Running Head: Media & Instructional Technology On Student Learning 1
Running Head: Media & Instructional Technology On Student Learning 1
Nichole Williams
Abstract
In this article, I examine the positions taken by both Richard Clark and Robert Kozma. I
summarize their position on media versus method and give a brief synopsis on where society was
in the technological continuum during their respective time periods. I discuss how John
Sweller’s cognitive load theory and Richard Mayer’s cognitive theory of multimedia learning
might influence the interpretation of their findings. Finally, I use current classroom action
research as well as Sweller’s and Mayer’s cognition and learning research to establish my
There has been on ongoing debate as to whether media has any influence on learning.
The two researchers that have been at the heart of this debate are Richard Clark and Robert
Kozma. Both theorist make fundamental claims regarding the impact media and instructional
methods have on learning. After eleven years of technological advancements in society, Clark’s
generalization, “…that there are no learning benefits to be gained from employing any specific
medium to deliver instruction” (1983, p. 445) faced strong rebuttal. Kozma sought to prove that
there are, “…conditions under which media will influence learning” (1994). While both
researchers support their claims with classroom action research, new investigations on the
understand the context and developments that were present during the time his article was
written. Technology, as we define it today, would be completely foreign to someone living in the
50s through part of the 80s. The World Wide Web did not exist and “media” was the buzz word
and it included any form of communicating information (newspapers, film, radio, television,
pictures, computers/gaming consoles, etc). Throughout the article, Clark repeatedly references
his research from the 1980s as well as intellectuals whose findings were based on evidence from
the50s, 60s, and 70s. To put things into perspective it may be helpful to note where we were in
terms of media and technology at the time of these conclusions. According to the timeline
the first typewriter and cassette tape were invented in 1961 and 1962. Atari was founded in 1972
and two years later Xerox introduced the PARC Alto. In 1981, IBM introduced the 5150, its first
personal computer. Apple soon followed, launching their Macintosh during the 1984 Super
Bowl. Although things were seemingly on the verge of a new era, by the mid-1980s it had taken
a turn for the worst. In 1983, a reporter for the Boston Phoenix summed it up by noting that
people were realizing that balancing checkbooks and keeping track of recipes were easier with a
calculator and index cards (Mitchell, 1983). By 1987, only 15% of American families owned a
computer, leading to Dan Gutman’s claim that the revolution was “in shambles”. Furthermore,
he explained that, “Software for one computer won’t run on any others. Disks crash.
Documentation is terrible. Software doesn’t load” (1987, p. 52). It is from this era, and earlier,
With public views predicting the demise of computers and programs coupled with
inadequate studies, it is understandable why Clark would concluded that, “Five decades of
research suggest that there are no learning benefits to be gained from employing different media
in instruction” (1994, p. 450). It was vital for Clark to distinguish the difference between media
and methods in regards to research studies. Clark quickly diffused prior research that confused
the two, citing a review from Lumsdaine (1963) that examined a study that compared different
step size in program instruction via television. The study mistakenly considered the medium,
television, as the contributing factor when, in fact, it was the step size (the method). It is
important to note that, based on examples from that article, anytime Clark referenced “media” he
was discussing either visual (pictures, film, television), audio (radio, tapes, recordings), text
modules). In addition, Clark delineates a clear difference between a medium and “its attributes,”
MEDIA & INSTRUCTIONAL TECHNOLOGY ON STUDENT LEARNING 5
or “symbols” (using the latter two terms interchangeably). In his opinion, if an attribute can be
accomplished by any other medium then, in essence, a specific medium cannot be credited with
that attribute in regards to research studies. He uses the symbolic animated arrows and zooming
as examples (1994, p. 452). Since the same elements could be accomplished using different
media, Clark argues that media can only be a “vehicle” by which instruction may be delivered
(1994). This theory lends itself to his argument that “…it is the method of instruction that leads
more directly and powerfully to learning” (1994, p. 449). In addition, any study that did show
gains, Clark downplayed as results of uncontrolled effects such as novelty, different content, or
Salvador as support for his claim (1994, p. 445). In this study, education was drastically
reformed with the introduction of the television in underserved rural schools. However, there
Kozma’s article was written over ten years after Clark’s findings. Between the time the
two publications were released, great technological advancements had been made. Societal
interactions, in terms of technology, were beginning to take shape and form what we would later
call, 21st Century America. Cell phones and texting were common. The World Wide Web was in
full swing and Yahoo search engine was on the verge of being introduced to the public. The
educational system was beginning to make the connections that businesses had realized for
years; it was possible to transfer greater information at quicker speeds, to anyone with computer
access. Kozma recognized the need to not only revisit Clark’s conclusion but to take it a step
further and indicate that if there are no known connections between media and learning, we
should “forge” some, or else risk sitting by idly, watching the rest of the world advance (1994, p.
MEDIA & INSTRUCTIONAL TECHNOLOGY ON STUDENT LEARNING 6
2). It is important to note that Kozma also redefines some vital supporting concepts and terms
from Clark’s article due to the new advancements and understandings in technology (in my
opinion, these alone are grounds for considering this debate futile). For example, Clark uses the
“attributes of media,” theory to help substantiate his position. However, Kozma asserts that there
is a difference between a medium’s capabilities and the variability of its use (1994). He clarifies
that a medium’s attributes “…are its capabilities” and it is the cluster of these, or its profile, that
distinguishes one medium from another (1994, p.13). He goes on to explain that Clark’s
assertion of isolating media from other factors, such as method, is unnecessary since both are
relevant to instructional design. Rather, he suggested that media capabilities combined with
Two studies Kozma used to support his claims were ThinkerTools program (White, 1993)
and Jasper Woodbury Series (Van Haneghan, Barron, Williams, Vye, & Bransford, 1992). With
understand and impart Newton’s actual scientific formulas. The Jasper project used videodisk to
portray scenarios needed for solving real-world problems. The videos were able to compensate
for student limits such as the lack of prior knowledge or mental limits in visualizing complex
settings. Both cases resulted in increased student scores when compared to others using more
textual media.
John Sweller is credited with developing the cognitive load theory (1988). Cognitive
load is the amount of effort being put forth by the working memory. Sweller states that we have
MEDIA & INSTRUCTIONAL TECHNOLOGY ON STUDENT LEARNING 7
a limited capacity in this area. He also emphasis the need for chunking or “schemas” in long-
term memory, and that “schema acquisition and automation” are the real goals (1994, p. 301).
Building upon his research is Richard Mayer’s Theory of Multimedia Learning. While Sweller
emphasized reducing the cognitive load by using more schemas, Mayer’s work suggests that
cognitive load could be reduced by utilizing the sensory memory channels more efficiently.
Incorporating Allan Pavio’s research on dual-coding (1986), Mayer asserts that information is
perceived either visually or auditorily (2014). It is then processed via working memory and
stored in long-term memory using prior knowledge. Although there is a limited capacity for each
channel, the two can work simultaneously so, splitting information between the two channels
could increase the amount of information stored in long-term memory at one time while reducing
the load on working memory. Mayer then developed multimedia principles that helped to reduce
Sweller’s and Mayer’s theories show greater support for Kozma’s claim, that both media
and method should be used conjointly to increase learning. By using a combination of visual and
auditory or verbal and nonverbal methods, the cognitive load is reduced. Although there may be
other contributing factors, this aspect alone would explain the success Kozma discussed in the
It is difficult to see how Clark’s position could be supported by either Sweller or Mayer.
As Kozma noted, Clark employed the outdated “stimulus and response” method (1994), which
does not recognize the multifaceted acclimation of knowledge. I do believe that Clark’s focus
may have seemed more relevant during the early 80s, when media in education was new and not
well defined. However, in trying to find all of the ways that media was useless, he missed the
MEDIA & INSTRUCTIONAL TECHNOLOGY ON STUDENT LEARNING 8
opportunities to see a glimmer of its future possibilities. He wasted time believing media should
be stripped down to a single attribute in order to be studied, rather than constructing a more
Conclusion
My Position
In starting my teacher career in 1999 (when we still used chalkboards and VCRs), I
experienced the transition from basic, black and white resources to more dynamic and interactive
hardware, as well as the excitement caused by the successful implementation of fresh and
innovative technologies. I agree with Kozma’s generalization that media can be used to
influence learning. It doesn’t take a research study to validate the fundamental increases I’ve
seen in student engagement, the complexity of products they produce, and the depth of questions
my students now ask. The standards have remained relatively unchanged, but the tools with
which to teach them have definitely evolved. While this is a broad view, not worthy of any
quantitative study, there has been research that supports such claims. Maya Escueta, Vincent
Quan, Andre Nickow, and Philip Oreopoulos recently completed an evidence-based review on
the effectiveness of technology-based approaches (2017). They used data from two sources,
online programs that provided immediate feedback and a software-based math curriculum. The
online program had an effective size of 0.18 standard deviation, and the software-based program
improved scores by 0.63 standard deviations in seventh grade, and by 0.56 standard deviations in
eighth grade (2017, p. 88). They concluded that “…computer-assisted learning can be quite
While on the surface it appears that both Clark and Kozma seemed to have found
evidence that supported their positions, it is Kozma’s argument that is the most compelling.
Clark’s position has simply become outdated for the complexity of technology today.
Completely isolating media from instructional methods and then stripping them down to a single
attribute in order to solely measure its effects on learning would be like measuring reading
fluency by separating words into individual letters and timing the syllables read. We don’t
perceive words in literature in isolated segments, nor do we interact with media using isolated
attributes. I think Kozma’s sums it up nicely when he declares that, “If media are going to
References
Clark, R. E. (1994). Media will never influence learning. Educational Technology, Research and
Development, 42(2), 21 - 29.
Kozma, R. (1994). Will media influence learning: Reframing the debate. Educational
Technology Research and Development, 42(2), 7-19.
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive
Science, 12(2), 257-285.
Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty, and instructional design. Learning
and Instruction, 4 (4): 295-312.
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Escueta, M., Quan, V., Nickow, A. J., & Oreopoulos, P. (2017). Education technology: An
evidence-based review. (NBER Working Paper No. 23744). Cambridge, MA: National
Bureau of Economic Research.
Gutman, D. (1987). What happened to the computer revolution? Commodore Magazine, 8 (9):
53-56.
Mitchell, P. (1983, September 6). A summer-CES report. The Boston Phoenix, pp. 4.