AP$20Feminism JOPPPAH
AP$20Feminism JOPPPAH
Erica Etelson
ABSTRACT: As attachment parenting has become more popular, many feminists condemn it as
fundamentally oppressive to mothers. Their critique is based on misinterpretation and misrepresentation of
attachment theory, the neuro-psychological body of research that underlies attachment parenting. In
contravention of the great weight of scientific evidence, many feminists downplay the nurturance needs of
young children as a defensive measure against the neo-conservative backlash against changing womens
roles. Mothers and their children would be better served by a feminist articulation of a real family values
agenda that calls for society to support fathers and mothers engaged in the socially meaningful work of
child-rearing.
KEY WORDS: Attachment parenting, Attachment theory, Feminism, Family values, Child-rearing,
Nurturing
Introduction
It is not my child who has purged my face from history and herstory
Not my child, who in a way beyond all this, but really of a piece with
it, destroys the planet daily, and has begun on the universeWe are
together, my child and I. Mother and child, yes, but sisters really,
against whatever denies us all that we are.
--Alice Walker (1979).
I am a feminist. I am also a mother, and do not wish to think of myself as one who
has betrayed feminist principles by staying home with my son and practicing the
bogeywoman of feminist motherhoodattachment parenting. This essay is my attempt to
rehabilitate attachment parenting from a feminist perspective and restore to new mothers
the freedom to practice it without fear that they are pawns in a neo-traditionalist
conspiracy (Ingman, 2006).
Attachment Parenting
2
The feminist critique of attachment theory must be taken as seriously as
attachment theory itself for it exposes many of the ways in which cultural, social, and
economic pressures undermine the best efforts of mothers and fathers. At the same time,
the condemnation of attachment parenting reveals a degree of inadequacy and
contradiction in feminist discourse around parenthood and early childhood development.
If feminism is to remain true to itself as a movement for human liberation, it cannot
achieve its goals by making the needs of infant girls and boys secondary to the needs and
aspirations of women and men. By critiquing the critique, we can begin to form an
outline of a third-wave feminist movement that champions the needs and rights of all
family members and calls upon the broader society to give families the support they need
to nurture healthy kids.
3
attachment as well as the tragic consequences of touch deprivation and emotional
misattunement or neglect. Their initial conclusions followed on the heels of and helped
usher out the behaviorist view of child-rearing that had prevailed for the previous thirty
or forty yearsthe cold-blooded notion that parents should not spoil infants by kissing
or cuddling them and should rigorously train them from birth to be independent and
morally upright by imposing rigid feeding and toilet-training schedules and employing
draconian measures to prevent them from touching their genitals and other unseemly
habits.
Attachment theorists believe that a child who is forced into premature self-
reliance may appear independent, because most children try to meet their parents
expectations as a matter of sheer survival. They become highly compliant and project a
false invulnerability to mask the unmet needs and abandonment terror that lurk beneath
the surface. When such children get older, their hidden wounds are likely to expose
themselves in various ways, including neuroses, depression, substance abuse, and
violence.
Whereas securely attached children feel connected to others and demonstrate
emotional resilience, cooperativeness and social competence, insecure children tend to be
emotionally stunted and inarticulate, anxious, aggressive, clingy, defiant or overly
compliant, unaffectionate and/or easily frustrated. While only the most extremely
neglected or abused children are at risk of developing severe psychopathologies, children
raised by well-intentioned but emotionally unavailable parents miss out on the kind of
intimate nurturing they need in order to avoid the commonplace but destructive
compulsions, depressions, anxieties and addictions that plague so many adults.
Attachment theorists have shown that the attachment style of an infant remains
remarkably stable over the course of childhood and indeed for the rest of his life. An
adult is very likely to recreate with significant others relationships that enjoy the same
degree of intimacy as his relationship with his parents. If he has children of his own,
chances are they will be only as securely attached to him as he was to his own parents.
For insecure parents, this intergenerational reproduction of attachment styles is a difficult
cross to bear, for it can powerfully undermine the efforts of even the most well-
intentioned of parents to create a secure bond with their children.
More recent attachment research (Seigel & Hartzell, 2003) suggests that the way
in which parents nurture their children after infancy may be a strong co-factor in the
development of secure or insecure attachments but, because parents who practice
attachment parenting are also likely to be emotionally available and non-authoritarian, it
can be difficult to correlate attachment outcomes to nurturing during early or later
childhood. Suffice to say that, as a matter of common sense, establishing and maintaining
a secure attachment during all stages of childhood are key ways in which parents can
enhance their childrens ability to form and enjoy intimate relationships for the rest of
their lives.
Feminists who have been skeptical of the long term stability of early attachments
will now have to contend with the publication of an unprecedented longitudinal study,
now its 30th year, that demonstrates a high degree of predictability. The Minnesota
Parent-Child Study (Stroufe, Egeland, Carlson, & Collins, 2005) has tracked 180 low-
income mothers since before the birth of their first child. They concluded that nothing
affects a childs development more than the care he receives in early childhood and that a
4
childs attachment style at eighteen months usually remains stable throughout childhood,
adolescence and early adulthood. Ongoing assessments of the studys subjects will test
the durability of attachment style into middle age.
Sadly, attachment experiments again and again conclude that one third of U.S.
children are insecurely attached, with even higher rates among high-risk families, the
nightmarish legacy of the behaviorist dream). High-risk factors for insecure attachment
include teenaged parents, poverty, premature births, parental depression, domestic
violence and substance abuse (Karen, 1998).
The attachment parenting movement that sprung up during the late 1970s took
note of the findings of attachment researchers and honed in on the common features of
modern parenting that were inconsistent with attachment principlesisolating infants in
cribs, car seats, and playpens; bottle-feeding and the cry-it-out approach to sleep training.
For each problematic element, attachment parenting advocates promote a more nurturing
alternative that can make infancy a more satisfying experience for parents and babies.
During the early decades of attachment research, mothers were, even more consistently
than today, the primary caregivers; hence, the research failed to address paternal and non-
parental relationships. In the eyes of early attachment theorists, good enough mothers
were exclusively responsible for meeting their babies attachment needs and entirely to
blame for the consequences of maternal deprivation. Nor did attachment proponents
seem to be aware of the socio-cultural obstacles in the way of mothers striving to be
good enough. The expectation was that, if Trobriand Island, Yequana, and Balinese
mothers could do it, so could western mothers, despite the fact that western families live
in isolation from one another without the assistance of extended family, clan or
community.
Feminist Perspective
Just as attachment theory was gaining ground, so too was feminist discontent with
the state of modern motherhood. With the publication of Bettys Friedans The Feminine
Mystique (1963) and Adrienne Richs Of Woman Born (1976), unfulfilled mothers began
to unleash an outpouring of pent-up anger, guilt and despair over the constrained course
of their lives, their entrapment in their biological destiny to bear and raise children, their
exclusion from the non-domestic realm and their ambivalence toward their children and
the entire mothering enterprise. They felt they had been set up to failburdened with
exclusive responsibility for their childrens well-being with little to no support from
fathers and the broader society and then bashed for failing to give their children
everything the experts said they needed or, alternatively, giving them too much and
smothering them with overprotective love. Moreover, to the extent that their mothering
abilities were inhibited from the get-go by the psychological legacy of their own
childhood experiences, they were being emotionally oppressed twice overfirst as little
girls whose needs were not met and again as mothers made to feel guilty for not doing
what they were not capable of doing. Such a psychological double burden was a recipe
for post-partum depression for mothers tormented by feelings of inadequacy, leading
many depressed and overwrought mothers to prescription pills and early afternoon
cocktails. It was an awkward time, to say the least, for attachment proponents to be
suggesting that parents, namely mothers, needed to be doing still more and that the
consequences of failure to foster a secure attachment were potentially dire.
5
Modern Attachment Theory
Modern attachment theory has evolved in three significant ways. First of all, there
is growing recognition of the role of non-maternal caregivers. Secondly, the theory is
now grounded not only in observational experiments but in the exploding new science of
neuropsychiatry. Finally, there is a growing trend to contextualize parenting styles by
examining the cultural, material, and psychological challenges parents, particularly
mothers, face in trying to care for young children. As Alan Sroufe (et al, 2005), head of
the Minnesota Parent-Child Study, commented in his latest book, The tendency to blame
parents is greatly reduced when one sees the challenges parents are facing[B]laming
parents for child problems is dramatically oversimplified and begins an infinite regress
back to the earliest human couple. (p. 18). Sroufe takes every opportunity to point out
the ways in which a parents own upbringing and other factors undermine her ability to
form a secure attachment with her own child and, conversely, how parental support can
dramatically improve the quality of ones parenting.
When attachment researchers belatedly examined the fathers as well as the
mothers attachment style, they observed that boys tend to reproduce their fathers
attachment style and girls their mothers. Most attachment proponents now agree that
children can form secure attachments to a small, stable core of caregivers, none of whom
must necessarily be the mother. When it comes to daycare, the concern is no longer with
the reality of it for most families but with the quality of care, measured first and foremost
by the extent to which the children form close and lasting attachments to the daycare
providers.
Recent advances in the fields of neurobiology and neurochemistry have afforded
psychiatrists an understanding of the physiological architecture of human emotion. What
is stunning about their discoveries is how precisely they corroborate what attachment
theorists have been saying all along, that nurturing early childhood interactions are the
building blocks of healthy psychological development. In the terms of neuro-
psychiatristry, repeated experiences literally carve out the neural pathways and forge the
neural connections that determine our capacity for compassion, self-worth, abstract
thought, impulse control, emotional regulation, and a sense of connection to other people
and the natural world. As we age, the nervous system loses its plasticity, making
personality change harder though not impossible to achieve (Siegel, 1999).
Early affective transactions trigger or suppress the growth of neurotransmitters
and hormones that profoundly affect the individuals emotional regulation, which takes
place in the limbic and orbitofrontal regions of the brain. The limbic system is what
enables us to sense our feelings at the most basic, visceral level. Neurotransmitters relay
these feelings to the orbitofrontal cortex which interprets and regulates them, generating
conscious awareness of emotional states such as joy or anger. The autonomic nervous
systems of a baby who experiences frequent intense attachment disruptions actually
shifts into a dysfunctional state in which abnormally high or low levels of stress and
pleasure hormones are produced. Over time, the abnormal neurochemical composition of
the infants brain becomes chronic and causes damage to the limbic and orbitofrontal
connectors and to the limbic region itself, damage that can be seen in a functional MRI
scan of the brain. (A functional MRI is a sophisticated neuro-imaging tool developed in
6
1993 that allows scientists to observe and record dynamic physiological processes as they
occur inside the brain).
What the researchers mean by intense attachment disruptions is chronic
misattunement and non-responsiveness or inappropriate responsiveness to the babys
emotional cues or, of course, outright abuse or neglect. When a caregiver promptly
responds with empathy to an infants distress, she is actually modulating the infants
neurological stress response so as to help the infant return to homeostasis. Recent
research has made the unhappy discovery that, when caregiver attunement is not
consistent, the alteration of neurochemical levels and resultant limbic and connective
impairment is permanent. It is no coincidence that the limbic circuits affected by
attachment deprivation are the very same ones targeted by modern psychiatric
medications that artificially alter the brains neurochemistry in such a way as to repair or
override damaged circuitry (Shore, 1997).
Normal brain development and mental health begin with an attuned caregivers
sensitive response to smiles, coos and cries of distress. As psychiatrist Daniel Siegel
(1999) explains, The attunement of emotional states provides the joining that is essential
for the developing brain to acquire the capacity to organize itself more autonomously as
the child matures.(p.278) And, the closer the babys body is to the caregivers, the more
appropriately responsive the caregiver can be. Acts of reciprocal affection like touch and
eye contact generate what neuro-psychiatrists call limbic resonance, a profoundly
satisfying state of emotional correspondence, the experience of which is what
distinguishes mammals from reptiles.
7
cultural factors (including racism, sexism, materialism, corporatization) and peer
influence as having equal or greater impact on a childs psychological well-being than
anything parents do or dont do. Any expert, from Dr. Spock to Dr. Sears, who dares to
suggest otherwise, is greeted with more than a modicum of suspicion and pilloried for
espousing the absurdly sentimental notion that a childs early familial experiences are
formative.
The tendency to go overboard in ascribing all aspects of child development to
parental omnipotence is a point well taken, and certainly all of the external factors listed
above have profound impacts on a childs development. But feminists stray too far into
wishful thinking when they insist that parental influence is minimal. The longstanding
nature versus nurture debate has in fact been more or less resolved by an emerging
consensus that ones genetic inheritance affords the individual a range of potential
personality traits, the development of which are a function of the interaction between
inborn temperament and social interactions and other experiences. As Dr. Siegel (1999)
explains, Genes contain the information for the general organization of the brains
structure, but experience determines which genes become expressed, how, and when
Early in life, interpersonal relationships are a primary source of the experience that
shapes how genes express themselves in the brain. (p. 14). The import of these
interpersonal experiences have their own hierarchy; early experiences have more impact
than later ones, and interactions with parents and primary caregivers are more formative
than interactions with siblings, peers, and others.
How well-tailored a parents care is to the young childs unique temperament
determines in large measure the childs capacity for emotional security and resilience,
qualities that in turn flavor the nature of the childs interactions with others and afford the
child more or less of an ability to cope with adversity and peer pressure. A follow-up
study of adults raised in the 1950s found that parental warmth (or lack thereof), more so
than poverty or divorce, was the strongest factor affecting the adults satisfaction with his
marriage, friendships and work (Goleman, 1991). This is not to deny that family poverty
causes enormous suffering and ill health and that its eradication is a moral imperative, but
rather to add that part of the tragedy of poverty is the way in which it sabotages parents
ability to nurture their children. There is no need to force people concerned about the
well-being of children and women to choose among parental, genetic or
social/environmental determinants of healthy child development, unless of course one is
attempting to rewrite the equation in a way that obscures maternal accountability.
The feminist critique of AP is a defensive measure in reaction to conservative
attacks against womens rights. Second wave feminism has focused primarily on
reproductive and workplace rights. The quarter-century old conservative backlash against
abortion, working mothers, and the scourge of broken or non-traditional (i.e. non-
patriarchal) families has kept feminists in a defensive posture. To be sure, if the retired
Reaganites and the active-duty Bush entourage were to fully realize their dream, women
would be even more vulnerable to coercive, full-time motherhood and less able to walk
away from unsafe or unfulfilling marriages, though optimism can be found in recent polls
showing strong public support for womens equality.
Social conservatives have on their side the long tradition of the sacralization of
motherhood--since the time of the Virgin Mary herself, an ever-evolving set of cultural
myths have prescribed the proper nature, role and behavior of mothers, all of which share
8
a paradoxical idealization and devaluation of motherhood. In other words, mothers have
always been held responsible for their childrens development, especially when it goes
awry, but given little no support in the child-rearing endeavor. The current version of the
myth is that good mothers are self-sacrificing, child-centered and pure of feeling in their
love for their children and should single-handedly perform their heroic mission without
compensation, recognition or support. Expressions of maternal ambivalence and the
desire to engage in pursuits other than parenting contradict the motherhood ideal and
expose the woman to a certain amount of cultural disdain.
Although the concerns underlying feminist criticism of AP are valid, the critique
itself is defective in several respects. First, there is a tendency to overlook or downplay
the accepted role of multiple caregivers in modern attachment theory, which has for
decades recognized that a childs attachment needs can be provided by a small, stable
corps of attachment figures, none of whom must necessarily be the mother. Modern
attachment theorists emphasize the importance of attachment-promoting care by fathers
and other non-maternal caregivers whose involvement not only guards against maternal
burnout but also prevents the infant from becoming so preoccupied with his relationship
with his mother that he has difficulty bonding with others in later childhood and
adulthood. For example, a recent study of Swedish children in high-quality day care
found that attachment security and non-parental care helped children develop social
competence (Bohlin, 2005).
What feminists often refer to as the discreditingof attachment theory is in fact a
refinement that does far more to corroborate than to challenge it. Alongside Freuds
discovery of the unconscious mind, attachment theory provides the very foundation for
the practice of modern psychology, not only because of its strong scientific basis but
because it resonates with what therapists and patients know to be true. Still, attachment
theory, like psychoanalysis, has a hard time shaking itself free from its original set of
sexist conclusionsthat maternal deprivation and only maternal deprivation causes
insecure attachment and that mothers are morally obligated to stay home for at least the
first year. Though this is no more the belief of most modern attachment theorists than it is
the belief of modern psychoanalysts that little girls suffer from penis envy, feminists with
a long memory for insult persist in their dismissal of attachment theory as psychobabble
pseudoscience (Eyer, 1996, p.102).
The only element of AP that men cannot perform is breastfeeding. Although the
nutritional superiority of breastfeeding is beyond dispute, it need not be treated as the
sine qua non of good enough parenting. There are many other attachment-promoting
techniques mothers and fathers can use if breastfeeding is not a realistic or desirable
option for their families. As a fervent believer in the benefits of breastfeeding and one
who looks back on the thousands of breastfeeding sessions with my son with great
fondness, this concession does not come easily. But, when I weigh it against the potential
for greater paternal involvement, I am compelled to relax my stance. For the legions of
mothers and fathers who would like to stay home and breast or bottle-feed their babies
for a period of time but cannot afford to do so, a feminist agenda that included financial
support for stay-at-home parents would serve them well. Still, it is difficult to disentangle
breastfeeding from the controversy over which parent (if any) stays home, for a desire to
feed their baby breast milk is one of the principal reasons why a couple would adopt the
traditional division of labor.
9
Lactation aside, the potential for fathers to practice AP is, in the feminist view,
still only theoretical. Because institutional daycare with up to six infants per caregiver (a
ratio of four to one is the norm) cannot by definition practice AP, parents who embrace
AP usually designate a stay-home parent for the childs first year. Given wage inequality
and the remnants of the age-old cultural taboo against male nurturing (as well, let us not
forget, the immutability of mammalian anatomy), most couples will still choose to have
mom stay home while dad goes out and maximizes the familys income. Thus do AP
proponents take on the starring role in the reincarnated cult of domesticity that keeps
women tied to the home. Douglass and Michaels book (2004) even includes a chapter
entitled Dr. Lauras Neighborhood: Baby-wearing, Nanny Cams, and the Triumph of the
New Momism), as though Dr. Lauras shrill call for cold-blooded nurturing bears any
resemblance to the kind of authentic bonding and affection promoted by AP.
In truth, fundamentalists like Dr. Laura are much more inclined to endorse tough
love approaches to parenting. I called in to Dr. Lauras radio show and was informed by
the call screener that Dr. Laura could not take my call because she had not heard of AP.
Dr. Lauras Christian brethren got themselves into trouble with the American Academy of
Pediatrics when they published Babywise, a popular baby care manual that gave parents
dangerous advice to enforce strict feeding schedules with their infants. The popular
conservative parenting expert, John Rosemond, who urges parents to focus more on
control than nurturance and endorsed Newt Gingrichs proposal to place the children of
teenage welfare mothers in orphanages, believes attachment theory is psychobabble
(Rankin, 2005). Meanwhile, Reverend James Dobsons Focus on the Family media
empire urges parents to spank children over seven months old hard enough to hurt. For
such conservatives, family values is shorthand for authoritarian child-rearing and a
package of regressive and hateful ideologies about the composition and structure of
marriage and family. The neo-traditionalist family movement is as unlikely to endorse AP
as it is gay marriage.
Most AP advocates believe that, ideally, one parent should stay home or that the
parents should try to work out a schedule that enables them both to engage in part-time
work and part-time child care and minimize the amount of time the baby spends in
daycare. In recent years, more pragmatic AP proponents have turned their attention to the
quality of daycare and, dismayed by what they see, are calling for a panoply of regulatory
reforms and subsidies that would enhance the quality and consistency of care. For now,
working parents can still take advantage of AP techniques to reestablish intimacy with
their babies by carrying and co-sleeping with them during non-work hours.
Still, babies who spend most of their days in institutional daycare do not receive
the constancy of attuned caregiving that babies of stay-at-home attachment parents can
bestow. It is this reality, more than anything, that makes feminists balk at AP.
Expostulating backwards from a womans right to work, even while her children are still
young, feminists go on the offensive against child-rearing advice that is infeasible for
dual earner or single parent working families. Fair enoughuntil the argument is carried
another step backward to arrive at the foregone conclusion that the advice in question is
based on a fabricated set of needs. Instead of asserting, We are concerned that working
parents cannot meet their babies attachment needs and demand that employers, day care
centers, hospitals, and government agencies do x, y and z to facilitate attachment and
bonding, a position that would push social policy in a progressive direction for all
10
concerned, they short-circuit the debate by denying that babies have needs that are
difficult for working parents to meet.
11
their children were little, Warner concludes that AP is yet another manifestation of yuppie
designer parenting. Douglas and Meredith (2004) not only lump AP in with hyper-
parenting but distort AP itself, summarizing it as follows: Reattach your baby to your
body the moment she is born and keep her there pretty much until she goes to college.
Funny, but wrong.
12
This critique is fair insofar as child-rearing experts have tended to overlook the
practical realities that undermine parents and have until recently ignored or condemned
mothers feelings of ambivalence and paid little more than lip service to womens desire
to pursue personal goals that do not revolve around their children. But from there on the
critique is, ironically, predicated on the very kind of dualistic thinking that patriarchy has
so successfully normalizedthe dividing of the world into the powerful and the obedient,
victor and vanquished, detached and connected, male and female, self and otherthe
dividing of ourselves against ourselves. Viewed dualistically, the parent-child relationship
is a zero-sum game in which the child flourishes at the expense of the parents personal
fulfillment. In this paradigm, the parents power over the child is threatened by the
imposed expectation that the parent meet the childs need for deep connection. AP, then,
is seen as an unacceptable inversion of the hierarchy of power, making parent slave to
child. As Adrienne Rich 1976) observed, the powerful do not have to enter intuitively
into the souls of the powerless, or to hear what they are saying in their many languages
Colonialism exists by virtue of this short-cut---how else could so few live among so
many and understand so little (p.65). Mothers and fathers have total power over their
infants, but when they listen and respond to their babies when they tell them what they
need, the parents may feel they surrender some of this power. By stepping outside of the
patriarchal mindset, parents are free to be as responsive as they want to be without fear of
servitude.
Attachment parents are not deluded slaves who have been duped into feeling
fulfilled by having a baby attached to their bodies 24/7. They are parents who have
transcended the dualistic paradigm and see their family as a web of intersecting needs and
desires that is constantly rewoven to accommodate evolving interests. Attachment parents
see their attuned caregiving style not as a form of over-gratification that will create a
spoiled tyrant but as a reflection of their loving recognition of the special dependency
needs of their children during the first precious year of life. To the extent that these
parents are able to organize their lives in such a way as to facilitate attachment-promoting
practices (and Im not saying this is easy), they derive a great deal of emotional and
sensual satisfaction from being in close contact with their warm, cooing, baby-smelling
little bundles. Limbic resonance is bi-directionally restorativea parent can enjoy close
contact with his baby as much as the baby needs it. As Rachel Cusk (2001) admits in her
otherwise depressing indictment of motherhood, I remain surprised by how proximate
the mythology of motherhood is to its reality (p. 205). No matter how disillusioned and
exhausted new mothers may be, there is still some aspect of childbirth and infant care that
corresponds to the expectation of awe, fusion and bliss.
Contrary to the feminist portrayal of AP as oppressively labor-intensive, it may in
fact represent an easier and more fulfilling approach, particularly for parents of high-
needs babies whose fussiness can be reduced by consistent babywearing. Attachment
parents avoid some of the pitched battles around weaning, pacifier dependency and sleep
resistance that mainstream parents often contend with. I know for myself that
breastfeeding was as much a personal convenience as it was a nutritional gift to my baby
as I write this article in anticipation of adopting a newborn baby, I am filled with more
than a little dread at the prospect of going down to the kitchen to heat up bottles of
formula several times a night instead of rolling over in bed and letting the baby nurse
back to sleep. I am grateful not to be parenting during the behaviorist era, and take
13
comfort in the belief that, by practicing AP, I add my voice to the chorus that will
eventually drown out the last vestiges of one of the most destructive child-rearing
philosophies in recorded history.
For many parents, AP is merely a label for a style they intuitively believe will
foster intimacy and joy in their relationships with their children. Though carrying a baby
in arms is more labor intensive than leaving a baby alone in her crib most of the time, the
satisfaction is, for many parents, well worth the effort. It is strikingly coincidental that the
proportion of women (roughly a third) who do not derive a strong sense of meaning from
motherhood is equivalent to the proportion of insecurely attached parents and children; is
it the same third? (consult Boulton, 1983, and Louis & Margolies, 1987) for discussions
of maternal discontent). Perhaps this third is imprisoned in a child-rearing time warp in
which parents are so determined not to spoil their babies that they wind up spoiling the
entire experience of parenthood. If they knew that there was an alternative way to care for
their babies that would in all likelihood yield better mental health outcomes for
themselves and their babies, how many more would choose it? What if women and men
were given the choice?
Whereas Warners (2005) mothers are trapped in a neurotic bind that compels
them to parent in a way that increases their anxiety and discontent, many parents choice
of attachment-promoting techniques is a reflection of their psychological freedom to do
things differently from their parents and from mainstream society and to appreciate the
uniqueness of their childs first year of lifeuniquely demanding and exhausting yes, but
also uniquely formative and flush with opportunities to experience a kind of love and joy
that many adults have never before known. Warners mothers are missing out on the joy
of parenthood, not because of attachment parenting, but in spite of it.
When the going gets rough, when sleep deprivation, mastitis, backaches and one
missed shower too many overwhelm their sense of well-being, they do not look to AP as
the evil culprit that robbed them of their autonomy but, rather, to their co-parents,
communities and society for support. Agitating for this kind of support, then, rather than
scuttling the needs of children, should be the aim of feminists.
Beyond the zero sum game: Societys responsibility for the next generation
Children do not ask to be born, and when they are it is the responsibility of their
parents and of society to meet their nurturance needs. Child development experts and
indeed many feminists writers have promulgated a wide variety of family-support
proposals that would help parents care for their young children in the way they deserve.
Such proposals include paid parental leave, flex-time, on-site daycare, daycare quality
reforms and subsidies, neighborhood family drop-in centers, mental health care, parent
education classes, expanded dependent child tax credits, tax credits for stay-home
parents, compassionate welfare reform, and many more. Amazingly, not one of these is
on the active agenda of the mainstream feminist movement (though, as I write this, a new
committee of the National Organization for Women is beginning to take up some of these
issues). In fact, many womens organizations would actively oppose measures to
financially support stay-home parents for more than a few post-partum months.
The scorn of many feminists toward stay-at-home mothers and the failure of the
womens movement to speak to the desire of stay-at-home and working mothers and
14
fathers to provide optimal care for their children illustrates the capitalist orientation of
mainstream liberal feminism. At its core rests the assumption that personal fulfillment is a
function of the equal right to work, despite the fact that the majority of women have
demeaning or tedious service sector or pink collar jobs that barely fulfill a womans
financial let alone aspirational needs. Is working the cash register at Wal-Mart more
liberating than caring for ones own child? Perhaps some women would find this to be the
case, but most I have to assume would not. Until feminists start challenging capitalist and
patriarchal values such as hyper-individualism, hierarchy, competition, production and
materialism, the womens movement will never achieve for women more than the equal
right to be exploited and for girls and boys the right to repeat with their own children
what was done to them. As Barbara Katz Rothman (1989) urges, A feminist agenda must
go beyond calls for inclusion into the world that is (p. 253).
Perhaps Janna Malamud Smith (2003) put it best when she wrote,
The best mother is a free mother. Simply stated, a mother who feels
valued and supported by her family, neighborhood, workplace, and
society will have an easier time communicating to her children a sense
that they are loved and valued. Conversely, a society cannot care
adequately for children without respecting and caring for mothers.
(p. 187)
Unfortunately, Smith also believes that a free mother is one who dismisses the
advice of child development experts as mother-blaming psychobabble. A child needs a
mother who is not constantly abraded by philosophies that inflate her accountability
while obscuring her effort (p. 168). Smiths point rings true for the most part but hits a
shrill note when carried too far. No ones interests are served when childrens needs are
exaggerated, nor when they are denied. To the extent that child development experts have
hit upon needs that most modern parents cannot reasonably be expected to fulfill, the
resolution lies not in parent-child needs fulfillment competition but in the formulation of
a pro-family political agenda that aims to enhance the nurturing capacities of families and
communities.
Conclusion
For many new parents, especially those who are single, poor and/or have twins or
several young children, AP may be an unattainable ideal. As a friend who provided child
care for extremely poor families reminded me, parents who rely on welfare benefits
spend much of their time pushing their strollers around from one government office to the
next and can hardly be expected to have the stamina to carry their babies while walking
the long city blocks and standing in endless lines. On the other hand, Ive seen plenty of
parents shleping around babies in cumbersome car seats when they could more easily
have carried the baby in a Bjornslingalas, Bjorns slings cost twice as much as strollers.
And for babies at high risk for SIDS, there is evidence that breastfeeding and co-sleeping
may have life-saving value.
The existence of real-life impediments to practicing AP does not mean that
attachment theory is wrong and that babies in fact are perfectly well-served when they
15
spend their days in strollers and car seats. Rather, it means that it is the responsibility of
adults who care about the well-being of our youngest and most vulnerable to give support
and encouragement to parents so that they can give their babies as much tender, loving
care as they possibly can.
Not every contradiction that AP and motherhood present can be reconciled,
whether within or outside of the dualistic paradigm. It would be disingenuous to pretend
that, so long as the mother practices AP, hers and the babys interests are always aligned.
Until fathers play an equal role in child-rearing and society values and supports parents,
the notion that mother-child conflicts of interest must be resolved in favor of the child
goes down with feminists as easily as an uncoated pre-natal vitamin.
Writing about parenthood more generally, Cornel West and Sylvia Ann Hewlett
(1998) had this to say:
References
16
your premature infant. New York: Bantam Books.
Rankin, J. (2005). Parenting Experts. London: Praeger Publishing.
Rich, A. (1976). Of woman born. New York: Norton and Company.
Rothman, B.K. (1989). Recreating motherhood. New York: Norton.
Schore, A. (1997). Early organization of the nonlinear right brain and development of a
predisposition to psychiatric disorders. Development and Psychopathology 9.
Sears, W., & Sears, M. (1992/2003). The baby book. New York: Sears & Sears
Siegel, D. (1999). The developing mind: Toward a neurobiology of interpersonal
experience. New York: Guilford Press.
Siegel, D. & Hartzell, M. (2003). Parenting from the inside out. New York:
Tarcher/Putnam.
Small, M. (1998). Our babies, ourselves: How biology and culture shape the way we
Parent. New York: Anchor Books.
Smith, J.M., (2003) A potent spell. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Sroufe, A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W.A. (2005). The development of the
person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood.
New York: Guilford Press.
Warner, J. (2005). Perfect madness: Motherhood in the age of anxiety. New York:
Penguin Group.
17