and status has shifted quite profoundly. Using data from a national survey of humananimal relations in Australia it will be shown that companion animals are widely regarded as, and act as, family members and that they occupy housing in profoundly different ways 1 . The paper argues that this new period of intimacy also ushers in the potential for greater mutual becomings (or co(a)gency to use Michaels term 2 ) as both companion species and their humans (together with their technonatural contexts) explore even more possibilities of co-presence. The paper concludes with an example of this, taken from the House Rabbit Society: a radical and ever more popular experiment in becomingrabbitbecominghuman (to use a Deleuzian convention) 3 . KEY WORDS: Post-humanism, Dogs, Human-animal relations, Family, Housing, Rabbits As an intellectual and theoretical object, housing conjures up, par excellence, the humanist-modernist project: human progress is its object and an anthropocentric world order is its principal outcome. Housing is by and for humans, obviously, and housing theory has largely confined its attention to theories of provision, distribution and meaning (to humans) and social construction, with a light flurry of aesthetic content here and there. But if housing is necessarily related to those contemporary social and cultural conditions that affect what it is to dwell as a human 4 , what it is to be properly and happily housed and the everyday nature of domestic lifestyle (or housing and ways of life) then those framing humanist themes Correspondence Address: Adrian Franklin, School Sociology, University of Tasmania, Private Bag 17, Hobart, Tas 7001, Australia. Tel.: +03 62267241. Fax: +03 62262279. Email: Adrian.Franklin@utas. edu.au Housing, Theory and Society, Vol. 23, No. 3, 137156, 2006 1403-6096 Print/1651-2278 Online/06/030137156 # 2006 Taylor & Francis DOI: 10.1080/14036090600813760 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 seem only too limiting and narrow; barely even a beginning. In times of liquid modernity, makeover culture and an experimental, playful and open-ended domesticity, we must begin to bring in perspectives that can cope with this complexity (see Urry 2003, Law and Mol 2002), with its relational materialism (Law 1999), its sociotechnical hybridity (Michael 2000) and semiotics (Latour 1993). If we look closely at people and their homes or what happens in homes from the perspective of what people do, or in John Laws (1994) terms deploy a sociology of verbs rather than nouns, then one of the first and most striking things we find in almost every home in the western world and beyond is that homes are not home just to humans but that they are home to humans living very closely and purposefully with other species, particularly with cats and dogs 5 . Other non-humans also figure in these heterogeneous assemblages: machines, technologies, texts, policies, restrictive covenants, cleaning agents and the agents cleansed. It goes without saying that such stories cannot properly be told with including the full cast of supporting actors. Mike Michaels (2000) salutary advice here (to view actors as necessarily involved in heterogeneous assemblages rather than free-standing and separable), particularly as it is demonstrated in the Hudogleddog essay is acknowledged, if not fully heeded, here. In this essay, however, I will confine my attention to the way humanist housing has been undermined by the increasingly intertwined nature of domestic life with companion species. The sociology of the family also remains resolutely humanist, though as a sub- discipline whose object has always been in permanent crisis and collapse it is hardly surprising that eventually those tensions have been resolved through recourse to the non-human world. Companionship, friendship, love and even community are words that have been rescued for many through new relationships with companion animals. Despite a growing literature that confirms this we have not seen serious sociological research investigate these new anthrozoological formations. We do not even have a systematic description of the communications or ethogram between these two species though one exists for species such as humans and dogs, separately (Smutts 2001). What follows is a beginning. The paper is arranged as follows. Firstly, it describes in fairly crude terms the extent of this multi-species occupation of housing and the changing designation from pet to companionate family member. Secondly, it accounts for this relatively new phenomenon by relating it to fairly standard dimensions of housing and family household formation, contemporary family structures, contemporary community life, ontological security, risk and, to use another of Baumans terms, liquid love. Thirdly, it argues that the nature of relationships with companion species has changed from instrumental to companionate to familial relations, and, that during the course of these shifts both species have had opportunities to make further experiments with each other, not least in how they live and are housed together. Survey data and interviews with veterinarians are used to support this claim. Finally the paper discusses how domestic life and other family and housing issues are affected by the accommodation of companion species and the emergence of trans- species housing. These include that changing nature of spatial use and boundaries within the home; new technologies and architectures of the multi-species house and garden; the changing nature of sociability and communal life; the tangible benefits to human health and well being; the implications of the latter for elderly people, 138 A. Franklin D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 children and the socially isolated and its translation into new areas of policy initiative. In short the socio-technical-semiotic complex that is housing and family can be shown to have affected a significant shift from a humanist to a post-humanist form. The paper will draw on a variety of empirical sources, but notably the first ever national survey of human animal relations conducted in Australia (Franklin 2006), the growing literature on general relationships with companion animals from the Anglophone world, the special literature relating to the health benefits of companion animals (from the USA, Australia and the UK and, finally, work relating to the special case of the American House Rabbit Society Multi-Species Households A recent trip to the national folk Museum in Cardiff, Wales, UK, reminded me that Western cultures have lived with and not just adjacent to animals for a very long time. Some of the oldest farm dwellings in their collection of historic buildings from Wales contained humans and farm animals for entirely practical reasons: in the cold winters when the cattle were kept indoors the heat from their bodies provided warmth for the humans. Similarly, there were instances of pig-hen houses: the heat from the pigs kept the hens above them warm in winter, extending their laying period considerably. We dont know much about these cohabitations because they went unrecorded and by modern times the differentiating impulse worked to keep species apart, in their rightful place. In 1950s modern suburban Australia and the USA pets were common enough, but by contemporary standards even they were differentiated spatially, behaviourally and ethically to a considerable degree. 1950s dogs lived outside in the kennel or dog house, a term that became synonymous with socially cast out or suspended (temporarily) from affection and in misery (Council for Science and Society 1988). Cats were put outside at night in the dark and cold. Moreover, as Franklin (1999) has shown, in the 1950s dogs and cats were differentiated by different naming strategies. Whereas from the 1970s it can be shown that dogs began to be given human names, and that from the 1990s they began to be given the same names currently being given to human babies, in the 1950s dogs (and cats) tended to be given specifically dog or cat names (Fido, Rover, Tibby, Kitty and the like). The materialist politics of the early to mid-twentieth century also meant that many household did not have pets, though we know poverty and low pay encouraged some to keep backyard or allotment animals for meat (Williamson 1982). The so-called post-materialist Western societies of the 1970 onwards lived in a world of plentiful food and keeping pets was no great financial obstacle, though why they should be so compelling under conditions of dual-income families, fast-time, careerism and the information age begs the question. That pet keeping, particularly of cats and dogs, has increased dramatically since the 1970s is not in question (Franklin 1999). Accounting for this, and in particular accounting for the changing nature of relations between them and their humans is more demanding. It was certainly after the 1970s that pets changed to companion animals. In a country such as Australia more than two-thirds of households today keep an animal on their property and given that Australia is almost completely settled in its A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing 139 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 six capital cities (and always has been) we can discount any major influence from its farming, rural/colonial past. Where property is extremely expensive and a greater proportion of households live in high-rise apartments, such as Sydney, the density of companion animal keeping is lower (see Table 1). This may be accounted for, almost entirely, by restrictive covenants and greater proportions of non-companion animal orientated ethnic groups. Elsewhere in suburban and more anglicized Australia, up to 86% of households keep an animal of one kind or another Mostly, as Table 2 shows, it is dogs and cats that dominate multi-species households, with dogs the species of preference (47%), cats a poor second (30%) and birds coming in third with unexpectedly high numbers (17%). On the other hand, the range of exotic species in the survey is also remarkable and relatively new. Table 3 offers some purchase on why contemporary households include companion animals. If in the 1950s pets were predominantly bought for children, for their function and for amusement (Council for Science and Society 1988, Franklin 1999) this is certainly not the case today. Dogs, cats, rabbits, fish and birds were all kept to amuse children, to a degree, but this was not why most of the animals in our survey were recruited to human households. Nor was it to provide company for children, though this was more significant. The single biggest reason given was as company for our 2000 adult Australian respondents. In the case of cats and dogs this was given for 80% of the number of animals recorded in our survey, but it was also the case in 50% of all birds kept and 35% of rabbits. Table 1. Do you keep animals on your property? Total Sydney Melbourne Brisbane Perth Adelaide ACT Hobart Respondents (n) 2000 430 361 153 140 122 36 21 Yes (%) 68 55 59 69 75 77 81 86 No (%) 32 45 41 31 25 23 19 14 Table 2. What types of animals do you currently keep on your property? Total Sydney Melbourne Brisbane Perth Adelaide ACT Hobart Respondents (n) 2000 430 361 153 140 122 36 21 Birds (%) 17 15 11 16 19 14 14 24 Cats (%) 30 21 28 30 29 40 31 52 Dogs (%) 47 36 39 46 49 53 44 71 Fish (%) 13 11 8 15 12 19 14 14 Guinea pigs or hamsters (%) 2 1 2 2 2 3 3 Horses (%) 4 1 1 4 1 3 Rabbits (%) 2 4 1 4 4 6 None of these (%) 1 1 0 1 3 Not asked* (%) 32 45 41 31 25 23 19 14 *The survey was administered to a nationally representative sample of 2000 Australians. However, detailed questions on animal ownership was only asked of those who kept animals on their property, hence there is a value in this and other tables for those Not asked. ACT5Australian Capital Territory. 140 A. Franklin D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 Elsewhere I have tried to account for this astonishing phenomenon in terms of critical social changes in contemporary society (Franklin 1999, Franklin & White 2001). Western societies have become individualized societies where extensive loneliness in society and high degrees of ontological insecurity are widely reported (Giddens 1984, Bauman 2000, Furedi 2005) 6 . As Bauman has repeatedly claimed, marriage, friendship, partnership, community ties and even love itself has become insecure, ephemeral and fugitive: in his own poetic words they have become until further notice (Bauman 2000, 2003). In a previous book Animals and Modern Cultures 7 I argued that those in Western Anglophone societies who have suffered family trauma and who find themselves alone and possibly socially and physically insecure or isolated often acquire companion animals, particularly dogs and cats. Divorce, separation, single parenthood, economic depression, the migration of young people from country areas, insecure local labour markets, all serve to increase the numbers of people living alone or households stranded away from former kin. The numbers of lone or small household units has increased dramatically over the past 30 years, to the point where the building industry now builds for a different, lonelier demography. According to US Census Bureau projections, by 2010, 31 million Americans will be living alone, a 40% increase from 1980 8 . According to the BBC, the independent Family Policy Studies Centre (FPSC) findings show that more than 6.5 million people in Britain about 28% of households now live on their own, three times as many as 40 years ago 9 . In Australia things are no different. Lindsay Tanner, MP for Melbourne, describes it as a crisis of loneliness 10 , citing significance proportions of elderly and young people as at risk. The latest survey on loneliness among Australians aged 2544 years, the group that has experienced the highest increase in solitary living, found that 16% of both men and women agreed with the statement I often feel lonely (Flood 2005:11). However 33% of men and 23% of women living alone reported feeling lonely often. Clearly, people believe that their loneliness will be alleviated by animal companionship (and as I will show, companionship is the single biggest reason given for acquiring a dog, a cat or a bird) and indeed, the most sophisticated research using the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale found that participants living entirely alone were more lonely than those living with pets 11 . Table 3. Thinking about all domestic animals you have at the moment, and excluding animals you keep for work, would you say you keep your pets for: Birds Cats Dogs Fish Rabbits Respondents (n) 303 592 945 204 40 Amusement and entertainment of adults (%) 38 24 26 45 20 Amusement and entertainment of children (%) 30 24 22 45 58 Competitive showing (%) 2 1 2 0 Other competition or sport (%) 1 0 2 0 Work (%) 1 1 6 0 3 Security and protection (%) 1 2 48 Company for yourself (%) 50 79 82 12 35 Company for children (%) 37 44 43 16 40 A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing 141 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 Even those who are currently setting up new households put off having children for longer and are far more likely to have no children or only one. Such households frequently buy dogs or cats to fill out their household, to provide a focus for their relationship or to provide surrogate siblings for only children. Our survey data supports the notion that pet keeping responds to transformations in family and lifecycle change. For example, while in married and de facto households the proportion of dog owners who chose dogs for their company was 80%, in divorced or separated households the proportion rose to 88%, in widowed households to 90% and among the retired 91%. Similarly, the divorced and separated are more likely to choose a dog for security and protection than married/de facto households (Franklin 2006). In Animals and Modern Cultures I also argued that a number of indicators show that companion animals had been increasingly brought closer to their human friends in emotional and social terms; indeed, that they were now often reckoned to be part of the family. We therefore asked whether respondents considered any of their animals to be members of their family. This not only indicates the surrogacy of animals for significant human relationships, but it also indicates a breakdown in the perceived difference between humans and non-humans. We also asked about animals as family members because this ascription came up spontaneously and frequently in a series of focus groups conducted in advance of the national survey. This translation is commonly referred to as anthropomorphism, or the attribution of human-like qualities to animals that are merely whimsical fantasies of the human imagination. This may be so, but it is not necessarily so. If people are merely extending to animals, as animals, the notion of belonging, and recognizing close bonds with them as equivalent to those within human families, then this is not a case of anthropomorphism, it is a case of hybridization: hybridization of the family. Unproblematic similarities might include co-residence, enduring ties, emotional inter-dependence, friendship, company and shared activities. Where this happens it is important to realize that it is not a one-way, human-orchestrated attribution, but one built of close feelings and emotions self-evidently expressed also by the animals themselves. We see with birds, especially of the parrot and cockatiel family, emotions such as jealousy and dependence, and embodied practices such as cuddling and kissing. Some of these, of course, are parrot expressions, translations of courtship and pair bonding behaviours that can be observed between parrots, but the critical point is that some of them are not. Some of them are specific to the bonds between humans and animals; unique to them. A good example of this is the vocal expressions between cats and humans. Cats are largely mute in their dealing with each other in the wild, but they seem to have learned of the significance of vocalization between humans and the fact that humans vocalize to them. The meow is the most significant (though it has many variations): cats do not meow to each other. And it is also true that the breeds that have been domesticated the longest are also the most vocal in their dealings with their human companions. According to Kersti Seksel, we really should understand cats better as theyve gone to the trouble of developing special forms of communication just to talk to humans, using body language and vocalization which theyd never use with other cats 12 . As Table 4 shows, the overwhelming majority of Australians did ascribe family membership to their pets. This may not be so new, but what seems to be new is the 142 A. Franklin D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 willingness to express it. One vet in an affluent suburb in Sydney had this to say about companion animals as substitute children or just children: Well funny enough people are actually willing to admit that its a substitute child. Theyre not embarrassed to actually say that. A lot of them actually say it is a substitute child. I notice that I dont recall that so much in the past, but in recent years I have noticed that people actually refer to it as their child in many ways, you know. Vet 3, Sydney. On average, 88% thought that the animals they keep were part of their family. Some places were well above this average, such as Perth and Hobart (94%) and Melbourne, rural WA and rural Victoria (91%). Some were well below the average, such as Sydney (84%) ACT (72% and NT (78%) (see Table 5). So in relation to this issue there is no clear-cut urban rural divide. Sydney consistently shows up as less sentimental and emotionally involved with animals, while in Melbourne and Hobart such characteristics are very strong. Similarly, rural Queensland and the Northern Territory are less emotionally and sentimentally attached than rural areas in Tasmania and rural Victoria. This suggests that the critical factors are not urbanness or ruralness but other cultural configurations in each place. In addition to place, the degree to which people considered animals to be part of their family varied, once again, with occupation and educational attainment. Although we must be clear that the overwhelming majority of Australians did consider their animals to be part of their family, it is also the case that the less educated and those in blue-collar occupations were far more likely to. Unskilled and skilled blue-collar groups dominated those who considered animals to be part of their family (93% and 92%, respectively) while the white-collar professional and managerial groups scored lowest (84% and 86%, respectively). Educational attainment data shows that some groups in society are much less likely to consider animals to be part of their family. Those with higher degrees are well below the average in these terms and that likelihood of seeing animals as family members varies gradually with educational level (Franklin 2006). Table 4. Do you think of any animals you keep as members of your family (capital cities)? Total Sydney Melbourne Brisbane Perth Adelaide ACT Hobart All respondents (n) 2000 430 361 153 140 122 36 21 Asked respondents (n) 1350 237 214 105 105 94 29 18 Yes (% asked respondents) 88 84 92 90 94 88 72 94 No (% asked respondents) 12 16 8 10 6 12 28 6 Dont know (n) 3 3 % respondents not asked 33 45 41 31 25 23 19 14 ACT5Australian Capital Territory. A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing 143 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 Clearly there is something important about formal education that disturbs attitudes and practices with animals. It is highly likely that those processed through the tertiary and higher educational mills are most exposed to enlightenment biopolitics and formal scientific positions on the essential (and proper) difference between humanity and animality. Such views are mirrored in many contemporary debates: in social and cultural studies of the environment and political movements, in the sciences of ecology and land management and in Australian eco-nationalistic history. One suspects that the less educated are less influenced by such taxonomies and ontologies and tend to take animals as they find them. To see whether ascribing family status to animals meant anything more than just sentimental labels, we asked whether companion animals had access to those parts of the house historically reserved for humans (Table 6). Anecdotal evidence suggests that in the 1950s and before, animals were largely kept out of the home, sleeping in kennels and on verandas. Today this is very much not the case. We scaled questions according to where animals were allowed in the home, from the backyard at one extreme to the bedroom and on furniture, including beds, at the other. Over half of respondents claimed their companion animals were allowed in their bedroom and 35% allowed animals in their childrens bedroom. Forty-eight percent of households allowed animals on their furniture. Seventy-six percent allowed their animals into the family room or lounge, 62% allowed their animals in the room where they eat and 66% allowed animals into the kitchen. In other words companion animals mostly have the run of the house. We expected to find a significant difference between rural and urban Australia over this question, but although there was a difference, the difference was not that marked: 57% of urban Australian respondents allowed animals into their bedroom for example, as against 47% of rural Australians. If our general hypothesis about changing family structure, social vulnerability and loneliness and companion animals was true we would expect the more sociably vulnerable groups to be more liberal in their sharing of household space. Certainly there is some evidence for this. For example, 58% of those between 70 and 75 years of age and 65% of those over 76 years of age allowed their companion into their bedroom. Similarly, while only 49% of married and de facto households allowed their companion animals into the bedroom, 59% of the divorced and separated did so. Other tolerant groups included Table 5. Do you think of any animals you keep as members of your family (rural areas)? Total NT NSW rural VIC rural QLD rural WA rural SA rural TAS rural All respondents (n) 2000 17 250 151 192 51 45 31 Asked respondents (n) 1350 9 175 122 144 43 34 21 Yes (% asked respondents) 88 78 84 91 90 91 82 81 No (% asked respondents) 12 22 15 9 10 9 15 19 Dont know (% asked respondents) 0 0 2 Not asked (%) 33 47 30 19 25 16 24 32 NT5Northern Territories; SA5South Australia; WA5Western Australia; QLD5Queensland; VIC5Victoria; NSW5New South Wales; TAS5Tasmania. 144 A. Franklin D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 two other groups vulnerable to loneliness: those under 20 years old (58%) and singles (60%). The symbolism of household space needs to be emphasized here. Bedrooms are largely highly private spaces, the inner sanctum of privatized societies. Partners, close friends and siblings and other close family members form the restricted group of intimates using bedrooms together. So, in this sense, when people in our survey stated that an animal was both a member of the family and allowed into their bedroom, it was a refined answer indicating that they were not just a member of the family but also a very close intimate member. Sitting rooms in modern homes are places of social gathering and communal activities. Chairs are symbolic of both belonging and status in Western cultures and this is illustrated both by norms to give up seats to elders and the associating of high status with seats (aristocratic estates), chairs (synonyms of high ranking authority) and of course thrones. To be seated together means to be equally ranked. Therefore in the past when dogs were kept outside in a separate house, or when they were allowed inside but not on furniture their separate, inferior status was being marked. To discover that half of those interviewed allowed their animals on furniture is to uncover a major shift in their status and position relative to humans and human society. To check whether this is so we framed a few statements in terms of moral and political equivalence. We asked if people agreed that Keeping animals as pets is unnatural and demeaning to both the humans and the animal; and People who mistreat their animals should be punished in the same way as people who mistreat human beings; and Animals should have the same moral rights as human beings. Table 7 shows that these triangulating questions confirm a substantial move towards moral and political equivalence. Fifty five percent agreed with the moral equivalence statement. Perhaps more significant is the fact that when this exact question was asked previously in a 1993 survey, only 42% of Australians agreed 13 . I suggest that this change is significant. Living Together in New Ways? We have not had the opportunity to investigate in any depth whether these changes in human-animal households and co-habitations have resulted in new ways of accommodating and relating to each other, but we suspect that this is happening. What is obviously so new about these relatings (to use term Haraway (2003) prefers) is their new intensity (especially the intensity of time spent together), their focus on essentially domestic/private/familial spaces, their emotional content and, at least from the human side, the degree to which relatings with animals replace those with other humans but it may well be that it is true for many animals too. The stories I am beginning to hear about these relatings is that they have an open-ended, experimental becoming rather than a fixed, behaviourally given and limited nature. This is why Haraway and I prefer the fluid character of relatings (or relationships) to the more definable and inflexible relationship. But it is also because relatings acknowledge, and maintain methodologically, a more symmetrical pattern of agency between humans and companion species. As Haraway says: A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing 145 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 Table 6. Are any of the domestic animals you keep, allowed into the following parts of your property? Back yard Veranda/ balcony Laundry Kitchen The room where you eat The family or lounge room Your bedroom Childrens bedroom On the furniture Total 1350 1350 1350 1350 1350 1350 1350 1350 1350 Yes (%) 95 79 72 66 62 76 52 35 48 No (%) 5 19 28 34 38 24 47 63 52 Dont know (%) 1 2 0 0 0 0 3 0 Table 7. Australian attitudes to animal issues It is quite acceptable to hunt feral animals, such as pigs and wild horses, that degrade the environment It is wrong to hunt native Australian animals Keeping animals as pets is unnatural and demeaning to both the humans and the animal People who mistreat their animals should be punished in the same way as people who mistreat human beings Animals should have the same moral rights as human beings It is right to use animals for medical testing if it might save human lives All respondents (n) 2000 2000 2000 2000 2000 2000 Strongly agree (%) 20 35 2 45 15 8 Agree (%) 48 36 8 39 40 47 Disagree (%) 21 19 48 12 34 26 Strongly disagree (%) 7 6 38 3 6 12 Have no opinion/dont know (%) 4 4 4 1 20 6 1 4 6 A . F r a n k l i n D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 There cannot be just one companion species; there has to be at least two to make one. It is in the syntax; it is in the flesh. Dogs are about the inescapable, contradictory story of relationships co-constitutive relationships in which none of the partners pre-exist the relating, and the relating is never done once and for all. Historical specificity and contingent mutability all the way down, into nature and culture, into naturecultures. (Haraway 2003:12) More specifically for Haraway, as for Law (1994, 1999), we need to search for a sociology of verbs not nouns: Reality is an active verb, and the nouns all seem to be gerunds with more appendages than an octopus. Through their reaching into each other, through their prehensions or graspings, beings constitute each other and themselves. Beings do not pre-exist their relatings. Prehensions have consequences. (Haraway 2003:6) While Haraways essay is specifically about domestic dogs the subtitle is Dogs, People and Significant Otherness a species with whom humanity has had a long and mutually constitutive series of relatings, the significance of prehensions, graspings and co-constitutions apply equally to other species from whenever point they enter into companionate relatings. The essential point is that they are open-ended, experimental and likely to fold in other objects and beings into their cultural field. In the case of Michaels Hudogledog, for example, it is argued that it is the co(a)gent or hybrid of the dog, the dog lead and the human dog walker that constitute the nature of interesting and important community relationships in English parks and recreational spaces. This story and co(agency) is but one in the evolving nature of companion animals, humans and community in contemporary society. Michael shows that it is the nature of the co(a)gency rather than objects acting separately that configures the social and cultural interactions in the park. Co(a)gents come into being as heterogeneous admixtures.specific technologies, bits of bodies, aspects of nature, parts of culture and traditions of discourse (Michael 2000:2). Similarly, my argument here is that co(a)gency is operating in the constitution of contemporary homes, household and families. Very advisedly, I avoid being too specific about the exact nature of this, since it will be something that needs to be investigated rather than reported fully here. However, it is possible to point to some examples and tell the story of one in some detail (although this means switching from Australia to the USA and from dogs to rabbits). The nature of housing choices, for example, is influenced by the consideration of the housing of companionate animals who are significant others in the lives of householders. Vets in Sydney, Perth and Hobart told me that their quality of life and their environment are aspects that contemporary owners emphasize more and more. Dog owners like to choose places with good walks for their dogs; areas where the other dogs are not too rough and off the lead; areas where walks are not too crowded; where there is a good choice and variability of walks (people who are more attentive of their dogs realize that they get bored of the same route, just as they do). A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing 147 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 Such consideration may make a considerable impact on the nature of local housing markets in a way that childrens schools once did (more significantly). Equally, the nature of house type enters into the equation with gardens, for example, becoming significant considerations among those who are not necessarily keen gardeners. Garden design for companion animals has become important since greater care has been exercised over their physical and mental health. A dog consigned to the back yard in Australia was once left to its own devices, but now, under more closely caring eyes, behavioural problems and mental suffering have become cause for concern. For example, vets and contemporary dog health manuals advise giving dogs a view of life in their neighbourhood; vantage points where they can look out. This breaks up the solid walls and fence lines of homes and leads to less neurotic digging and unending barking. Similarly, gardens that were once designed for vegetables, childrens play, lawns and roses are now being more actively considered from both human and animal perspectives. Some, for example, allow for unhindered running, for playing fetch and ball games. Others include a water pool for animals, a water source, elevation, climbing, hiding, digging and sleeping. It is a mistake to think that this is just a question of human choice and agency because typically it is knowledge and experience of companion species agency that inform or inscribes itself on human decisions. The interiors of homes are also changing. At one time the choice of coming into and out of the house was determined by humans, but the spread of cat and dog doors has changed that and extended more choice to the animals. The amount of animal- specific furniture being added to the home is on the increase. Cats like to be in elevated, draught-free, warm spots and owners have experimented with nests, lookouts, climbing posts, ramps and boxes. In the UK owners can buy nests that fix to central heating radiators. Across the Western world new companies have emerged to provide luxurious, species-specific furniture that recognizes the elevated status of companion animals in the family and household. Widely considered as pampering and kitsch, this nonetheless expresses something important: animals are just as good as people as objects for the expression of love and attachment and they are equally good at asserting their agency in human households. All of this is not merely the artefact of human cultural displacement, the welling up of human emotion and ingenuity in the absence of proper (human) objects of the imagination. Much of what changes is done in relation to/in combination with what the animals do, prefer, enact. Much of it is or begins with non-human agency. For example, the opening up of the full range of human domestic spaces to species that had hitherto little experience of them began an emergent history of experimentation and discovery. One woman in Brisbane discovered that her companion cane toads that lived in specially constructed homes in her garden liked the sound of her computer keyboard as she typed her work in the kitchen. They would gradually all come inside to listen. Then the cane toads discovered that the womans bare feet were warm and pleasant to sit on and before long the local cane toad family fought each other for prime corpo(real) estate (Lewis 1989:58). Evidently they became soothed and hypnotized by their kinesthesic experience. In the case of my own dog Coco, who came to us shortly after this project began (and whose biography with us was all the more acutely observed as a result), her access to the entire house was a problem. We had a large, open plan house with a large kitchen area that she could enter from 148 A. Franklin D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 one of two directions. She is a large, rangy boisterous dog and her normal behaviour in the kitchen with children learning to cook, etc, was clearly dangerous. What did we need to do? Alter the kitchen design with door or barriers? Actually no, what we discovered is that dogs are hypersensitive to and respectful of the micro-politics of space. After saying no to her coming into the kitchen, for what seemed like hardly any time at all, she never come in and would never even try while we were there. We did not know this before or read it somewhere; we discovered it about her, just as she discovered that we didnt want her in the kitchen. Over time we realized that Coco always like to locate herself centrally or equidistantly in relation to humans and cats in the house and in a watchful position for them. At night when humans sat around a television Coco sensed the significance of the gathering and wanted to be as close as possible. She was fidgety and restless without a place on a chair around the television but perfectly relaxed and mellow once installed in her television chair place. Relatings between humans and companion species also have very significant impacts on who we are and how we are, how partners come to be who we are in flesh and sign (Haraway 2003:25, my emphasis). In fleshy terms nothing could be more significant than our health and physical (and mental) wellbeing, but these are positively reconfigured, mainly in positive ways by our relatings with companion species. In 1992 Anderson, Reid & Jennings found that in a survey of those attending a cardiovascular screening service (n55741) in Melbourne, pet owners reported significantly fewer visits to doctors and significantly less consumption of specified medications (for high blood pressure, high cholesterol, sleeping difficulties or heart problems). Pet owners had significantly lower systolic blood pressure and plasma triglycerides than non-owners, but the two groups did not differ in body mass index, socio-economic indicators, or smoking habits (Jennings et al. 1998:163). Moreover, pet owners in the study ate more meat and take-out food. Since then numerous international follow-up studies have largely confirmed these findings (Headey 1998, Freidmann, Thomas & Eddy 2000). The significance of all this has not passed unnoticed by the researchers concerned. In 1998 Jennings et al. estimated the health benefits of companion animals based on their 1992 survey. Using 199394 health costs in Australia, the total savings were estimated at $144,892 million, comprising of savings from GP visits of $26,244 million; savings on pharmaceuticals of $18,856 million and savings on hospitaliza- tion of $144,892 million. According to a later study based on nationally representative data, the estimate figure was considerably higher. Headey and associates replicated Andersons survey and found similar results: pet owners made significantly fewer visits to doctors and used significantly less medicine. Using 1994 95 Medicare expenditure and assuming that all recurrent health expenditure can be divided up proportionately to the number of doctor visits people make, Headey (1998) calculated the saving to be $988 million, representing 2.7% of the nations health expenditure. However, as we enter a new phase of more intensive and detailed study of this phenomenon, the benefits may be more significant as a result of being able to direct them more effectively in the population through training and supervision. For example, Jennings et al. strongly suggest that the critical benefit may not be from ownership per se but from specific types of relationship. They found, for example, that non-partnered people who reported feeling close to their A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing 149 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 dogs made significantly fewer doctor visits and took less medication than non- partnered people who were not close to their dogs (Jennings et al. 1998:168). This suggests that we need to understand the relationship itself and its variation, how close relationships develop and why they deliver health benefits. With greater confidence in their administration and armed with Headeys finding that people over the age of 55 years have the most to gain, Australia might elect enact policies that promote their take-up. While cardiovascular disease is a prominent problem for the nation, the benefit of understanding the relationship we have with companion species is considerably wider. It extends to general wellbeing (Garrity & Stallones 1998), treatment of depression, loneliness and anxiety (Wilson 1998:61), and Alzheimers disease (Batson, McCabe, Baun & Wilson 1998) to say nothing about therapeutic uses in prisons, hospitals and homes for elderly people. The health benefits of the humancompanion animal relation is not one way. While there may be reciprocated life-prolonging influences for animals by simply being with humans, there are now very tangible medical benefits through the willingness of humans to pay for veterinary services. During interviews with vets in Australia, in Perth, Sydney and Hobart, it was obvious that owners are willing to pay very large sums of money for their animals health. This is bottom-line proof of the meaningfulness of owners claims that their animals are their family. As we will see owners now spend small fortunes on the health needs of cats and dogs, but it is not restricted to these species. One of the most extraordinary stories was told by a Perth vet. It concerned a pet rabbit: I have a reputation to be one of the best vets for rabbits in Australia and I had this lady with a sick rabbit in Brisbane and she paid me $100,000 to come and treat it. A lot of people will feed the rabbit and dont think much about the rabbit, but for this person the importance of the rabbit was at least worth $100,000. But thats the extreme, but Ive had people telling me that they would give everything they have to treat their animal and they are usually able to find $5000 to $10,000. Another vet in Sydney emphasized that people tend to follow very similar treatment options for animals and not merely the cheapest. Nor are they necessarily in non-human hospitals: I have a dog with a tumour of its lower jaw that, umm, we could its a cancer and the real option for this guy is to have his jaw removed surgically. Now the owner said, I dont want to do that. What other options do I have? The other option is radiation therapy and so weve negotiated to deal with a human radiotherapist. I am transporting this dog every afternoon up to the Human Radiotherapy Unit at Wahroonga Hospital, which takes me about three- quarters of an hour drive to an hour drive. We anaesthetize the dog at that site. He is treated in the human hospital and then I wake him up from the anaesthetic and I transport him back again and were doing it every day for ten days. Now that is going to cost the owner $6500 by the time he has finished that treatment. Now we could have removed his jaw for probably $2000 to 150 A. Franklin D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 $3500 going through all of that procedure and the aftercare and what have you. But the owner didnt want to do that. He wanted to maintain the integrity of the dogs jaw, its appearance and its function and so to do that were spending $6500 in an attempt to cure his cancer. Now we know he mightnt get a cure. He has only got probably a 75% chance of getting a cure, but the owner is prepared to do it. Umm is that a good enough example? A further demonstration of the extension of humanity to companion species is expressed in owners concern about pain. It may surprise some that pain relief was hardly administered to companion species in the past. This is no longer true, but again the change is quite recent, as this Sydney vet acknowledged: they just have higher expectations, yeah. So, for example, they are very concerned about pain relief and we offer a lot of pain relief these days, which certainly going back some years was not so broadly done. You might give pain relief initially, but we give on going pain relief and people are often very concerned about what pain their animal might be in. I think thats something different. But prehensions and graspings such as this go beyond the practical realm of everyday life and embodiment, rich though that is. The intensity of life with contemporary companion animals is such that this serial dance of agency alters forever whatever conceptions of animals or particular species one might begin with and in particular demonstrates the instability of the notion of species. Species are understood as fully and finally formed entities with a coherent and limited body of behaviour; with what we might call a nature, to use everyday language, that defines but also circumscribes them. However, life in the new environment of the contemporary home destabilizes such narrow conceptions and both species of the companionate relation live in this unstable but mutually re-constituting ontology. The case of the House Rabbit Society in the USA, my final example is all that it takes to demonstrate the infinity of possible worlds of relatings, affordances, prehensions, naturecultures and indeed technonatures that constitute the prospect for humans and animals. The House Rabbit Society What follows is based mainly on the work of Julie A. Smith (2003) and to a lesser extent on other publications by other House Rabbit Society (HRS) members. When I first heard Smith talk about the HRS at a conference in London in 2001 I began to realize that this was probably not an eccentric oddity but the beginning of something that was becoming has already become more widespread. One immediately thinks of natural horse riding for example as a cognate development. Whether eccentric or zeitgeist, it is clearly the archetype of developments and becomings suggested in this paper. The HRS is an organization founded in 1988 in California by Marinell Harriman and others that encourages a new way of living with rabbits: not imprisoned in hutches, alone and outside but relatively free to live with humans and other rabbits A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing 151 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 inside human dwellings (though as it progressed the latter can only be considered humanrabbithomes). Holes would be a good term for them, not only because this is one way of describing a natural rabbit home or burrow, but because this is how many conventional middle American people would uncharitably describe the homes of the HRS after they had become rabbitized.as you will see and hear. Smiths wonderful paper is part biography and part analysis. One of the main driving anxieties for Smith is how to live ethically with animals such as domestic rabbits and she begins her paper responding to Tuans (1984) critique of pet keeping and the sinister side of humanity it expresses (the pleasure of dominance, as he put it). Throughout the paper Smith tries to rescue an ethical position for keeping domestic rabbits in the way the HRS does. They rescue rabbits from a variety of perils, recruiting most of them from animal shelters. Thus, the first ethical position is that she is rescuing them from danger, then looking after them, then, by applying the rules of the HRS adopting them out to those agreeing to abide by the HRS rules. But next she worries about the ethics of their life with humans: surely humans are exercising an unacceptable dominance even those as caring as the HRS. For example, HRS rabbits are neutered. Smith agonizes over this, but eventually understands it as a medical intervention in favour of the rabbits health (a life of breeding and fighting is not without substantial risk) and in any case, as she argues, a life not spent breeding can be spent on other things, other activities and explorations that enable the rabbit to be just as natural or rabbit-like in their approach to them. The argumentation has a certain resemblance to the human liberation from a life of childbirth and family. Those other things and activities centre on the life with and home shared with humans. An environment inside the home can be contrived in such a way that the rabbit not only feels comfortable and safe, but can arrange things themselves to a considerable degree. Most potently however Smith describes their mutual relatings as governed, at least as far as she governed, by a performance ethics: I propose a performance ethics, which will both celebrate the human desire to dismantle the boundary between humans and companion animals and acknowledge its difficulty (Smith 2003:183). HRS Approach to Living With Rabbits It is worth quoting Smith at length to gauge the extent to which the HRS members homes and family life was disturbed by the new performance ethics: The HRS did not simply manage difficult issues of control discursively. In fact, its members surrendered enormous control over their homes. Many HRS members rabbit-proofed their houses, a playful word that euphemized extensive modifications. In my own house, rabbit-proofing meant that most of the furniture was made of metal, electrical cords were fastened behind furniture or covered in hard plastic or metal tubing, and protective wood strips were tacked on to wood baseboards and wood trim around closets and windows. In addition, linoleum replaced carpet or the carpet was abandoned to shredding and fencing enclosed bookcases. So-called litterbox training primarily meant capitalizing on the rabbit habit of urinating consistently in one or two 152 A. Franklin D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 places. We simply put litterboxes where the rabbits decided to eliminate. Many of us found it easier to change ourselves than the premises. At present, the rabbit who lives in the bedroom is excavating my mattress. She bounces around inside the dust cover and chews the wooden frame around the metal springs. . Indeed, I have heard HRS members laugh about taking turns with their human partners sleeping on the wet spot in the bed; putting fencing around their beds at night to keep rabbits from urinating on their pillow or barbering their eyebrows; and catering to rabbits who nip ankles, box hands, or trip-up human bodies when caretakers are too slow with the treats. Frankly, I love this way of living, this version of becoming animal. It was the genius of HRS founder Harriman to naturalize this life, so that those of us who came after felt social permission to live as we had always wanted to. (Smith 2003:187188.) I can see the attraction. However, as with the examples of dogs and cats above, the Smith household began to be changed not by their own performance ethics but by rabbit agency: it both asserted itself and enlightened the humans in their becomingrabbit. Here is Smith again: Over time, we came to understand the principles of rabbit space and changed our abodes even more. After many years of living with rabbits, I noticed that they liked free corridors along perimeters. (Smith 2003:189.) In addition rabbits like to pile things up in the middle of the room creating a mess that was at the same time a rabbit ordered mess. In time Smith became attuned to rabbit aesthetics and space management and began to enjoy the way rabbits could make themselves at home in a dwelling shared with humans. Equally, in addition to these housing issues, there emerged new interactions new intimacies and prehensions 14 : In other major, minor, practical, and discursive ways did HRS members express their vision of shared space. Interaction with rabbits was presented as best happening on the floor. While the human lay quietly, the rabbit would investigate, groom, climb and sit on the human, and allow him/herself to be petted. In this way, rabbits were given freedom to initiate interaction, a key component of relational partnerships (Harker, Collis & McNicholas 2000:191, Smith 2003:189) What is so clear from the writings of the HRS is the experimental nature of their domestic relations with rabbits. They are experimenting with how it might be possible to live with rabbits differently and in so doing discovering new forms of possible relationships between rabbits and humans. Conclusion In this paper I have demonstrated, in the light of findings from an Australian national survey, the extent to which human housing, household and family life has become entangled in new ways with companion species. I have also located these A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing 153 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 changes in the individualized world of liquid modernity and liquid love and have argued that new intensities of co-presence have offered opportunities for new ways of relating to companion species and new ways of thinking about relating to them. In particular, new relatings unearth the instability and unreliability of species being and reveal companion animals to be exercising agency, creativity and companionability in reference to their humans. I offer examples of this and an archetype: the Society of House Rabbits. Formerly exclusively human homes, households and family formations are now undergoing change as they become sites for human-animal cohabitations. This affects housing choice and design, furnishing and the internal configuration of space. Glibly, one might understand this transformative process as a sad substitution of animal objects for proper human sociability and bonds; sad for both the humans reduced to such straits and for animals so anthropomorphized. However I have argued that these circumstances are not such that animals cannot express agency or that humans cannot relate to them as significant others and that their co(a)gency results in new effects. Humans and animals appear to make very good companions to each other and of course many have been tempered by thousands of years of co- evolution. But many (cane toads, rabbits) have not but they did not take long to realize the benefits of cohabitation. Cohabitation is expressed both semiotically and sensually, in communication and in flesh. Both, probably, account for the remarkable finding that living with a companion species can improve human health and well being. Notes 1. The study, an Australian Research Council funded project Sentiments and Risks: The Changing Nature of Human-Animal Relations took place between 2000 and 2004. It combined a nationally representative survey of 2000 respondents with a series of case studies focussed around veterinary practice and relationships with wildlife. The survey was conducted over the telephone with Australians over the age of 16 years and we randomized the choice of respondent in each household by asking to speak with the person whose birthday was next. This guaranteed that all ages and genders are represented. We also created statistically representative interview targets for all capital cities and state rural areas. The main survey was administered by NCS Pearson and the survey instrument was comprised of 13 key questions of which 5 established key data on the type of animals respondents shared their lives with, 7 were Likert-type questions which investigated values and practices with respect to animals generally and one question was comprised of a battery of sub- questions obtaining key social , economic and demographic data. The overall response rate was 35% (calculated as a proportion of answered calls). 2. See Michael (2000). 3. This derives from Deleuze & Guataris work on becoming animal in One Thousand Plateaus. This involves a radical decentring of the subject through imagining and practicing what it might mean to be another species. 4. See Ingold 1995; 2000. 5. Of course there are other species that we do not purposefully live with, that also matter: woodworms, mice, rats, mites, etc. 6. According to Abercrombie, Hill & Turner (The Penguin dictionary of sociology, 2005) ontological security refers to the security, order and regularity that people feel in their lives, which are likely to be most clearly experienced in a stable sense of personal identity over time. Clearly, divorce, spatial mobility, labour market change and cultural change produce a churning of ontological security or ontological insecurity. 7. Adrian Franklin (1999) Animals and modern cultures. London: Sage. 154 A. Franklin D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 8. Josh Schonwold. NIA will fund a study about loneliness, its physical risks. University of Chicago chronicle 21(5), pp. 59. 9. BBC News (2005) Britain singled out as lonely nation, 27 March, 2000. London, UK. 10. Lindsay Tanner. Address to The Sydney Institute, May 4 1999. 11. Garrity, T. F. & Stallones, L. (1998) Effects of pet contact on human well-being: review of recent research, in: C. Wilson & D. Turner (Eds), Companion animals in human health (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage). 12. Royal New Zealand Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals (2003). The truth about cats and dogs. Press release, 14 May 2003. 13. Zentralarchiv fu r Empirische Sozialforschung (1995) Machine readable codebook ZA Stut 2450: ISSP 1993 environment (Ko ln: Zentralarchiv fu r Empirische Sozialforschung). 14. By this I mean reaching out in a sensual and cognitive way to inscribe rabbits more closely in human culture and practice. References Anderson, W., Reid, P. & Jennings, G. L. (1992) Pet ownership and risk factors for cardiovascular disease. Medical journal of Australia, 157, pp. 298301. Arluke, A. & Sanders, C. (1996) Regarding animals (Philadelphia: Temple University Press). Batson, K., McCabe, B., Baun, M. & Wilson, C. (1998) The effect of a therapy dog on socialization and physiological indicators of stress in persons diagnosed with Alzheimers disease, in: C. Wilson & D. Turner (Eds), Companion animals in human health (Thousand Oaks: Sage). Bauman, Z. (2000) Liquid modernity (Cambridge: Polity). Council for Science and Society (1988) Companion animals in society (Oxford: Oxford University Press). Deleuze, G. & Guattari, F. (1999) A thousand plateaus (London: Athlone). Flood, M. (2005) Mapping loneliness in Australia (Canberra: The Australia Institute). Franklin, A. S. (1999) Animals and modern cultures (London: Sage). Franklin, A. S. (2006) Animal nation: how animals made Australia (Sydney: University of NSW Press). Franklin, A. S. & White, R. (2001) Animals and modernity: changing human-animal relations, 194998. Journal of sociology, 37(3), pp. 219238. Freidmann, E., Thomas, S. A. & Eddy, T. J. (2000) Companion animals and human health: physical and cardiovascular influences, in: A. Podberscek, E. Paul & J. Serpell (Eds), Companion animals and us exploring the relationships between people and pets (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press). Furedi (2005) The politics of fear (London: Continuum). Garrity, T. F. & Stallones, L. (1998) Effects of pet contact on human well-being: review of recent research, in: C. Wilson & D. Turner (Eds), Companion animals in human health (Thousand Oaks: Sage). Giddens, A. (1984) The constitution of society (Cambridge: Polity Press). Haraway, D. (2003) The companion species manifesto (Chicago: Prickly Paradigm Press). Harker, R. M., Collis, G. M. & McNicholas, J. (2000) The influence of current relationships upon pet animal acquisition, in: A. L. Podberscek, E. S. Paul & J. Serpell (Eds), Companion animals and us: exploring the relationships between people and pets, pp. 189208 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press). Headey, B. (1998) Health benefits and health cost savings due to pets: preliminary estimates from an Australian national survey, Social indicators research, 47, pp. 233243. Jennings, G. L. R., Reid, C. M., Christy, I., Jennings, J. & Anderson, W. P. (1998) Animals and cardiovascular health, in: C. C. Wilson & D. C. Turner (Eds), Companion animals in human health (London: Sage). Latour, B. (1993) We have never been modern (New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf). Law, J. (1994) Organizing modernity, London, Sage. Law, J. (1999) After ANT: complexity, naming and topology, in: J. Law & J. Hassard (Eds), Actor network theory and after (Oxford: Blackwell Publishers). Law, J. and Mol, A. (Eds) (2002) Complexities (London: Duke University Press). Lewis, S. (1989) Cane toads: an unnatural history (New York: Doubleday). Michael, M. (2000) Narrating co(a)gents: the case of the Hudogledog, in: Reconnecting culture, technology and nature: from society to heterogeneity (London: Routledge). A Post-Humanist Approach to Housing 155 D o w n l o a d e d
B y :
[ T o m a ,
C l a u d i u ]
A t :
1 3 : 1 2
7
D e c e m b e r
2 0 1 0 Smith, J. A. (2003) Beyond dominance and affection: living with rabbits in post-humanist households, Society and animals, 11(2), pp. 181197. Smutts, B. (2001) Encounters with animal minds, Journal of consciousness, 8(57), pp. 87113. Tuan, Y.-F. (1984) Dominance and affection: the making of pets (New Haven: Yale University Press). Urry, J. (2003) Global complexity (Cambridge: Polity). Williamson, B. (1982) Class, culture and community a biographical study of social change in mining (London: Routledge and Kegan Paul). Wilson, C. C. (1998) A conceptual framework for human-animal interaction research, in: C. Wilson & D. Turner (Eds), Companion animals in human health (Thousand Oaks: Sage). 156 A. Franklin D o w n l o a d e d