Meyer Herscovitch 2001 Commitment in The Workplace Toward A General Model
Meyer Herscovitch 2001 Commitment in The Workplace Toward A General Model
PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
2000,53
This research was financially supported by a grant from the University of Amsterdam.
I thank Danielle Galdeij, Suzanne de Haan, Margot Roozen, Larissa Wladimiroff, and
Mirjarn van der Zande for collecting the data, and Benjamin Schneider, Carsten De Dreu,
Agneta Fischer, Daan van Knippenberg, the editor, and the reviewers for their suggestions.
Correspondence and requests for reprints should be addressed to Annelies E. M. Van
Vianen, University of Amsterdam, Department of Work and Organizational Psychology,
Roetersstraat 15, 1018 WB Amsterdam, The Netherlands; (+31) 205256856; Fax (+31)
206390531; [email protected].
113
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
114 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
1 Culture and climate are complimentary topics (Schneider, 1987). The operationaliza-
tions used in this study refer both to organizational practices and basic values underlying
these practices. However, because our operationalizations were comparable with the op-
erationalizations used in previous studies on person-culture fit (O'Reilly, et al., 1991), we
decided to use the concept of organizational culture. We do, however, realize that the
conceptualization of organizational culture (see Schein, 1990) is far more comprehensive
than the one used in this study.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 115
One of the P-0 fit models that has initiated much empirical research
in the past decade is Schneider's attraction-selection-attrition (ASA)
framework (Schneider, 1987). This framework describes the mechanism
of mutual adaptation between the person and the organization. People
are not randomly assigned to organizations, but they select themselves
into and out of organizations. This selection process includes several
steps. First, people find organizations differentially attractive as a func-
tion of their judgment of the congruence between the characteristics of
the organization and their own characteristics (Cable & Judge, 1997;
Schneider, Goldstein, & Smith, 1995). A second step in the matching
process is the selection procedure through which those people are hired
who have the attributes the organization desires. Finally, once people
have become citizens of the organization and find that they do not fit
their work environment, they will tend to leave.
A basic issue in measuring P-0 fit is how to conceptualize and opera-
tionalize the components of the fit measure, especially the 0-component
of this measure. Organizational culture is usually operationalized by
measuring the shared perceptions of organizational citizens, for instance,
by asking people what they experience as the basic values of their orga-
nization. Schneider et al. (1995) emphasized that "persons make envi-
ronments" (p. 751) and that situations should not be conceptualized as
separate and distinct from the individuals behaving in them. The person-
ality attributes and attitudes of people in a setting are considered to be
the fundamental defining characteristics of that setting. Hence, should
organizational culture be measured based on people's perceptions about
the existing culture, as was done in most P-0 fit studies, or should orga-
nizational culture be assessed in terms of characteristics of people in the
setting? In short, do people's characteristics (the P-component) need
to match the shared perceptions of others about organizational charac-
teristics or do they need to match other people's characteristics in the
organization?
The central assumption of the ASA model that people make the place
was tested recently (Schneider, Smith, Thylor, & Fleenor, 1998). Schnei-
der et al. revealed that organizations are relatively homogeneous with
respect to the personality attributes of their managers. This test of ASA
theory offers an empirical argument to incorporate individual charac-
teristics for assessing P-0 fit. However, the culture of a work setting
is not only constituted by means of the characteristics of people. Many
aspects of organizational life may be influenced by the personality and
attitudes of people in the organization, but this does not mean that the
culture of a work setting origins in the characteristics of people. The
organization's strategic position and environment are, for instance, im-
portant determinants of shared culture perceptions because they impose
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
116 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 117
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
118 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
& Tan, 1992; Tan & Singh, 1995), similarity of values (Johnson, 1989)
and similarity of personality (Byrne, Griffit, & Stefaniak 1967; Dyce
& O'Conner, 1992; Moskowitz & Cote, 1995). Studies on attitude and
value similarity, however, showed the most unequivocal results: People
like those who hold similar attitudes and opinions more than those with
dissimilar attitudes (Byrne, Clare & Smeaton, 1986; Condon & Crano,
1988; Shaikh & Kanekar, 1994). Moreover, interpersonal attraction
among members of a work group was found to be related to group cohe-
sion and job satisfaction of those work groups (Colarelli & Boos, 1992).
Support was also found for the positive relationship between value simi-
larity and individual job satisfaction and commitment (Meglino, Ravlin,
& Adkins, 1989; Salancik & Pfeffer, 1978). In this study, we therefore
hypothesized that P-P fit, conceptualized as the similarity between an
individual's culture preferences and those preferences of others, would
affect individual outcomes.
The purpose of the present study was to examine the effects of two
different measures of fit on individual outcomes. First, we compared or-
ganizational culture preferences of newcomers in the organization with
organizational culture as perceived by others in the work setting (P-0
fit). Second, we compared organizational culture preferences of new-
comers in the organization with culture preferences of others in the work
setting (P-P fit). Both fit measures were related to newcomer's organi-
zational commitment and turnover intentions. Because there are several
possible functional forms of the relationship between fit and outcomes,
we also tested each of these forms for both fit measures.
P-0 fit has been conceptualized in several ways (Kristof, 1996). Dis-
tinctions are made between supplementary and complementary fit and
between supplies-values and demands-abilities fit. Supplementary fit
occurs when an individual possesses characteristics that are similar to
those of others in the environment. For instance, when a person has
similar preferences and attitudes as the other members of his or her
work team. Complementary fit occurs when an individual's characteris-
tics add to the environment what is missing. This is the case when a per-
son has special skills that are beneficial for functioning of the work team.
Other researchers distinguished between supplies-values and demands-
abilities fit (French, Caplan, & Harrison, 1982; Schuler, 1980). From the
supplies-values perspective, P-0 fit occurs when an organization satis-
fies individuals' values. Values represent conscious desires held by the
person and encompass preferences, interests, motives, and goals (Ed-
wards, 1996). For instance, when an employee has high preferences
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 119
Supplies-Value Fit
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
120 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 121
an individual is from the other members of the team the more likely is
his or her turnover.
In the present study, we operationalized individual characteristics
as individual's organizational culture preferences (see Judge & Cable,
1997). We investigated the congruence between newcomers' and re-
cruiters' organizational culture preferences (P-P fit). Based on similar-
ity-attraction theories and previous empirical studies we hypothesized
that P-P fit between newcomers' preferences and recruiters' prefer-
ences would be related to newcomers' organizational commitment and
turnover intentions.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
122 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 123
ers (i.e., subjective P-0 fit) for different culture dimensions. They re-
vealed that actual turnover was related to fit indices for only some di-
mensions of organizational culture. These dimensions mainly concerned
elements of the open systems model and the rational goal model of
Quinn's (1988) framework, that is, risk orientation and work pressure.
Moreover, quadratic difference indices yielded significant results for the
(curvilinear) relationships between cultural dimensions (i.e., risk orien-
tation, reward, and work pressure) and affective outcomes (i.e., job sat-
isfaction, commitment, and turnover intentions).
In the present study we therefore hypothesized that commitment
would decrease (Hypothesis 1) and turnover intentions would increase
(Hypothesis 2) as culture supplies exceed or fall short of culture pref-
erences for culture dimensions concerning the open systems model and
the rational goal model. These hypotheses assume the highest outcomes
along the line of perfect fit. The longitudinal study of Van Vianen and
Prins (1997), however, showed that the magnitude of newcomers' prefer-
ences also contributed to actual turnover. Newcomers with high prefer-
ences for risk orientation more often changed their job than newcomers
with low preferences for risk orientation, but these preferences were not
associated with turnover intentions as these were assessed in the first
months of employment. Because very few empirical evidence is avail-
able about the combined effects of optimal fit and the magnitude of its
components, we decided to develop hypotheses about the main func-
tional forms as discussed above (i.e., monotonic, asymptotic, or curvi-
linear relationships). Our data will be analyzed with using polynomial
regression, which implies a further exploration of specific aspects of fit,
such as the relationships between the components of the fit measure and
outcomes.
The human relations model of organizational culture includes cul-
ture facets such as the amount of peer support, feedback, and participa-
tion. These aspects of organizational life are highly valued by employ-
ees. Peer cohesion, for instance, received the highest preferences ratings
in the study of Van Vianen and Prins (1997). Their results revealed a
main effect for organizational supplies (as perceived by the newcomer).
Newcomers perceiving fewer peer support were less satisfied and less
committed to the organization and showed higher turnover intentions
than those experiencing more peer support. Weak evidence was found
for the relationship between an algebraic fit index for peer support and
commitment, which actually can be labeled as deficiency.
Further support for a deficiency relationship between fit measure
and affective outcomes concerning the human relations dimension of or-
ganizational culture can be found in a study of Downey, Hellriegel, and
Slocum (1975). They revealed that individuals requiring social contact
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
124 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 125
Method
Participants
Subjects were 154 newcomers, 104 peers, and 101 supervisors from 68
different organizations in The Netherlands. Among these organizations
there were four large companies with 10 to 22 newcomers. These new-
comers, however, were employed in different departments, located in
different areas of the country. The organizations and participants were
asked for their participation by a Dutch personnel selection consultancy
which had been involved in selecting the newcomers. Newcomers were
97 males and 52 females, subject's sex was missing for 5 subjects. Av-
erage age of newcomers was 29.8 (SD = 5.4). They were all higher ed-
ucated and employed fulltime. Mean job tenure was 6 months (SD =
3.9). Peers were 69 males and 34 females, sex of one subject was miss-
ing. Average age of peers was 34.6 (SD = 9.5). They were all higher
educated and employed fulltime. Mean job tenure was 7.2 years (SD =
10.9). Supe!Visors were 88 males and 13 females. Mean age was 42.2 ( SD
= 7.7). They were all higher educated and employed fulltime. Mean job
tenure was 10.5 years (SD = 11.9).
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
126 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 127
bach's alphas were .78 and .85. Effort was measured with 5 items con-
cerning the amount of effort that is expected from the employees and
work morale, such as: " ... it is expected that employees put more ef-
fort into their work than they are paid for." Cronbach's alphas were .78
and .85. Competition consisted of 7 items, such as: " ... employees are
expected to compete with each other." Cronbach's alphas were .85 and
.89.
Two fit measures were used in this study. P-0 fit was assessed by com-
paring newcomer's culture preferences with organizational culture sup-
plies, as perceived by the recruiters. P-P fit was assessed by comparing
newcomer's culture preferences with recruiters' preferences. The two
fit measures were estimated with different fit indices. Furthermore, we
tested the data on the amount of deficiency or excess, that is, the propor-
tion of negative and positive scores. Finally, polynomial regression was
used in order to avoid methodological problems with P-0 fit measures
(Edwards, 1994, 1996).
Affective outcomes were commitment and turnover intentions. Com-
mitment was measured with 7 items, selected from the Organizational
Commitment Questionnaire (OCQ; Mowday, Steers, & Porter, 1979).
These items can be described as affective commitment (Alien & Meyer,
1990). One of the items is: "I have this organization very much at heart."
Cronbach's alpha was .89. Turnover intentions were measured with 3
items derived from Ten Horn (1983). Cronbach's alpha was .81. One
of the items is: " When I have the opportunity to work in another orga-
nization, I will take that opportunity."
Results
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
128 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
TABI..El
Descriptive Statistics and Zero-Order Correlations For Measures of Preferences, Supplies, and Affective Outcomes
M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
Preferences: Concern for people
1. Newcomers 5.91 .56 ~
2. Peers 5.86 .55 .02 ~
3. Supervisors 5.88 .50 -.12 .03 t::
trl
Preferences: Goal accomplishment Cll
4. Newcomers 4.47 .83 .15 .03 -.03 trl
5. Peers 4.42 .85 -.22 -.17 .21 .29** - ~
6. Supervisors 4.84 .61 -.06 .14 .22* .33** .20
Supplies: Concern for people ~
7. Peers
8. Supervisors
4.41
4.97
.90
.72
.01
-.03
.17
.05
-.01
.39**
.09
- .21
.06
-.06
.02
.06 .11
;:;
z~
Supplies: Goal accomplishment
9. Peers 4.22 .86 -.26* .02 .02 .23* .29* .36** .40** .15
10. Supervisors 4.50 .80 -.16 .07 .23 -.15 .03 .40** -.13 .49** .35*
Affective outcomes
11. Commitment 3.72 .78 .19* .01 -.02 .07 .00 .01 .30** -.06 .11 -.07
12. Turnover intentions 2.38 1.15 -.08 .14 -.03 .02 .08 -.07 -.15 .02 - .07 -.07 -.71**
Note: **p < .01 *p < .05 (two-tailed).
N
\.0
130 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 131
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
......
w
N
TABLE2
Unconstrained Equtltions for Asymptotic and Optimal P- 0 and P-P Models
:3D .·.
... . -·,
~
zo
10
!,•" ·' i~
G) (/')
~ ...
t'l1
-~
0
~
%
(.)
~
$
...,
......
......
V>
~
10
-c
Q)
'"Cl
§
"§ ~
10 ~
Cll
g
<.)
I
, ~
__
--a...
-
~~ _...."- ..
· )=
.··.
··- . .
";>
~<b~
'"Cl
Cll
--<
@
0
5
Q
~~ ~,~~0
~ .!!>
Figure 1b: Concern for People P-Ope er Measures Predicting Newcomer Commitment.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 135
.,;
~
=
=
...i
• - •• - -.·-- · - . . . . • • • • •• -.!
...
·
.".
I
......
c\
·... ..
.
,, 0
~ ~
loo
l.l
E
Fl
~
c
Ill)
~
]
~
"'
~
"'i1
' ~
.. ...
"""-
.. '
a
.' .
.' '•
,
0..
I
' .,t'
''
'' .!l
Cl.
~
~
.~. (,
·. '~l
loo
.s
E
t!
~ ~
rl~ u
=
c
SUO\\Ua\\1\ 1ai\OU1~\1.
.....i;
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
136 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M . VAN VIANEN 137
Regression coefficients for the P-P fit measures are shown in Ta-
ble 2. We hypothesized similarity effects, that is, newcomers' commit-
ment would decrease (Hypothesis 6) and newcomers' turnover inten-
tions would increase (Hypothesis 7) as their culture preferences exceed
or fall short of the culture preferences of others. Moreover, we assumed
that P-P fit with the supervisor would be more important for newcomer's
commitment and turnover intentions than P-P fit with a peer.
Table 2 reveals significant multiple correlations for the unconstrained
regression equations for the comparisons of concern for people prefer-
ences of newcomers and supervisors predicting newcomer commitment
and turnover intentions (R 2 = .186; F (5, 87) = 3.694, p < .01 and R 2
= .161; F (5, 87) = 3.119, p < .05). No significant relationships were
found for concern for people P-P fit with peers. However, the regression
equations for the goal accomplishment dimension predicting newcomer
turnover intentions showed significant results for the comparison with
peer preferences (R 2 = .174; F (5, 68) = 2.607, p < .05). Surfaces for
the three significant equations (see Figures 2a-2c) show the relation-
ships between the component measures and outcomes.
The surface for supervisor and newcomer concern for people pref-
erences predicting commitment (Figure 2a) is somewhat concave, be-
cause the downward curvature is least along the first principal axis (a~
= -.254, ns) and greatest along the second principal axis (a~ = -1.338,
ns). The slope along theY =X line is positive (a. = 4.614, ns), indi-
cating that commitment is higher when supervisor and newcomer pref-
erences are both high than when both are low. The slope along the Y
= -X line is slightly negative and curved downward (a. = - .946, ns;
a~ = -.766, p < .05). These results show that commitment decreased
somewhat more at the right side of the line of perfect fit, thus when new-
comer concern for people preferences exceeded those of the supervisor
than when it fell short of supervisor preferences. Hypothesis 6 was only
partly supported, that is, commitment was highest when supervisor and
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
-...,
00
10
-c
Q)
""C
E ....n •-- trl
~ -~- :;o
"E C/l
g -.n 0
(.)-.7-'
~
.AO ~
""C
C/l
....::
(")
~ ~- ::r:
~-6.~~0
~ .....~ 0
'!> ----- - -
--
---- -- _....;.--::; ..., l'
0
~ -~;-
_..... --- ~~0"-
"':...._.t;-0-~ ~
'
"
~ -
/.';; ~ -~,d- <;p"""
, _ ~:?:>
~
Figure 2a: Concern for People P - Psupervisor Measures Predicting Newcomer Commitment.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 139
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
......
""'"
0
"'c::
0
-;::;
c::
~
"'1:1
4)
~
(/)
g 0
:s ~
..... t'I1
r
'1::1
(/)
....::
()
::r:
0
,,· .•• ::~
.-.--·· ••• -"1.-~ 5
• • • ••
_.....
•• <;:) ";.._<b<b~
~0..... -
Q
~,d-~-
Figure 2b: Concern for People P - Psupervisor Measures Predicting Newcomer Turnover Intentions.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
30
J • ___ ,
I •'
20
tJ>
c::
0
·;:;
~
c::
~ 10
(j)
~ tr1
c:: (/)
:; tr1
.......
··. ~
~
' ~
~ '2- •.
~ .....
~
,~~
.. -·. ···.
"<>... -".
, .·
......_"' .....
.. ..
,•",
..
.... ·
. ..
. < ·.
-~ . . ~.......:.,#'
~ I
~ ~ !1.- .-0!;.~
-=' ~....<$ c;y-
~
Figure 2c: Concern for goal accomplishment P- Ppeer Measures Predicting Newcomer Thrnover Intentions. ......
e
142 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Discussion
Most P-0 fit research has been primarily concerned with the match
between persons and organizations. The purpose of the present study
was to address some limitations of this approach to person-organization
fit. We argued that matching persons with organizations is a restricted
operationalization of the theoretical assumptions underlying the attrac-
tion-selection-attrition (ASA) model (Schneider, 1987; Schneider et al.,
1995). ASA theory assumes that individuals' characteristics are the ba-
sic ingredients of existing organizational culture. This implies that the
match between individuals and their organization also should refer to
the match between individuals' characteristics and the characteristics of
others in the organization. Schneider et al. (1998) provided empirical
support for this assumption underlying ASA theory by showing that or-
ganizations are relatively homogeneous with respect to personality at-
tributes. Thus, the mechanism of mutual attraction between the per-
son and the organization can partly be predicted by similarity of people.
In addition to personality attributes, P-0 fit researchers emphasize that
values (often operationalized as preferences) are also important com-
ponents on which to evaluate fit because they are fundamental and rela-
tively enduring. Moreover, they guide individuals' attitudes, judgments,
and behaviors (Chatman, 1989, 1991). In the present study, we there-
fore focused on the role of individuals' preferences for organizational
cultures. We compared the culture preferences of newcomers with orga-
nizational supplies (the traditional approach). In addition, we compared
newcomers' culture preferences with the preferences of salient others in
their work setting. There were two main reasons to expect that this sec-
ond operationalization of fit would be an important predictor for affec-
tive outcomes. First, if newcomers' culture preferences are similar to the
preferences of salient others these newcomers probably will match the
organizational culture, because those similar others already have proven
to match (they did not leave). Second, if newcomers show similar prefer-
ences as others in the work setting, they will feel attracted towards those
others. Feelings of similarity and attraction will affect their individual
work attitudes.
The results of this study lend some support for the hypothesized role
of P-P fit for newcomers' commitment and turnover intentions, but only
for the comparison between newcomers and supervisors. Newcomers'
P-P fit with their supervisors concerning the "concern for people" di-
mension of culture preferences appeared to be related to newcomers'
affective outcomes. When both newcomers and supervisors showed
high concern for people preferences newcomers were more commit-
ted. Overall, incongruency between the supervisor and the newcomer
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 143
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
144 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
ting are those with whom one communicates frequently, that is, new-
comers' peers and their supervisors. The similarity-attraction perspec-
tive assumes that newcomers who hear others express their preferred
culture values will feel more committed to their work setting than new-
comers who hear others express different culture values. SIP theory,
however, proposes that other people's preferences will affect newcom-
ers' attitudes only if newcomers consider the values of others to be rel-
evant. The results of this study suggest that for the concern for goal ac-
complishment dimension of organizational culture newcomers use their
peers' preferences as a frame of reference. For the concern for people
dimension they rely more on the preferences of their supervisor. New-
comers may expect the concern for goal accomplishment preferences of
their supervisors to be high and they therefore focus on the preferences
of their peers. If their peers show high concern for goal accomplish-
ment preferences-such as high preferences for competition-this may
hold a promise for a challenging work environment. Furthermore, new-
comers may expect the concern for people preferences of their peers to
be high and comparable to their own preferences. The concern for peo-
ple preferences of their supervisor, however, may be conceived of as less
predictable. Newcomers probably do realize that the concern for people
preferences of their supervisor are one of the main sources for creating
(and changing) the human side of organizational culture (Schein, 1985),
such as the amount of supportive feedback and opportunities for partici-
pation. They therefore put weight to the concern for people preferences
of their supervisors.
This is one of the first studies that examined P-P fit measures in
the context of personal selection. Our findings have some implications
for the application of fit measures in personnel selection procedures.
Practitioners may hesitate to include P-0 fit measures in their selection
procedures, because of problems with measuring organizational culture.
Moreover, incorporating a measure of applicant's values into the person-
nel selection maybe risky in applied organizational settings. Such mea-
sures are fakable and there is no clear evidence that a person's values are
related to the ability to perform the job. The results of the present study
indicate that comparing applicants' preferences with the preferences of
their recruiters (i.e., supervisor and peers) is a way to establish fit. This
would be applied in developing realistic job previews or in the establish-
ment of recruitment and retention programs. Of course, more research
is needed in order to confirm and extend our P-P fit findings.
Our results for the P-0 fit measures did not collaborate with previ-
ous studies showing P-0 fit effects. This is probably due to our opera-
tionalization of P-0 fit. Other P-0 fit studies used aggregated measures
of organizational culture based on the perceptions of a large number of
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 145
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
146 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
some doubts about the construct that was measured. Although both con-
cern for people and concern for goal accomplishment dimensions reflect
some basic values underlying organizational culture (see Schein, 1990)
we do recognize that these two dimensions do not cover organizational
culture. Future research should adopt a broader approach to organiza-
tional culture.
This study has shown some important implications for future P-0
fit studies. The comparisons between newcomers, peers, and supervi-
sors yielded relative homogeneity with respect to the concern for peo-
ple preferences of organizational citizens. Note, for instance, that su-
pervisors and peers were most similar regarding their concern for peo-
ple preferences. This result is in line with the conclusion of Schnei-
der et al. (1998) that organizations are relatively homogeneous with
regard to personality characteristics. However, the personality char-
acteristics used in their study referred to social behavior and interper-
sonal relationships (thinking-feeling, sensing-intuition, extraversion-
introversion, judging-perceiving). Personality characteristics such as
conscientiousness and achievement motivation that are related to the
concern for goal accomplishment dimension of organizational life were
not included. These latter personality characteristics may be less homo-
geneous in organizations, because they are, for instance, more related to
specific jobs or positions. Future studies, therefore, should consider di-
mensions of organizational and individual values that are specifically rel-
evant for effects of supplementary, complementary, or supplies-values
fit on individual and organizational outcomes.
REFERENCES
Alien NJ, Meyer JP. (1990). The measurement and antecedents of affective, continuance
and normative commitment to the organization. Journal of Occupational Psychol-
ogy, 63, 1-18.
Bretz RD Jr, Judge TA (1994). Person-organization fit and the theory of work adjustment:
Implications for satisfaction, tenure, and career success. Journal of Vocational Be-
havior, 44, 32-54.
Byrne D. (1971 ). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic press.
Byrne D, Clare GL, Smeaton G. (1986). The attraction hypothesis: Do similar attitudes
affect anything? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1167-1170.
Byrne D, Griffit WB, Stefaniak D. (1967). Attraction and similarity of personality charac-
teristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 82-90.
Cable OM, Judge TA (1997). Interviewers' perceptions of person-Drganization fit and
organizational selection decisions. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 82, 546-561.
Caplan RD. (1987). Person-environment fit theory and organizations: commensurate
dimensions, time perspectives, and mechanisms. Journal of Vocational Behavior,
31,248-267.
Chatman JA (1989). Improving interactional organizational research: A model of person-
organization fit. Academy of Management Review, 14, 333-349.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 147
Chatman JA. (1991). Matching people and organizations: Selection and socialization in
public accounting firms. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 459-484.
Colarelli SM, Boos AL. ( 1992). Sociometric and ability-based assignment to work groups:
Some implications for personnel selection. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13,
187-196.
Condon J\V, Crano WD. (1988). Inferred evaluation and the relation between attitude
similarity and interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54, 789-797.
De Dreu CKW, Harinck F, Van Vianen AEM. (1999). Conflict and performance in groups
and organizations. In Robertson IT, Cooper CL (Eds.), International Review of
Industrial and Organizational Psychology (pp. 369-414). West Sussex: Wiley.
Downey HK, Hellriegel D, Slocum JW. (1975). Congruence between individual needs,
organizational climate, job satisfaction and performance. Academy of Management
Journal, 18, 149-155.
Dyce J, O'Conner BP. (1992). Personality complementarity as a determinant of group
cohesion in bar bands. Small Group Research, 23, 185-198.
Edwards JR. (1993). Problems with the use of profile similarity indices in the study of
congruence in organizational research. PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 46, 641-665.
Edwards JR. (1994). The study of congruence in organizational behavior research: Cri-
tique and a proposed alternative. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 58,51-100.
Edwards JR. (1996). An examination of competing versions of the person--environment
fit approach to stress. Academy of Management Journa~ 39, 292-339.
Edwards JR, Van Harrison R. (1993). Demands and worker health: Three-dimensional
reexamination of the relationship between person--environment fit and strain. Jour-
nal of Applied Psychology, 78, 628-648.
Edwards JR, Parry ME. (1993). On the use of polynomial regression equations as anal-
ternative to difference scores in organizational research. Academy of Management
Journa~ 36, 1577-1613.
French JRP Jr, Caplan RD, Harrison RV (1982). The mechanisms ofjob stress and strain.
London: Wiley.
Furnham A, Gunter B. (1993). Corporate culture: Definition, diagnosis, and change.
International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 8, 233-261.
Jackson SE, Brett JF, Sessa VI, Cooper DM, Julin JA, Peyronnin K. (1991). Some dif·
ferences make a difference: Individual dissimilarity and group heterogeneity as
correlates of recruitment, promotions, and turnover. Journal ofApplied Psychology,
76, 675-{)89.
lames LR, Demaree RG, Wolf G. (1984). Estimating within-group interrater reliability
with and without response bias. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 69, 85-98.
Johnson MA (1989). Variables associated with friendship in an adult population. Journal
of Social Psychology, 129, 379-390.
Joyce WF, Slocum JW (1990). Strategic context and organizational climate. In Schneider
B (Ed.), Organizational climate and culture (pp. 130-150). San Francisco: Jossey-
Bass.
Judge TA, Cable DM. (1997). Applicant personality, organizational culture, and organi·
zation attraction. PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 50, 359-394.
Koene BA, Boone CAJJ, Soeters JL. (1997). Organizational factors influencing homo-
geneity and heterogeneity of organizational cultures. In Sackmann SA (Ed.), Cul-
tural complexity in organizations: Inherent contrasts and contradictions (pp. 273-293).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Kristof AL. (1996). Person-organization fit: an integrative review of its conceptualiza-
tions, measurement, and implications. PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 49, 1-49.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
148 PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY
Locke EA (1976). The nature and causes of job satisfaction. In Dunnette MD (Ed.),
Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1297-1350). Chicago:
Rand McNally.
Lott AJ, Lott BE. (1965). Group cohesiveness as interpersonal attraction: A review of
relationships with antecedent and consequent variables. Psychological Bulletin, 64,
259-309.
Meglino BM, Ravlin EC, Adk.ins CL. (1989). A work values approach to corporate cul-
ture: A field test of the value congruence process and its relationship to individual
outcomes. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 74, 424-432.
Meglino BM, Ravlin EC, Adk.ins CL. (1992). The measurement of work value congruence:
A field study comparison. Journal of Management, 18, 3~3.
Mowday RT, Steers RM, Porter LW (1979). The measurement of organizational commit-
ment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 14,224-247.
Moskowitz DS, CoteS. (1995). Do interpersonal traits predict affect? A comparison of
three models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 915-924.
O'Reilly CA, Chatman J, Caldwell DF. (1991). People and organizational culture: A pro-
file comparison approach to assessing person-organization fit. Academy of Man-
agement Journal, 34, 487-516.
Quinn RE. (1988). Beyond rational management. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Russell DW, Altmaeir E, Van Velzen D. (1987). Job-related stress, social support, and
burnout among classroom teachers. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 72, 269-274.
Saffold GS. (1988). Culture traits, strength, and organizational performance: Moving
beyond "strong" culture. Academy of Management Review, 13, 546-558.
Saks AM, Ashforth BE. (1997). A longitudinal investigation of the relationships between
job information sources, applicant perceptions of fit, and work outcomes. PERSON·
NELPSYCHOLOGY, 50, 395-426.
Salancik GR, Pfeffer J. (1978). A social information processing approach to job attitudes
and task design. Administrative Science Quarterly, 23, 224-253.
Schein EH. (1985). Organizational culture and leadership. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Schein EH. (1990). Organizational culture. American Psychologist, 45, 109-119.
Schmidt FL, Hunter JE. (1989). Interrater reliability coefficients cannot be computed
when only one stimulus is rated. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 74, 368-370.
Schneider B. (1987). The people make the place. PERSONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 40,437-453.
Schneider B, Goldstein HW, Smith DB. (1995). The ASA framework: An update. PER-
SONNEL PSYCHOLOGY, 48,747-773.
Schneider B, Smith DB, Thylor S, Fleenor J. (1998). Personality and organizations: A test
of the homogeneity of personality hypothesis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83,
462-470.
Schuler RS. (1980). Definition and conceptualization of stress in organizations. Organiza-
tional Behavior and Human Performance, 25, 184-215.
Shaikh T, Kanekar S. (1994). Attitudinal similarity and affiliation need as determinants of
interpersonal attraction. Journal of Social Psychology, 134, 257-259.
Singh R, Thn LSC. (1992). Attitudes and attraction: A test of the similarity-attraction and
dissimilarity-repulsion hypotheses. British Journal of Social Psychology, 54, 798-
810.
Smith M. (1994). A theory of the validity of predictors in selection. Journal ofOccupational
and Organizational Psychology, 67, 13-31.
Starbuck WH, Mezias JM. (1996). Opening Pandora's box: studying the accuracy of
managers' perceptions. Journal of Organizational behavior, 17, 99-117.
Tan DTY, Singh R. (1995). Attitudes and attraction: A developmental study of the
similarity-attraction and dissimilarity-repulsion hypotheses. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 21, 975-986.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ANNELIES E.M. VAN VIANEN 149
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.