Clinical Diagnostic Criteria For Dementia Associat
Clinical Diagnostic Criteria For Dementia Associat
discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/227682701
CITATIONS READS
857 1,393
23 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
All in-text references underlined in blue are linked to publications on ResearchGate, Available from: Jennifer G Goldman
letting you access and read them immediately. Retrieved on: 29 September 2016
Movement Disorders
Vol. 22, No. 12, 2007, pp. 1689-1707
© 2007 Movement Disorder Society
Murat Emre, MD,1* Dag Aarsland, MD,2,3 Richard Brown, PhD,4 David J. Burn, MD,5
Charles Duyckaerts, MD,6 Yoshikino Mizuno, MD,7,8 Gerald Anthony Broe, MD,9,10
Jeffrey Cummings, MD,11 Dennis W. Dickson, MD,12 Serge Gauthier, MD,13 Jennifer Goldman, MD,14
Christopher Goetz, MD,14 Amos Korczyn, MD,15 Andrew Lees, MD,16 Richard Levy, MD, PhD,17
Irene Litvan, MD,18 Ian McKeith, MD,19 Warren Olanow, MD,20 Werner Poewe, MD,21
Niall Quinn, MD,22 Christina Sampaio, MD, PhD,23 Eduardo Tolosa, MD,24 and Bruno Dubois, MD25
1
Department of Neurology, Behavioral Neurology and Movement Disorders Unit, Ístanbul Faculty of Medicine, Ístanbul
University, Ístanbul, Turkey
2
Norwegian Centre for Movement Disorders, Stavanger University Hospital, Stavanger, Norway
3
School of Medicine, University of Bergen, Norway
4
King’s College, MRC Centre for Neurodegeneration Research, Institute of Psychiatry, London, United Kingdom
5
Institute for Ageing and Health, Newcastle University, Wolfson Research Centre, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
6
Neuropathology Laboratory, La Salpetriere Hospital, Paris VI Pierre et Marie Curie University, Inserm U679, Paris, France
7
Department of Neurology, Juntendo University School of Medicine, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo, Japan
8
Research Institute for Diseases of Old Ages, Juntendo University School of Medicine, Bunkyo-ku, Tokyo, Japan
9
Ageing Research Centre, Prince of Wales Hospital, Randwick, New South Wales, Australia
10
Faculty of Medicine, Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute, University of New South Wales, Randwick,
New South Wales, Australia
11
Department of Neurology, David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Los Angeles, California, USA
12
Department of Neuroscience, Mayo Clinic College of Medicine, Jacksonville, Florida, USA
13
Alzheimer Disease and Related Disorders Unit, McGill Center for Studies in Aging, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
14
Department of Neurological Sciences, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois, USA
15
Department of Neurology, Tel-Aviv University, Ramat -Aviv, Israel
16
Department of Neurology, The National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London, United Kingdom
17
Federation de Neurologie, INSERM U.610, AP-HP, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpetriere, Paris, France
18
Movement Disorder Program, Department of Neurology, University of Louisville School of Medicine, Louisville, Kentucky, USA
19
Wolfson Research Centre, Newcastle General Hospital, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
20
Department of Neurology and Department of Neuroscience, Mount Sinai School of Medicine, New York, USA
21
Department of Neurology, Medical University of Innsbruck, Innsbruck, Austria
22
Sobell Department of Motor Neuroscience and Movement Disorders, Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London, United Kingdom
23
Laboratorio de Farmacologia Clinica e Terapeutica, Faculdade de Medicina de Lisboa e Instituto de Medicina Molecular,
Lisboa, Portugal
24
Parkinson’s Disease and Movement Disorders Unit, Neurology Service, Institut Clinic de Neurociences, Hospital Clinic de
Barcelona, IDIBAPS, Universitat de Barcelona, Spain
25
INSERM U610, Pavillon Claude Bernard, Hôpital de la Salpêtrière, Paris, France
This article is part of the journal’s CME program. The CME form Neurology, İstanbul Faculty of Medicine, İstanbul University, 34390
can be found on page 1837 and is available online at http://www.move- Çapa İstanbul, Turkey. E-mail: [email protected]
mentdisorders.org/education/activities.html Received 19 February 2007; Accepted 10 March 2007
*Correspondence to: Dr. Murat Emre, Professor of Neurology, Be- Published online 31 May 2007 in Wiley InterScience (www.
havioral Neurology and Movement Disorders Unit, Department of interscience.wiley.com). DOI: 10.1002/mds.21507
1689
1690 M. EMRE ET AL.
Abstract: Dementia has been increasingly more recognized to point-prevelance is close to 30%, older age and akinetic-rigid
be a common feature in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD), form are associated with higher risk. PD-D is characterized by
especially in old age. Specific criteria for the clinical diagnosis impairment in attention, memory, executive and visuo-spatial
of dementia associated with PD (PD-D), however, have been functions, behavioral symptoms such as affective changes, hal-
lacking. A Task Force, organized by the Movement Disorder lucinations, and apathy are frequent. There are no specific
Study, was charged with the development of clinical diagnostic ancillary investigations for the diagnosis; the main pathological
criteria for PD-D. The Task Force members were assigned to correlate is Lewy body-type degeneration in cerebral cortex
sub-committees and performed a systematic review of the and limbic structures. Based on the characteristic features as-
literature, based on pre-defined selection criteria, in order to sociated with this condition, clinical diagnostic criteria for
identify the epidemiological, clinical, auxillary, and patholog- probable and possible PD-D are proposed. © 2007 Movement
ical features of PD-D. Clinical diagnostic criteria were then Disorder Society
developed based on these findings and group consensus. The Key words: Parkinson’s disease; dementia; Parkinson’s dis-
incidence of dementia in PD is increased up to six times, ease dementia; diagnosis; diagnostic criteria.
Historically, Parkinson’s disease (PD) has been con- tive and neuropsychiatric features, motor and other clin-
sidered largely as a motor disorder. It has been increas- ical features, ancillary examinations, and clinico-patho-
ingly recognized, however, that PD is frequently associ- logical correlations, based on pre-defined criteria for
ated with cognitive deficits, and that dementia eventually selection of publications to be included (Appendix). To
develops in a substantial number of patients. Currently exclude cases with Dementia with Lewy Bodies (DLB),
there are no specific and operationalized criteria to diag- those papers which used the 1-year rule with regard to
nose dementia associated with PD (PD-D). The existing the onset of dementia were grouped together and consti-
criteria in DSM IV subsume PD-D under “dementia due tuted the primary source, whereas those which did not
to other medical conditions”, and the section specifically use the 1-year rule or did not indicate if this rule was
devoted to PD-D is rather descriptive. As potential ther- applied were considered as secondary. Findings and typ-
apeutic approaches for PD-D become available and clin- ical features emerging from these reviews, described in
ical trials are underway, it is important to establish spe- detail later, were tabulated (Table 1) and used to describe
cific and operationalized diagnostic criteria for PD-D in consensus-based definitions of probable and possible
order to include homogenous groups of patients in these PD-D (Table 2).
trials. This would also help to prospectively determine
the natural history and other characteristics of this con- Relation of PD-D to DLB
dition in a more systematic way and would allow more DLB and PD-D share many pathological and clinical
accurate clinico-pathological correlation studies. features, and probably represent two clinical entities on a
The Movement Disorder Society (MDS) recruited a spectrum of Lewy body disease.1 While the time-course
Task Force to define the clinical diagnostic criteria for
of the symptoms and presenting features primarily dif-
PD-D. This article summarizes the process and conclu-
ferentiate these disorders, there are also pathological
sions of this effort.
differences such as different patterns of Lewy body dis-
METHODS tribution. Indeed, there is no rational clinical or patho-
logical basis to dictate a definite time interval between
The Task Force Composition and Procedure development of motor symptoms versus onset of demen-
The MDS Task Force comprised 23 members with tia in differentiating PD-D from DLB. Nevertheless,
different areas of expertise in the field. They were invited based on an empirical approach and to avoid diagnostic
to join the Task Force because of their interest and track confusion in clinical practice, we recommend that a
records in diverse aspects of the disease including epi- diagnosis of PD-D should be made when dementia de-
demiology, clinical aspects, ancillary methods, and pa- velops within the context of established PD, whereas a
thology, and representing various disciplines to include diagnosis of DLB is appropriate when the diagnosis of
neurology, geriatric psychiatry, neuropscyhology, and dementia precedes or coincides within 1 year of the
pathology as well as different geographical regions with development of motor symptoms. This recommendation
differing medical traditions. The group was divided into is operationally consistent with that described in the
five sub-committees, each in charge of systematically Third Report of the DLB Consortium2: “DLB should be
reviewing the literature related to epidemiology, cogni- diagnosed when dementia occurs before or concurrently
with parkinsonism, and PD-D should be used to describe and found that 3 to 4% of dementia cases in the general
dementia that occurs in the context of well-established population were due to PD-D. The estimated prevalence
PD. The appropriate term will depend upon the clinical of PD-D in the general population aged 65 and over was
situation and generic terms such as Lewy Body disease found to be 0.2 to 0.5%.5 In more recent studies, preva-
are often helpful. In research studies in which distinction lence rates of dementia in PD patients of 48%,6 23%,7
is made between DLB and PD-D, the 1-year rule be- and 22%8 were reported. The Rotterdam study, based on
tween the onset of dementia and parkinsonism for DLB a door-to-door survey, found a lower prevalence rate,
should be used.” though this pattern is typical of such methodology which
usually includes a larger proportion of mildly affected
EPIDEMIOLOGY cases, when compared with clinic-based studies.8
The epidemiology of PD-D has usually been studied A few studies have focused on the prevalence of
among hospital-based populations. Few door-to-door dementia in newly diagnosed PD patients. In the Sydney
studies have reported the prevalence or, in particular, study, prevalence of dementia was 16%.9 Recent popu-
incidence, of PD-D in the general population, and the lation-based studies have reported rates of 8%10 and
methodology for identifying PD-D patients has varied. 17%11 of “marked cognitive impairment” defined as a
Most studies are cross-sectional, providing an estimate of Mini-Mental State Examination score below 24.
the proportion of PD patients who are demented (point
prevalence). For several reasons, including the higher Incidence
mortality rate in PD-D versus non-demented PD sub-
In community based studies, incidence rates of 95.3,12
jects,3 more accurate information can be drawn from the
107.1,13 and 112.5,14 in 1,000 patient-years were re-
few longitudinal studies. These provide information on
ported, indicating that ⬃10% of a PD population will
the incidence of PD-D, while studies including a control
develop dementia per year. The reported relative risk for
group can also deduce the relative risk of developing
developing dementia in PD compared to non-PD subjects
dementia in PD versus non-PD subjects. In addition,
ranges from 1.7,14 to 4.7,8 5.1,6 and 5.9.12 There are
period prevalence, i.e. the proportion of demented pa-
several reasons for this variation, such as case selection
tients in a PD cohort during a specified time, by com-
procedures and the use of different estimates of risk. In
bining prevalence, incidence and mortality rates, pro-
one of the very few incidence studies of dementia in
vides important information concerning the total
general including subjects with PD (diagnosis based on
proportion of PD patients who will eventually develop
self-report), the odds ratio for dementia in PD was 3.5.15
dementia.
The mean duration from onset of PD to development onset of motor symptoms. This could be a methodolog-
of dementia was reported to be ⬃10 years,18,19 there are, ical bias however, since patients with early dementia
however, wide variations. Some patients develop cogni- may be excluded due to subjects not fulfilling PD
tive impairment and subsequent dementia within few criteria.
years after onset of PD, whereas others may develop The onset is insidious. In one prospective study, the
dementia 20 or more years after disease onset.20 The type mean annual decline on the MMSE during 4 years was 1
and extent of pathology may vary in patients with early point in non-demented and 2.3 points in the PD-D group,
versus late onset dementia.21 Age is a crucial factor, and the latter figure being similar to the decline observed in
dementia is infrequent in patients with young onset and patients with AD.26 A similar rate of decline was re-
who are chronologically still young at the time of assess- ported in another longitudinal study, mean decline in
ment, despite very long disease duration.22 MMSE over 2 years was 4.5, and comparable to that seen
in patients with DLB, with a mean decline of 3.9
Risk Factors points.27
Many demographic and clinical features have been
assessed as potential risk factors, with inconsistent find- Cognitive Features
ings. The most consistent risk factors in longitudinal
The majority of studies on cognitive function in pa-
studies were higher age, more severe parkinsonism, in
tients with PD have reported on non-demented patients
particular rigidity, postural instability and gait distur-
or on non-selected groups including those with dementia.
bance, and mild cognitive impairment at baseline. Age
A wide variety of cognitive impairments have been re-
and severity of motor symptoms seem to have a com-
ported, even early in the course of the disease, including
bined rather than additive effect on the risk of demen-
memory, visuospatial function, and executive func-
tia.23 Inconsistent findings have been reported for old age
tion.28 –33 Although the latter deficits are often described
at onset, male gender, education, depression, visual hal-
as dominating the cognitive profile, there is some evi-
lucinations, and other clinical features. A recent study
dence of heterogeneity, with some patients expressing an
found that age at onset does not influence the risk for
dementia after adjusting for age.20 A significant relation- amnestic profile, while others present with a predomi-
ship between drug use and risk for dementia has not been nantly dysexecutive or mixed profile.32 There is some
convincingly demonstrated. The effect of genetic dispo- indication from prospective studies that executive defi-
sition as a risk factor is discussed in “Genetic Aspects of cits may be the more important predictors of subsequent
PD-D.” decline.34,35 However, the relationship between initial
deficits and subsequent profile of dementia has not been
Conclusions From Epidemiological Studies clearly established. Therefore, this review was restricted
to studies that report explicitly on cognitive function in
The point prevelance of dementia in PD is close to
groups of PD patients with dementia. Most studies in-
30% and the incidence rate is increased 4 to 6 times as
compared to controls. The cumulative prevalence has cluded patients with mild or moderate dementia, with
been reported to range between 48 and 78% after 15 and little published evidence on the cognitive manifestations
8 years of follow-up, respectively. The main risk vari- of more severe PD-D. Unless clearly stated, the evidence
ables are higher age, more severe parkinsonism, in par- reviewed relates only to the profile of cognitive impair-
ticular rigidity, postural instability and gait disturbance, ment in mild-moderate PD-D. Comparative studies con-
and mild cognitive impairment at baseline. sidered in this review successfully matched dementia
groups on the basis of an extended mental status exam-
THE COGNITIVE AND NEUROPSYCHIATRIC ination (e.g. Dementia Rating Scale [DRS]), or clinical
PROFILE OF PATIENTS WITH PD-D rating of dementia severity.
Most of this review focuses on cognition defined in
Onset and Time Course terms of primary domains. However, some caution must
Mild cognitive impairment was found already at the be exercised, as terms such as “attention” and “executive
time of diagnosis in some patients in an incident cohort function” has been used interchangeably in different
of PD.10 There is some evidence that dementia is merely studies. Many neuropsychological tests tap a number of
a progression of this early cognitive impairment,24 while different domains, and patients may perform poorly on a
other evidence suggests a different profile in PD-D and given test for different reasons. Unfortunately, most pub-
non-demented PD patients.25 Epidemiological studies lished studies have not employed tests that permit the
suggest that dementia usually develops years after the underlying cognitive processes to be defined in detail.
Conclusions on the Profile of Cognitive Impairment PD-D and DLB contrasts with relatively low rates re-
Impaired cognitive domains in PD-D include atten- ported in mild-moderate AD (4 – 8% on NPI).67,69,70 The
tion, memory, visuo-spatial, constructional, and execu- significance of hallucinations as marker of Lewy-body
tive functions. There are some phenomenological differ- pathology has been confirmed by both retrospective and
ences between PD-D and AD, particularly in executive prospective post-mortem studies.68,71,72
functions, so that a “subcortical” or “dysexecutive” pat- The phenomonology of hallucinations in PD-D and
tern predominates in PD-D, although there are overlaps. DLB is very similar. Visual hallucinations occur twice as
These differences are most apparent in the early and frequently as auditory ones, the majority being complex,
middle stages of dementia and difficult to identify in the formed hallucinations.62,63,73 Most common are anony-
later stages. Many studies involving a wide range of tests mous people, but they may also be family members,
have failed to distinguish between patients with PD-D body parts, animals, or machines. They tend to be in
and AD with certainty. The differentiation between color, static and centrally located, and occur with similar
PD-D and DLB in terms of cognitive profile is even less frequency and severity in PD-D and DLB.62,73
evident. Many of the tests used may be insufficiently Delusions are less common than hallucinations in
sensitive to disentangle alternative mechanisms under- PD-D, although the two symptoms often coexist. While
pinning impaired performance, such as role of impaired occurring in ⬃17% of patients with PD overall,60,61 their
attention in poor memory function as opposed to primary prevalence in PD-D is 25 to 30%,63– 65 somewhat lower
deficits in memory. than rates seen in AD,64,67,70,74 and particularly in DLB
Neuropsychological assessment serves an important where rates of 57 to 78% have been reported.63,67,74
role in providing objective evidence of cognitive impair- Another study suggested that when delusions occur they
ment to support the clinical diagnosis of dementia in PD. do so with similar frequency over time and severity in
However, its role in differential diagnosis is not conclu- DLB and PD-D,73 and with similar phenomenology.
sive, at least with the standard tests typically employed in Paranoid delusions and “phantom boarder” are among
published studies. While indicative profiles can be de- the most common content in both disorders.63 Mis-iden-
scribed on the basis of sets of tests, the evidence is not tification syndromes appear to be particularly prevalent
yet sufficiently robust to use these as the sole basis of in DLB occurring in up to 40% of patients, compared to
diagnosis. 10% in AD.68 Whether mis-identification delusions are
also characteristic of PD-D is currently unclear. The
Behavioral and Neuropsychiatric Symptoms prevalence of Capgras delusions is reported to be 10% in
DLB, when compared with no cases in patients with
While formal diagnostic criteria can be applied to PD-D.63
elicit symptoms such as depression or anxiety, the ma-
jority of clinical features are identified by informant Mood Disturbance.
ratings, the most popular one in the assessment of PD-D In a community-based sample applying formal diag-
being the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI).58 While nostic criteria, the rate of major depression in PD-D has
there is evidence that symptoms often occur in clusters,59 been reported as 13%, compared to 9% for non-de-
the organization of this review is based on individual mented patients, and 19% for patients with DLB.63 Both
symptoms. The review focused on those symptoms that severity of depressed mood and prevalence of major
occur in a significant proportion of PD-D patients, depression may be higher in PD-D than in AD.43 How-
namely, hallucinations, delusions, mood disturbance, and ever, dysphoric mood as assessed by the NPI occurs with
apathy. the same frequency in PD-D and AD (40 –58%),64 pos-
sibly higher than seen in patients with DLB.65,74
Hallucinations and Delusions. Anxious mood occurs at a similar frequency (30 –
Hallucinations have been reported as common in both 49%) to depressed mood; the two disturbances are fre-
population-based studies of PD (25%)60,61 and in clinic quently co-morbid,75 or occur in the same symptom
samples (40%)62 assessed by NPI, with a substantially cluster.59,65 The prevalence of anxious mood in PD-D,
higher prevalence in PD-D (45– 65%).62– 65 When found DLB, and AD appears similar to that of depressed
in non-demented patients, hallucinations are a major mood.64,68,70,74,76 Irritable mood and problems with anger
predictor of subsequent dementia,18 and nursing home and aggression, common in AD, are not prominent clin-
placement.66 In DLB, hallucinations are even more com- ical features of PD-D, while the frequency in DLB is
mon than in PD-D, with figures in the range of 60 to similar to that seen in AD.76 Irritability has a prevalence
80%.63,67,68 The high prevalence of hallucinations in of less than 10% in non-demented patients with PD,61
with only a slightly higher rate in PD-D (14%).64 and study patients with older age/high severity had a relative
tends to be infrequent even in patients who show signif- risk of incident dementia of 9.7 when compared with the
icant problems in other domains.59 younger age/low severity group.3 Tremor dominance at
Elevated mood is rare in PD-D, being reported as presentation has been associated with relative preserva-
either absent,43 or occurring in only 2% of the popula- tion of mental status in some, but not all studies.78,79
tion.64 A similar picture is reported in DLB,74 with a Other neurological features at baseline associated with
somewhat higher rate in AD, although it remains rela- increased risk of dementia include symmetric EPS, sub-
tively rare.70,74 optimal response to levodopa (L-dopa) and dystonic
dyskinesias.
Apathy. A number of studies considered putative dopaminer-
Apathy is often regarded as a hallmark feature of gic-responsive and non-responsive features and their re-
frontotemporal dementia67 and progressive supranuclear lationship with PD-D. Non-L-dopa responsive features
palsy,61 where frequencies of 80% or more have been are classically considered to be axial disturbance, includ-
reported. Similar rates are also reported in DLB,67 with ing gait, posture and balance, as well as facial masking
increasing severity with worsening dementia.77 How- and speech disturbance. These features are predictive of
ever, apathy is also a significant problem in AD, with incident dementia,18,80,81 while the “postural instability
frequencies of 50% or more,64,70,74 increasing with cog- gait disorder”(PIGD) phenotype is also over-represented
nitive deterioration.70 In PD-D, lower figures had been in prevalent PD-D.82 In a recent study, patients with
reported with a frequency of 23 to 24%,64 although a tremor-dominant subtype at baseline did not become
recent study analyzing a large sample of mild-moderate demented until they had a transition to PIGD subtype,
PD-D patients reported a higher figure (54%),65 com- and dementia did not occur among patients with persis-
pared to 17% in non-demented patients.61 tent tremor-dominant subtype.83
a retrospective clinicopathological study (73.3% of pa- there were no significant differences between DLB and
tients with documented falls), “cognitive dysfunction” as PD-D subjects.99
a late clinical feature was significantly more common in
fallers (71.7%) versus non-fallers (49.5%).91 L-dopa Responsiveness
sleep quality was poorer in PD-D, PD, and DLB patients, While fewer dyskinesias were reported in the de-
when compared with AD and normal controls. mented PD patients in a cross-sectional study,78 a longi-
tudinal study found greater mental deterioration in those
Autonomic Features patients exhibiting dystonic L-dopa-induced dyskinesias
A higher frequency of symptomatic orthostasis was at baseline.108 These data are insufficient to infer differ-
reported in association with cognitive impairment in PD ences in the occurrence of L-dopa induced motor com-
in one study that used cluster analysis.84 A high fre- plications in PD-D versus PD.
quency of carotid sinus syndrome and orthostatic hypo-
tension has been reported in DLB as compared to pa- Neuroleptic Sensitivity
tients with AD.98 There was a reduction in heart rate While placebo-controlled studies have excluded pa-
variability in all frequency bands in PD patients both tients with PD-D, open label studies with clozapine in
with and without dementia in one study, and notably PD psychosis have included PD-D subjects and found
that the drug was similarly well tolerated in demented Brain Perfusion and Cerebral Blood Flow Assessed
and non-demented patients, sedation and hypotension by SPECT and PET
being the main side-effects.110 One study reported As compared to healthy controls, PD-D patients ex-
greater risk for low-dose quetiapine (mean dose 50 mg/ hibit brain hypoperfusion or decreased CBF in several
day) to induce motor worsening in PD-D compared to areas of association cortex, in particular in the temporal,
PD without dementia.111Severe neuroleptic sensitivity lateral parietal, precuneus, posterior cingulate, and oc-
has been reported in up to 40% of PD-D patients exposed cipital regions,121–130 whereas in non-demented patients
to neuroleptic drugs.112 there are either no changes128,129 or decreased CBF is
limited to the frontal lobes.121,123 However, extensive
cortical hypoperfusion in non-demented PD patients was
Conclusions on Motor, Autonomic, and Other
reported in two studies, including the parietal and tem-
Features poral cortices,125,127 suggesting that, although temporal-
A PIGD phenotype is more frequent in PD-D, and also parietal-occipital hypoperfusion is usually associated
constitutes a risk factor for incident dementia. Patients with PD-D, it should be used cautiously to distinguish
with PD-D have a more rapid motor decline, and falls are PD-D from non-demented patients.
more frequent. RBD and EDS occur both in demented No difference in the topographical pattern of changes
and non-demented PD patients, but may be more fre- was observed in PD-D as compared to patients with
quent in association with dementia, the presence of RBD DLB.122,123 Over a period of 1-year, reduction in brain
may be useful in differentiating PD-D from AD. The perfusion in PD-D and DLB were comparable. An in-
relative frequency of autonomic symptoms between de- crease in striatal perfusion was interpreted as a compen-
mented and non-demented patients is not known. satory mechanism in response to decreasing dopaminer-
L-dopa responsiveness and L-dopa related dyskinesias gic striatal input.131 The topographical pattern of
may differ between demented and non-demented pa- hypoperfusion in PD-D was similar to that of AD, except
tients, but cannot be relied upon for predictive or diag- that it was more pronounced in AD.129
nostic purposes. Glucose Metabolism
A greater decrease of glucose metabolism was found
ANCILLARY INVESTIGATIONS in the inferior parietal132,133 and occipital133 cortices in
PD-D as compared to PD. A globally similar pattern of
Structural Neuro-Imaging decreased cerebral glucose metabolism, affecting the
Neuroimaging studies examining structural changes in frontal, parietal and parietal association cortices, and
PD-D patients have focused on MRI-based evaluations posterior cingulate area, was observed in PD-D com-
of whole brain113–115; regions of interest were selected in pared to AD,132,134 in PD-D, a greater decrease of glu-
the gray matter113,116 –119 and white matter.120 The re- cose metabolism was observed in the occipital visual
viewed studies indicate that whole brain and regional cortex.134
structural changes are present in PD as compared to AD Studies of Neurotransmitter Abnormalities
and normal controls. Studies suggest greater whole brain
Studies of brain cholinergic transmission, using PET-
atrophy rates in PD-D and a pattern of regional temporal
scan and MP4A binding135 or measuring acetylcholines-
lobe atrophy greatest in AD, followed by PD, then nor-
terase activity,136 demonstrated a more profound cortical
mal controls. Significant differences between non-de-
cholinergic deficit in PD-D as compared to PD, although
mented PD patients and PD-D were reported bilaterally in one study, the difference was not significant.136 Con-
in the occipital lobes. There appears, however, to be an flicting results were obtained regarding the relationship
overlap of the pattern of regional atrophy in PD-D, PD, between dopaminergic loss and PD-D. Whereas Ito et
and DLB, compared to AD and normals. As a result, no al.137 demonsrated that 18F-dopa uptake in PD-D was
consistent pattern of structural changes clearly separates significantly more decreased in the ventral striatum, right
PD-D from non-demented PD or from other comparison caudate nucleus and in the anterior cingulate area as
groups. Sample sizes have not been large enough in the compared to PD, no differences in terms of dopaminergic
cited studies to allow for statistical modeling to control loss and rate of decline between PD and PD-D were
for the increased motor impairment and longer disease found in other studies.138 –139
duration often seen in PD-D compared to non-demented Loss of nigro-striatal dopaminergic terminals can be
PD subjects. visualized using markers of dopamine transporter, such
as FP-CIT SPECT. Significant reductions were found in non-demented patients, so that P300 latencies cannot
FP-CIT binding in the caudate, anterior and posterior differentiate PD-D from PD. Prepulse inhibition of the
putamens in subjects with DLB compared to those with N1/P2 component of auditory evoked potentials was
AD and controls. Transporter loss in DLB was of similar significantly reduced in DLB compared to controls and
magnitude to that seen in PD, the greatest loss in all three AD; the impairment was of intermediate intensity in
areas was seen in patients with PD-D.140 Thus, this PD-D.165
method can be used to differentiate patients with DLB or
PD-D from AD patients with extrapyramidal symptoms, Conclusions on Ancillary Investigations
e.g. because of post-synaptic dysfunction such as neuro- Although there are differences between PD-D and
leptic use, but cannot differentiate from other conditions non-demented PD patients in structural and functional
associated with degeneration of nigro-striatal pathways. imaging as well as electrophysiological studies, none of
these techniques can be recommended for routine diag-
In vivo Biochemical Studies Using Proton MR nostic purposes because of lack of specificity. Similarly
Spectroscopy they do not have sufficient discriminative power to dif-
Lactate/N-acetyl aspartate ratio was found to be in- ferentiate PD-D from DLB and AD.
creased in the occipital lobes in PD, and in particular in
PD-D patients as compared to healthy controls, suggest- GENETIC ASPECTS OF PD-D
ing that the impairment of oxidative energy metabolism The effect of genetic disposition as a risk factor has
is greater in PD-D.141 Similarly, Summerfield et al.142 only been studied systematically for APOE genotypes,
found a decreased ratio of N-acetylaspartate in the oc- and the results have been conflicting. A positive corre-
cipital lobes in PD-D as compared to PD. lation of E2 or E4 alleles with PD-D was reported in
some studies,8,166,167 whereas a negative association with
Cardiac Metaiodobenzylguanidine Scintigraphy E4 was found in a number of others.168 –171 Dementia has
Metaiodobenzylguanidine (MIBG) is a specific been reported in familial forms of PD such as PARK1172–174
marker for noradrenergic transporters. Cardiac MIBG and PARK8.175 Dementia is rare in PARK2, PARK6,
uptake is significantly reduced in PD143–147 as well as in and PARK7. There is some evidence for familial aggre-
DLB,148 because of neurodegeneration of post-gangli- gation of dementia in PD176 and increased frequency of
onic sympathetic nerve fibers.149 Cardiac MIBG is useful dementia in the relatives of PD patients with dementia
for the differentiation of PD from MSA143,147,150 –152 and has been found in some,177 but not all178,179 studies.
from PSP,143 as cardiac MIBG uptake usually remains
CLINICO-PATHOLOGICAL CORRELATIONS
normal in these non-Lewy body parkinsonian disorders.
Thus, although specific studies in PD-D are lacking, Studies describing clinico-pathological correlations in
cardiac MIBG may differentiate LB-related dementias patients with PD-D can be broadly classified into three
from non-LB related types, but cannot differentiate groups: those studies suggesting a correlation of demen-
PD-D and DLB. tia with nigral and brainstem pathology, those suggesting
that limbic and cortical LB-type degeneration is the main
EEG correlate, and those suggesting co-incident Alzheimer
PD-D patients were reported to have distinctly slower type pathology as the main correlate of dementia. These
baseline EEG activity than patients without dementia.153 studies and their main findings are tabulated in Table 3.
Significant decrease in relative alpha power was also On the basis of the recent studies using ␣-synuclein
reported in PD-D compared to PD.154 A positive corre- immunohistochemistry, and assessing several potential
lation between EEG frequency and MMS scores was pathological correlates simultaneously, the main patholog-
reported.155 ical correlate of dementia in PD seems to be the LB-type
degeneration in cerebral cortex and limbic structures. AD-
Event-Related Potentials type pathology frequently co-exists, but it often does not
Prolongation of P300 latency in PD-D was reported in reach a severity to justify pathological diagnosis of AD.
several studies, with no difference between PD-D and
DLB.156 –158 Controversial results have been reported in CONCLUSIONS
P300 latency in non-demented PD patients, prolonged The prevelance of dementia in PD is close to 30%, and
latencies being found in some,159,160,161,162 but not in its incidence is increased by 4 to 6 times over the general
other studies,156,157,163,164 Thus, P300 latency is usually age-appropriate population. Main risk factors are old
prolonged in PD-D, but this may be the case also in age, severity of motor impairment, and already compro-
1979 Hakim and Mathieson (1979) 19/34 PD cases with dementia; ATP in 33/34 cases Dementia in PD due to ATP.
1980 Boller et al. (1980) 29 PD; 9 severe and 7 mild dementia; ATP in 9/9 Dementia in PD due to ATP.
with severe and 3/7 with mild dementia
1982 Jellinger and Grisold (1982) 100 PD; 3 types––a) PD ⫹ATP or AD; b) AD Dementia in PD due to ATP.
with normal SN; c) LB with ATP; dementia
severity correlates with ATP
1984 Gaspar and Gray (1984) 18/32 PD with dementia; SN, LC and nbM Dementia in PD is
pathology; loss and decreased ChAT correlate heterogenous, and includes
with dementia; increased ATP more in demented subcortical pathology and
ATP.
1988 Yoshimura (1988) 37/56 PD with dementia; 3 types––a) pure PD; b) Dementia in PD is
PD with ATP; c) PD with VaD heterogenous. In some cases
dementia in PD due to
subcortical pathology.
1988 Kosaka et al. (1988) 35 LBD cases; 23 with dementia Dementia in PD is
heterogenous. In some cases
dementia in PD due to ATP
and in some subcortical
pathology.
1989 Sudarsky et al. (1989) 4 PD with dementia; with SN and nbM pathology Dementia in PD due to
subcortical pathology.
1989 Rinne et al. (1989) 12 PD; dementia in PD correlated with medial SN Dementia in PD due to
neuronal loss subcortical pathology.
Ubiquitin immunohistochemistry for Lewy bodies (Kuzuhara et al., 1988)
1993 Hughes et al. (1993) 44/100 PD with dementia; 29 AD; 4 diffuse cLBs; Dementia in PD is
6 limbic LB; 6 VaD heterogenous, including
cLBs, ATP, subcortical
pathology, or VaD.
1993 Zweig et al. (1993) 13 PD with dementia without ATP; LC neuronal Dementia in PD due to
loss correlates with dementia subcortical pathology.
1993 Vermersch et al. (1993) 24 PD some with dementia; biochemical evidence Dementia in PD due to ATP.
of tau pathology in frontal cortex (relatively
more than temporal cortex)
1997 Churchyard and Lees (1997) 27 PD with dementia; MMSE correlates with Dementia due to LB (amygdala
density of Lewy neurites in hippocampus and and hippocampus).
amygdala
Alpha-synuclein immunohistochemistry for Lewy bodies (Spillantini et al., 1997)
1998 Wakabayashi et al. (1998) 12/22 PD with dementia; more APOE e4; higher Dementia due to LB (limbic
LB densities and amyloid plaques without NFT and diffuse types).
1998 Mattila et al. (1998) 44 PD; cognitive impairment correlates with cLB Dementia due to LB (especially
and NFT if concurrent AD excluded).
1998 Brown et al. (1998) 12 PD with dementia; 6/12 minimal ATP; 6/12 LB Dementia due to either ATP or
LB or both.
2000 Hurtig et al. (2000) 22 PD with dementia; LB better correlate with Dementia due to LB.
dementia than ATP
2000 Mattila et al. (2000). 45 PD some with dementia; LB in 43/45; ATP in Dementia due to LB.
18/45; APOE e4 cases had more LB; LB
correlates best with dementia
2003 Apaydin et al. (2002) 12 PD with dementia; LB (limbic and diffuse) in Dementia due to LB.
dementia; mild ATP; 2 VaD
2003 Kovari et al. (2003) 22 PD with dementia; CDR scores correlate with Dementia due to LB (limbic
LB and SP, not NFT type).
2003 Colosimo et al. (2003) 276 PD; 67/276 diffuse LB and 50 limbic LB; 11 Dementia due to LB, but some
PD with dementia; 9 PD without dementia had cases with LB are not
diffuse or limbic LB demented.
2004 Bertrand et al., (2004) 10/41 PD with dementia; dementia associated with Dementia due to LB.
hippocampal LB and cLB
2005 Braak et al. (2005) 88 PD some with dementia; MMSE correlates with Dementia due to LB, but some
ATP (NFT stage and SP); PD stage correlates cases with advanced stage are
with PD severity and dementia not demented.
2005 Parkkinen et al. (2005) 32/106 LBD had dementia or parkinsonism; LB Dementia due to factors other
also in normals than LB.
TABLE 3. (Continued)
2005 Pletnikova et al. (2005) 21 PD some with dementia; LB Dementia due to factors other
density correlates with SP; not all than LB.
PD with dementia have LB
2005 Aarsland et al. (2005) 18/22 PD with dementia; LB and Dementia due to LB.
Braak NFT stage correlate with
dementia
PD, Parkinson disease; ATP, Alzheimer type pathology; SN, substantia nigra; LB, Lewy bodies; cLB, cortical Lewy bodies; LC, locus ceruleus;
nbM, basal nucleus of Meynert; VaD, cerebrovascular disease; LBD, Lewy Body Disease; APOE, apolipoprotein E; NFT, neurofibrillary tangles; SP,
senile plaques.
mised cognitive functions at baseline assessment. The posed (Table 2). As there is no gold standard for diag-
clinical features of PD-D include insidious onset and nosis, the sensitivity and specificity of these criteria
slowly progressive course of cognitive impairments in cannot be ascertained. Because the criteria are based on
attention, executive and visuospatial functions as well as a comprehensive review of the existing literature, we
memory, with relatively preserved core language func- propose that they be adopted and tested prospectively to
tions. A dysexecutive profile predominates. Hallucina- assess their clinical utility, sensitivity, and specificity.
tions, delusions, apathy, and mood changes are fre- Such prospective studies may provide the basis for future
quently associated behavioral features (Table 1). revisions.
Dementia in PD is more commonly associated with the
PIGD motor phenotype. Imaging studies demonstrate APPENDIX: INCLUSION CRITERIA FOR
atrophy and hypometabolism, more prominent in the
PUBLICATIONS
temporal and posterior areas. However, there is no single
ancillary investigation which would help to diagnose Search Strategy: Medline search, no time limit, in addition use of
individual patients. Retrospective as well as prospective personal archives and reference list from relevant papers
Evidence Sources: As primary source: Full empirical papers pub-
clinical-pathological studies reveal that dementia in PD lished in English language journals.
best correlates with LB pathology, so that PD-D can be As secondary source: Published abstracts of scientific meetings, or
designated as a LB-associated dementia. English abstracts of non-English journal publications that (1) meet
other criteria below and (2) provide useful new information
The defining feature of PD-D is that dementia devel-
Diagnosis of PD: Study used formal established clinical diagnostic
ops in the context of established PD. Hence, diagnosis of criteria for idiopathic PD or
idiopathic PD before the development of dementia symp- Study did not use formal established criteria, but provided suffi-
toms is the essential first step in the diagnosis. Diagnosis cient details of inclusion/exclusion criteria to suggest patients had
idiopathic PD
of dementia must be based on the presence of deficits in Diagnosis of Dementia: Diagnosis made at the time of the study by
at least two of the four core cognitive domains (attention, a Neurologist, Psychiatrist, or other specialist in dementia and
memory, executive and visuo-spatial functions) as shown Study used formal criteria (DSM/ICD) for dementia following clin-
in clinical and cognitive examination, and be severe ical examination (not retrospective case note examination) or
Study used operational definition based on appropriate age norm-
enough to affect normal functioning. Although there are referenced cognitive impairment measured using an extended mental
some differences in the extent and profile of deficits in status examination such as Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (NB: not
individiual cognitive domains compared to patients with MMSE alone). or
Operational definition based on the presence of significant impair-
AD (more prominent memory impairment in AD, more ment (appropriately defined relative to age-related normative data) in
prominent executive dysfunction in PD-D), these may two or more separate domains of cognitive function.
vary from patient to patient and cannot be used as the Presence of a Control Group: A non-disease control group was not
sole basis of diagnosis. Neuropsychiatric and behavioral required for studies using standard neuropsychological measures with
published age-related normative data, and those used norms to establish
symptoms are frequent, but are not invariable. The pro- the presence/absence of cognitive impairment and its severity.
file of cognitive and behavioral symptoms in PD-D and An age- and education-matched, non-disease control group was
DLB are very similar. There are several suggestive fea- required for studies that used non-standard measures, or where the
measures did not have adequate age-related normative data.
tures which are found more frequently in PD patients A disease control group was not necessary. However, the presence of
with dementia, such as PIGD phenotype and RBD, but a non-neurological patient group was considered to add weight to the
they lack specificity and are hence not included in the interpretation of clinical evidence when factors such as motor impair-
diagnostic criteria. ment or mood may influence neuropsychological test performance.
Minimum Number of Subjects per Group: At least 10 subjects per
Based on the features described above, clinical diag- group.
nostic criteria for probable and possible PD-D are pro- Greater evidential weight was given to studies using larger samples.
Use of Accepted Scales/Test (for Neuropsychological Assess- 15. Yip AG, Brayne C, Matthews FE. MRC Cognitive Function and
ment): Studies using standard version of published neuropsychological Ageing Study. Risk factors for incident dementia in England and
tests or established behavioral scales or Wales: the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and
Studies which used non-standard versions of published tests/scales, Ageing Study. A population-based nested case-control study. Age
or novel non-published tests, provided sufficient detail was given to Ageing 2006;35:154-160.
allow an evaluation of the nature of the tests and any deficit identified 16. Hely MA, Morris JG, Reid WG, Trafficante R. Sydney Multi-
and center Study of Parkinson’s disease: non-L-dopa-responsive prob-
Tests were appropriate for the population and tests were interpreted lems dominate at 15 years. Mov Disord 2005;20:190-199.
appropriately (e.g., inferences about cognitive impairment were not 17. Portin R, Rinne UK. Predictive factors for cognitive deterioration
made on the basis of completion-time alone without appropriate and dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Adv Neurol 1986;45:413-
controls). 416.
Statistics: Details of test performance provided in the forms of 18. Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, Lolk A, Kragh-Sorensen P.
tables or figures that showed both mean (or median) value, and a Prevalence and characteristics of dementia in Parkinson disease:
measure of variability (SD, SEM, IQR, 95%CI) an 8-year prospective study. Arch Neurol 2003;60:387-392.
Studies which provided details of the statistical procedures employed 19. Hughes TA, Ross HF, Musa S, et al. A 10-year study of the
to analyze the data and criteria for statistical significance (␣ ⬍ 0.05). incidence of and factors predicting dementia in Parkinson’s dis-
The nature of the analysis considered to be appropriate for the data ease. Neurology 2000;54:1596-1602.
(e.g., non-parametric tests for small samples, highly skewed distribu- 20. Aarsland D, Kvaløy JT, Andersen K, et al. The effect of age of
tions, ordinal data) onset of PD on risk of dementia. J Neurol (in press).
21. Ballard C, Ziabreva I, Perry R, et al. Differences in neuropatho-
Acknowledgments: We thank Dr. Hasmet Hanagası for his logic characteristics across the Lewy body dementia spectrum.
help with the organization of references. Neurology 2006;67:1910 –1911.
22. Schrag A, Ben-Schlomo Y, Brown R, Marsden CD, Quinn N.
REFERENCES Young-onset Parkinson’s disease revisited-clinical features, nat-
ural history, and mortality. Mov Disord 1998;13:885-894.
1. McKeith IG, Burn D. Spectrum of Parkinson’s disease, Parkin-
23. Levy G, Schupf N, Tang MX, et al. Combined effect of age and
son’s dementia, and Lewy body dementia. In: DeKosky ST,
severity on the risk of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Ann
editor. Neurologic clinics. Philadelphia: WB Sauders; 2000. p
Neurol 2002;51:722-729.
865– 883.
24. Girotti F, Soliveri P, Carella F, et al. Dementia and cognitive
2. McKeith IG, Dickson DW, Lowe J, et al. Diagnosis and manage-
impairment in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychia-
ment of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB
try 1988;51:1498-1502.
Consortium. Neurology 2005;65:1863-1872.
3. Levy G, Tang MX, Louis ED, et al. The association of incident 25. McFadden L, Mohr E, Sampson M, Mendis T, Grimes JD. A
dementia with mortality in PD. Neurology 2002;59:1708-1713. profile analysis of demented and nondemented Parkinson’s dis-
4. Cummings JL. Intellectual impairment in Parkinson’s disease: ease patients. Adv Neurol 1996;69:339-341.
clinical, pathologic, and biochemical correlates. J Geriatr Psychi- 26. Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, et al. The rate of cognitive
atry Neurol 1988;1:24-36. decline in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2004;61:1906-1911.
5. Aarsland D, Zaccai J, Brayne C. A systematic review of preva- 27. Burn DJ, Rowan EN, Allan LM, Molloy S, O’Brien JT, McKeith
lence studies of dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord IG. Motor subtype and cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease,
2005;20:1255-1263. Parkinson’s disease with dementia, and dementia with Lewy
6. Hobson P, Meara J. Risk and incidence of dementia in a cohort of bodies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006;77:585-589.
older subjects with Parkinson’s disease in the United Kingdom. 28. Canavan AGM, Passingham RE, Marsden CD, Quinn N, Wyke
Mov Disord 2004;19:1043-1049. M, Polkey CE. The performance on learning tasks of patients in
7. Athey RJ, Porter RW, Walker RW. Cognitive assessment of a the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 1989;
representative community population with Parkinson’s disease 27:141-156.
(PD) using the Cambridge Cognitive Assessment-Revised 29. Cooper JA, Sagar HJ, Jordan N, Harvey NS, Sullivan EV. Cog-
(CAMCOG-R). Age Ageing 2005;34:268-273. nitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson’s disease and its
8. de Lau LM, Schipper CM, Hofman A, Koudstaal PJ, Breteler relationship to motor disability. Brain 1991;114:2095-2122.
MM. Prognosis of Parkinson disease: risk of dementia and mor- 30. Cooper JA, Sagar HJ, Sullivan EV. Short-term memory and
tality: the Rotterdam Study. Arch Neurol 2005;62:1265-1269. temporal ordering early Parkinson’s disease: Effects of disease
9. Reid WGJ, Hely MA, Morris JGL, et al. A longitudinal study of chronicity and medication. Neuropsychologia 1993;31:933-949.
Parkinson’s disease: clinical and neuropsychological correlates of 31. Levin BE, Katzen HL. Early cognitive changes and nondement-
dementia. J Clin Neurosci 1996;3:327-333. ing behavioral abnormalities in Parkinson’s disease. In: William
10. Foltynie T, Brayne CE, Robbins TW, Barker RA. The cognitive JW, Anthony EL, editors. Behavioral neurology of movement
ability of an incident cohort of Parkinson’s patients in the UK. disorders. New York: Raven; 1995. p 85–95.
The CamPaIGN study. Brain 2004;127:550-560. 32. Lewis SJG, Foltynie T, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Owen AM,
11. Kang GA, Bronstein JM, MAsterman DL, Redelings M, Crum Barker RA. Heterogeneity of Parkinson’s disease in the early
JA, Ritz B. Clinical characteristics in early Parkinson’s disease in clinical stages using a data driven approach. J Neurol Neurosurg
a central California population-based study. Mov Disord 2005; Psychiatry 2005;76:343-348.
20:1133-1142. 33. Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Summers BA, Polkey CE,
12. Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP, et al. Risk of dementia in Robbins TW. Dopamine-dependent frontostriatal planning defi-
Parkinson’s disease: a community-based, prospective study. Neu- cits in early Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychology 1995;9:126-
rology 2001;56:730-736. 140.
13. Hobson P, Gallacher J, Meara J. Cross-sectional survey of Par- 34. Janvin CC, Aarsland D, Larsen JP. Cognitive predictors of de-
kinson’s disease and parkinsonism in a rural area of the United mentia in Parkinson’s disease: a community-based, 4-year longi-
Kingdom. Mov Disord 2005;20:995-998. tudinal study. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2005;18:149-154.
14. Marder K, Tang MX, Cote L, Stern Y, Mayeux R. The frequency 35. Mahieux F, Fenelon G, Flahault A, Manifacier MJ, Michelet D,
and associated risk factors for dementia in patients with Parkin- Boller F. Neuropsychological prediction of dementia in Parkin-
son’s disease. Arch Neurol 1995;52:695-701. son’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;64:178-183.
36. Aarsland D, Litvan I, Salmon D, Galasko D, Wentzel-Larsen T, 54. Mosimann UP, Mather G, Wesnes KA, O’Brien JT, Burn DJ,
Larsen JP. Performance on the dementia rating scale in Parkin- McKeith IG. Visual perception in Parkinson disease dementia and
son’s disease with dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies: dementia with Lewy bodies. Neurology 2004;63:2091-2096.
comparison with progressive supranuclear palsy and Alzheimer’s 55. Cummings JL, Darkins A, Mendez M, Hill MA, Benson DF.
disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2003;74:1215-1220. Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease: comparison of
37. Cahn-Weiner DA, Grace J, Ott BR, Fernandez HH, Friedman JH. speech and language alterations. Neurology 1988;38:680-684.
Cognitive and behavioral features discriminate between Alzhei- 56. Janvin CC, Larsen JP, Salmon DP, Galasko D, Hugdahl K,
mer’s and Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Aarsland D. Cognitive profiles of individual patients with Par-
Behav Neurol 2002;15:79-87. kinson’s disease and dementia: comparison with dementia with
38. Litvan I, Mohr E, Williams J, Gomez C, Chase TN. Differential Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Mov Disord 2006;21:337-
memory and executive functions in demented patients with Par- 342.
kinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57. Lewis SJG, Foltynie T, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Owen AM,
1991;54:25-29. Barker RA. Heterogeneity of Parkinson’s disease in the early
39. Paolo AM, Troster AI, Glatt SL, Hubble JP, Koller WC. Differ- clinical stages using a data driven approach. J Neurol Neurosurg
entiation of the dementias of Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease Psychiatry 2005;76:343-348.
with the dementia rating scale. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1995; 58. Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, et al. The Neuropsychiatric
8:184-188. inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in de-
40. Noe E, Marder K, Bell KL, Jacobs DM, Manly JJ, Stern Y. mentia. Neurology 1994;44:2308-2314.
Comparison of dementia with Lewy bodies to Alzheimer’s dis- 59. Bronnick K, Aarsland D, Larsen JP. Neuropsychiatric distur-
ease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Mov Disord 2004; bances in Parkinson’s disease clusters in five groups with differ-
19:60-67. ent prevalence of dementia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2005;112:201-
41. Beatty WW, Ryder KA, Gontkovsky ST, Scott JG, McSwan KL, 207.
Bharucha KJ. Analyzing the subcortical dementia syndrome of 60. Aarsland D, Larsen JP, Geok Lim N, et al. Range of neuropsy-
Parkinson’s disease using the RBANS. Arch Clin Neuropsychol chiatric disturbances in patients with Parkinsons’s disease. J Neu-
2003;18:509-520. rol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:492-496.
42. Ballard CG, Aarsland D, McKeith I, et al. Fluctuations in atten- 61. Aarsland D, Litvan I, Larsen JP. Neuropsychiatric symptoms of
tion: PD dementia vs DLB with parkinsonism. Neurology 2002; patients with progressive supranuclear palsy and Parkinson’s
59:1714-1720. disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2001;13:42-49.
43. Starkstein SE, Sabe L, Petracca G, et al. Neuropsychological and 62. Fenelon G, Mahieux F, Huon R, Ziegler M. Hallucinations in
psychiatric differences between Alzheimer’s disease and Parkin- Parkinson’s disease: prevalence, phenomenology and risk factors.
son’s disease with dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry Brain 2000;123:733-745.
1996;61:381-387.
63. Aarsland D, Ballard C, Larsen JP, McKeith I. A comparative
44. Pillon B, Deweer B, Agid Y, Dubois B. Explicit memory in
study of psychiatric symptoms in dementia with Lewy bodies and
Alzheimer’s disease, Huntington’s disease, and Parkinson’s dis-
Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Int J Geriatr
ease. Arch Neurol 1993;50:374-379.
Psychiatry 2001;16:528-536.
45. Kuzis G, Sabe L, Tiberti C, Merello M, Leiguarda R, Starkstein
64. Aarsland D, Cummings JL, Larsen JP. Neuropsychiatric differ-
SE. Explicit and implicit learning in patients with Alzheimer
ences between Parkinson’s disease with dementia and Alzhei-
disease and Parkinson disease with dementia. Neuropsychiatry
mer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2001;16:184-191.
Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1999;12:265-269.
46. Tierney MC, Nores A, Snow WG, Fisher RH, Zorzitto ML, Reid 65. Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Ehrt U, De Deyn PP, Tekin S, Emre M,
DW. Use of the Rey auditory verbal learning test in differentiat- Cummings JL. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in patients with PD
ing normal aging from Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s dementia. and dementia: frequency, profile and associated caregiver stress.
Psychol Assess 1994;6:129-134. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007;78:36-42.
47. Helkala EL, Laulumaa V, Soininen H, Riekkinen PJ. Different 66. Aarsland D, Larsen JP, Tandberg E, Laake K. Predictors of
error pattern of episodic and semantic memory in Alzheimer’s nursing home placement in Parkinson’s disease: a population-
disease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Neuropsycholo- based, prospective study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2000;48:938-942.
gia 1989;27:1241-1248. 67. Hirono N, Mori E, Tanimukai S, et al. Distinctive neurobehav-
48. Higginson CI, Wheelock VL, Carroll KE, Sigvardt KA. Recog- ioral features among neurodegenerative dementias. J Neuropsy-
nition memory in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia: chiatry Clin Neurosci 1999;11:498-503.
evidence inconsistent with the retrieval deficit hypothesis. J Clin 68. Ballard C, Holmes C, McKeith I, et al. Psychiatric morbidity in
Exp Neuropsychol 2005;27:516-528. dementia with Lewy bodies: a prospective clinical and neuro-
49. Whittington CJ, Podd J, Kan MM. Recognition memory impair- pathological comparative study with Alzheimer’s disease. Am J
ment in Parkinson’s disease: power and meta-analyses. Neuro- Psychiatry 1999;156:1039-1045.
psychology 2000;14:233-246. 69. Aarsland D, Cummings JL, Larsen JP. Neuropsychiatric differ-
50. Paolo AM, Axelrod BN, Troster AI, Blackwell KT, Koller WC. ences between Parkinson’s disease with dementia and Alzhei-
Utility of a Wisconsin card sorting test short form in persons with mer’s disease. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2001;16:184-191.
Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 70. Benoit M, Robert PH, Staccini P, et al. One-year longitudinal
1996;18:892-897. evaluation of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease.
51. Emre M, Aarsland D, Albanese A, et al. Rivastigmine for demen- The REAL.FR Study. J Nutr Health Aging 2005;9:95-99.
tia associated with Parkinson’s disease. N Engl J Med 2004;351: 71. Ala TA, Yang K-H, Sung JH, Frey WH. Hallucinations and signs
2509-2518. of parkinsonism help distinguish patients with dementia and
52. Cahn-Weiner DA, Williams K, Grace J, Tremont G, Westervelt cortical Lewy bodies from patients with Alzheimer’s disease at
H, Stern RA. Discrimination of dementia with lewy bodies from presentation: a clinicopathological study. J Neurol Neurosurg
Alzheimer disease and Parkinson disease using the clock drawing Psychiatry 1997;62:16-21.
test. Cogn Behav Neurol 2003;16:85-92. 72. Williams DR, Lees AJ. Visual hallucinations in the diagnosis of
53. Cormack F, Aarsland D, Ballard C, Tovee MJ. Pentagon drawing idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a retrospective autopsy study.
and neuropsychological performance in Dementia with Lewy Lancet Neurol 2005;4:605-610.
bodies, Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease and Parkinson’s 73. Mosimann UP, Rowan EN, Partington CE, et al. Characteristics
disease with dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2004;19:371-377. of visual hallucinations in Parkinson disease dementia and de-
mentia with lewy bodies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006;14:153- 95. Pacchetti C, Manni R, Zangaglia R, et al. Relationship between
160. hallucinations, delusions, and rapid eye movement sleep behavior
74. Hirono N, Mori E, Imamura T, Shimomura T, Hashimoto M. disorder in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2005;20:1439-1448.
Neuropsychiatric features in dementia with lewy bodies and Alz- 96. Olson EJ, Boeve BF, Silber MH. Rapid eye movement sleep
heimer’s disease. Brain Nerve 1998;50:45-49. behaviour disorder: demographic, clinical and laboratory findings
75. Menza MA, Robertson HD, Bonapace AS. Parkinson’s disease in 93 cases. Brain 2000;123:331-339.
and anxiety: comorbidity with depression. Biol Psychiatry 1993; 97. Gjerstad MD, Aarsland D, Larsen JP. Development of daytime
34:465-470. somnolence over time in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 2002;
76. Engelborghs S, Maertens K, Nagels G, et al. Neuropsychiatric 58:1544-1546.
symptoms of dementia: cross-sectional analysis from a prospec- 98. Ballard C, Shaw F, McKeith I, Kenny R. High prevalence of
tive, longitudinal Belgian study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005;20: neurovascular instability in neurodegenerative dementias. Neu-
1028-1037. rology 1998;51:1760-1762.
77. Del Ser T, McKeith I, Anand R, Cicin-Sain A, Ferrara R, Spiegel 99. Kenny RA, Allan LM. Autonomic dysfunction in dementia with
R. Dementia with Lewy bodies: findings from an international Lewy bodies. In: Brien JO’, McKeith I, Ames D, Chiu E, editors.
multicentre study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2000;15:1034-1045. Dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s disease dementia.
78. Elizan TS, Sroka H, Maker H, Smith H, Yahr MD. Dementia in Oxford: Taylor and Francis; 2006. p 107–127.
idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: variables associated with its oc- 100. Bonnet A-M, Loria Y, Saint-Hilaire MH, Lhermitte F, Agid Y.
currence in 203 patients. J Neural Transm 1986;65:285-302. Does long-term aggravation of Parkinson’s disease result from
79. Reid WG, Broe GA, Hely MA, et al. The neuropsychology of de nondopaminergic lesions? Neurology 1987;37:1539-1542.
novo patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: the effects of 101. Ebmeier KP, Calder SA, Crawford JR, Stewart L, Besson JA,
age of onset. Int J Neurosci 1989;48:205-217. Mutch WJ. Clinical features predicting dementia in idiopathic
80. Levy G, Tang M-X, Cote LJ, et al. Motor impairment in PD: Parkinson’s disease: a follow-up study. Neurology 1990;40:1222-
relationship to incident dementia and age. Neurology 2000;55: 1224.
539-544. 102. Giladi N, Treves TA, Paleacu D, et al. Risk factors for dementia,
81. Zetusky WJ, Jankovic J, Pirozzolo FJ. The heterogeneity of depression and psychosis in long-standing Parkinson’s disease.
Parkinson’s disease: clinical and prognostic implications. Neu- J Neural Transm 2000;107:59-71.
rology 1985;35:522-526. 103. Stern Y, Marder K, Tang MX, Mayeux R. Antecedent clinical
82. Burn DJ, Rowan EN, Minnett T, et al. Extrapyramidal features in featatures associated with dementia in Parkinson’s disease. Neu-
Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia and dementia with rology 1993;43:1690-1692.
Lewy bodies: a cross-sectional comparative study. Mov Disord 104. Papapetropoulos S, Ellul J, Polychronopoulos P, Chroni E. A
2003;18:884-889. registry-based, case-control investigation of Parkinson’s disease
83. Alves G, Larsen JP, Emre M, Wentzel-Larsen T, Aarsland D. with and without cognitive impairment. Eur J Neurol 2004;11:
Changes in motor subtype and risk for incident dementia in 347-351.
Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord 2006;21:1123-1130. 105. Ebersbach G, Sojer M, Muller J, et al. Dysequilibrium in idio-
84. Graham JM, Sagar HJ. A data-driven approach to the study of pathic Parkinson disease. The effect of cerebrovascular comor-
heterogeneity in idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: identification of bidity. Nervenarzt 2002;73:162-165.
three distinct subtypes. Mov Disord 1999;14:10-20. 106. Bonelli SB, Ransmayr G, Steffelbauer M, Lukas T, Lampl C,
85. Hely MA, Morris JGL, Reid WGJ, et al. Age at onset: the major Deibl M. L-dopa responsiveness in dementia with Lewy bodies,
determinant of outcome in Parkinson’s disease. Acta Neurol Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Neurology 2004;
Scand 1995;92:455-463. 63:376-378.
86. Louis ED, Tang MX, Cote L, Alfaro B, Mejia H, Marder K. 107. Molloy S, O’Brien JT, McKeith IG, Burn DJ. The motor response
Progression of parkinsonian signs in Parkinson disease. Arch to levodopa in dementia with Lewy bodies: a comparison to
Neurol 1999;56:334-337. Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. J Neurol Neuro-
87. Koller WC, Glatt S, Vetere-Overfield B, Hassanein R. Falls and surg Psychiatry (in press).
Parkinson’s disease. Clin Neuropharmacol 1989;12:98-105. 108. Caparros-Lefebvre D, Pecheux N, Petit V, Duhamel A, Petit H.
88. Wood BH, Bilclough JA, Bowron A, Walker RW. Incidence and Which factors predict cognitive decline in Parkinson’s disease?
prediction of falls in Parkinson’s disease: a prospective multidis- J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1995;58:51-55.
ciplinary study. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2002;72:721-725. 109. Joyce JN, Ryoo HL, Beach TB, et al. Loss of response to
89. Wielinski CL, Erickson-Davis C, Wichmann R, Walde-Douglas levodopa in Parkinson’s disease and co-occurrence with demen-
M, Parashos SA. Falls and injuries resulting from falls among tia: role of D3 and not D2 receptors. Brain Res 2002;955:138-
patients with Parkinson’s disease and other parkinsonian syn- 152.
dromes. Mov Disord 2005;20:410-415. 110. Goetz CG, Koller WC, Poewe W, Rascol O, Sampaio C. Man-
90. Allan LM, Ballard CG, Burn DJ, Kenny RA. Prevalence and agement of Parkinson’s disease: an evidence-based review. Mov
severity of gait disorders in Alzheimer’s and non-Alzheimer’s Disord 2002;17:120-127.
dementias. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005;53:1681-1687. 111. Reading PJ, Luce AK, McKeith IG. Rivastigmine in the treatment
91. Williams DR, Watt HC, Lees AJ. Predictors of falls and fractures of parkinsonian psychosis and cognitive impairment: preliminary
in bradykinetic rigid syndromes: a retrospective study. J Neurol findings from an open trial. Mov Disord 2001;16:1171-1195.
Neurosurg Psychiatry 2006;77:468-473. 112. Aarsland D, Perry R, Larsen J, et al. Neuroleptic sensitivity in
92. Rascol O, Clanet M, Montastruc JL, et al. Abnormal ocular Parkinson’s disease and parkinsonian dementias. J Clin Psychia-
movements in Parkinson’s disease. Evidence for involvement of try 2005;66:633-637.
dopaminergic systems. Brain 1989;112:1193-1214. 113. Almeida OP, Burton EJ, McKeith I, Gholkar A, Burn D, O’Brien
93. Mosimann UP, Müri RM, Burn DJ, Felblinger J, O’Brien JT, JT. MRI study of caudate nucleus volume in Parkinson’s disease
McKeith IG. Saccadic eye movement changes in Parkinson’s with and without dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s
disease dementia and dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain 2005; disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2003;16:57-63.
128:1267-1276. 114. Burton E, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, Williams ED, O’Brien JT.
94. Sinforiani E, Zangaglia R, Manni R, et al. REM sleep behavior Cerebral atrophy in Parkinson’s disease with and without demen-
disorder, hallucinations, and cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s tia: a comparison with Alzheimer’s disease, dementia with Lewy
disease. Mov Disord 2006;21:462-466. bodies and controls. Brain 2004;127:791-800.
115. Burton EJ, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, O’Brien JT. Brain atrophy rates 135. Hilker R, Thomas AV, Klein JC, et al. Dementia in Parkinson
in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia using serial disease: functional imaging of cholinergic and dopaminergic
magnetic resonance imaging. Mov Disord 2005;20:1571-1576. pathways. Neurology 2005;65:1716-1722.
116. Camicioli R, Moore MM, Kinney A, Corbridge E, Glassberg K, 136. Bohnen NI, Kaufer DI, Hendrickson R, et al. Cognitive correlates
Kaye J. Parkinson’s disease is associated with hippocampal atro- of cortical cholinergic denervation in Parkinson’s disease and
phy. Mov Disord 2003;18:784-790. parkinsonian dementia. J Neurol 2006;253:242-247.
117. Junque C, Ramirez-Ruiz B, Tolosa E, et al. Amygdalar and 137. Ito K, Nagano-Saito A, Kato T, et al. Striatal and extrastriatal
hippocampal MRI volumetric reductions in Parkinson’s disease dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease with dementia: a
with dementia. Mov Disord 2005;20:540-544. 6-[18F]fluoro-L-dopa PET study. Brain 2002;125:1358-1365.
118. Summerfield C, Junque C, Tolosa E, et al. Structural brain 138. Rinne JO, Portin R, Ruottinen H, et al. Cognitive impairment and
changes in Parkinson’s disease with dementia: a voxel-based the brain dopaminergic system in Parkinson disease: [18F]fluoro-
morphometry study. Arch Neurol 2005;62:281-285. dopa positron emission tomographic study. Arch Neurol 2000;
119. Tam CWC, Burton EJ, McKeith IG, Burn DJ, O’Brien JT. Tem- 57:470-475.
poral lobe atrophy on MRI in Parkinson’s disease with dementia: 139. Colloby SJ, Williams ED, Burn DJ, Lloyd JJ, McKeith IG,
a comparison with Alzheimer’s disease and dementia with Lewy O’Brien JT. Progression of dopaminergic degeneration in demen-
bodies. Neurology 2005;64:861-865. tia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson’s disease with and without
120. Beyer MK, Aarsland D, Greve OJ, Larsen JP. Visual rating of dementia assessed using 123I-FP-CIT SPECT. Eur J Nucl Med
white matter hyperintensities in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord Mol Imaging 2005;32:1176-1185.
2006;21:223-229. 140. O’Brien JT, Colloby S, Fenwick J, et al. Dopamine transporter
121. Antonini A, De Notaris R, Benti R, De Gaspari D, Pezzoli G. loss visualized with FP-CIT SPECT in the differential diagnosis
Perfusion ECD/SPECT in the characterization of cognitive defi- of dementia with Lewy bodies. Arch Neurol 2004;61:919-925.
cits in Parkinson’s disease. Neurol Sci 2001;22:45-46. 141. Bowen BC, Block RE, Sanchez-Ramos J, et al. Proton MR
122. Firbank MJ, Colloby SJ, Burn DJ, McKeith IG, O’Brien JT. spectroscopy of the brain in 14 patients with Parkinson disease.
Regional cerebral blood flow in Parkinson’s disease with and Am J Neuroradiol 1995;16:61-68.
without dementia. Neuroimage 2003;20:1309-1319. 142. Summerfield C, Gomez-Anson B, Tolosa E, et al. Dementia in
123. Kasama S, Tachibana H, Kawabata K, Yoshikawa H. Cerebral Parkinson disease: a proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy
blood flow in Parkinson’s disease, dementia with Lewy bodies, study. Arch Neurol 2002;59:1415-1420.
and Alzheimer’s disease according to three-dimensional stereo- 143. Yoshita M. Differentiation of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease from
tactic surface projection imaging. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord striatonigral degeneration and progressive supranuclear palsy us-
2005;19:266-275. ing iodine-123 meta-iodobenzylguanidine myocardial scintigra-
124. Kawabata K, Tachibana H, Sugita M. Cerebral blood flow and phy. J Neurol Sci 1998;155:60-67.
dementia in Parkinson’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 144. Satoh A, Serita T, Seto M, et al. Loss of 123I-MIBG uptake by
1991;4:194-203. the heart in Parkinson’s disease: assessment of cardiac sympa-
125. Liu RS, Lin KN, Wang SJ, et al. Cognition and 99Tcm-HMPAO thetic denervation and diagnostic value. J Nucl Med 1999;40:371-
SPECT in Parkinson’s disease. Nucl Med Commun 1992;13:744- 375.
748. 145. Orimo S, Ozawa E, Nakade S, Sugimoto T, Mizusawa H. (123)I-
126. Mito Y, Yoshida K, Yabe I, et al. Brain 3D-SSP SPECT analysis metaiodobenzylguanidine myocardial scintigraphy in Parkinson’s
in dementia with Lewy bodies, Parkinson’s disease with and disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:189-194.
without dementia, and Alzheimer’s disease. Clin Neurol Neuro- 146. Takatsu H, Nishida H, Matsuo H, et al. Cardiac sympathetic
surg 2005;107:396-403. denervation from the early stage of Parkinson’s disease: clinical
127. Osaki Y, Morita Y, Fukumoto M, Akagi N, Yoshida S, Doi Y. and experimental studies with radiolabeled MIBG. J Nucl Med
Three-dimensional stereotactic surface projection SPECT analy- 2000;41:71-77.
sis in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Mov Disord 147. Taki J, Nakajima K, Hwang EH, et al. Peripheral sympathetic
2005;20:999-1005. dysfunction in patients with Parkinson’s disease without auto-
128. Sawada H, Udaka F, Kameyama M, et al. SPECT findings in nomic failure is heart selective and disease specific. Eur J Nucl
Parkinson’s disease associated with dementia. J Neurol Neuro- Med 2000;27:566-573.
surg Psychiatry 1992;55:960-963. 148. Yoshita M, Taki J, Yamada M. A clinical role for [(123)I]MIBG
129. Spampinato U, Habert MO, Mas JL, et al. (99mTc)-HM-PAO myocardial scintigraphy in the distinction between dementia of
SPECT and cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: a com- the Alzheimer’s-type and dementia with Lewy bodies. J Neurol
parison with dementia of the Alzheimer type. J Neurol Neurosurg Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001;71:583-588.
Psychiatry 1991;54:787-792. 149. Orimo S, Amino T, Itoh Y, et al. Cardiac sympathetic denervation
130. Tachibana H, Kawabata K, Tomino Y, Sugita M, Fukuchi M. precedes neuronal loss in the sympathetic ganglia in Lewy body
Three-dimensional surface display of brain perfusion with 123I- disease. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 2005;109:583-588.
IMP in Parkinson’s disease. Neuroradiology 1994;36:276-280. 150. Braune S, Reinhardt M, Schnitzer R, Riedel A, Lucking CH.
131. Firbank MJ, Burn DJ, McKeith IG, O’Brien JT. Longitudinal Cardiac uptake of [123I]MIBG separates Parkinson’s disease
study of cerebral blood flow SPECT in Parkinson’s disease with from multiple system atrophy. Neurology 1999;53:1020-1025.
dementia, and dementia with Lewy bodies. Int J Geriatr Psychi- 151. Druschky A, Hilz MJ, Platsch G, et al. Differentiation of Parkin-
atry 2005;20:776-782. son’s disease and multiple system atrophy in early disease stages
132. Peppard RF, Martin WR, Clark CM, Carr GD, McGeer PL, Calne by means of I-123-MIBG-SPECT. J Neurol Sci 2000;175:3-12.
DB. Cortical glucose metabolism in Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s 152. Reinhardt MJ, Jungling FD, Krause TM, Braune S. Sintigraphic
disease. J Neurosci Res 1990;27:561-568. differentiation between two forms of primary dysautonomia early
133. Piert M, Koeppe RA, Giordani B, Minoshima S, Kuhl DE. after onset of autonomic dysfunction: value of cardiac and pul-
Determination of regional rate constants from dynamic FDG-PET monary iodine-123 MIBG uptake. Eur J Nucl Med 2000;27:595-
studies in Parkinson’s disease. J Nucl Med 1996;37:1115-1122. 600.
134. Vander Borght T, Minoshima S, Giordani B, et al. Cerebral 153. Soikkeli R, Partanen J, Soininen H, Paakkonen A, Riekkinen P,
metabolic differences in Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s diseases Sr. Slowing of EEG in Parkinson’s disease. Electroencephalogr
matched for dementia severity. J Nucl Med 1997;38:797-802. Clin Neurophysiol 1991;79:159-165.
154. Neufeld MY, Blumen S, Aitkin I, Parmet Y, Korczyn AD. EEG 167. Parsian A, Racette B, Goldsmith LJ, Perlmutter JS. Parkinson’s
frequency analysis in demented and nondemented parkinsonian disease and apolipoprotein E: possible association with dementia
patients. Dementia 1994;5:23-28. but not age at onset. Genomics 2002;79:458-461.
155. Sinanovic O, Kapidzic A, Kovacevic L, Hudic J, Smajlovic D. 168. Camicioli R, Rajput A, Rajput M, et al. Apolipoprotein E-ε4 and
EEG frequency and cognitive dysfunction in patients with Par- catechol-O-methyltransferase alleles in autopsy-proven Parkin-
kinson’s disease. Med Arh 2005;59:286-287. son’s disease: relationship to dementia and hallucinations. Mov
156. Hayashi R, Hanyu N, Shindo M, Tamaru F, Yanagisawa N. Disord 2005;20:989-994.
Event-related potentials, reaction time, and cognitive state in 169. Inzelberg R, Chapman J, Treves TA, et al. Apolipoprotein E4 in
patients with Parkinson’s disease. Adv Neurol 1993;60:429-433. Parkinson disease and dementia: new data and meta-analysis of
157. Toda K, Tachibana H, Sugita M, Konishi K. P300 and reaction published studies. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1998;12:45-48.
time in Parkinson’s disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 1993;6: 170. Koller WC, Glatt SL, Hubble JP, et al. Apolipoprotein E geno-
131-136. types in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Ann
158. Ito J. Somatosensory event-related potentials (ERPs) in patients Neurol 1995;37:242-245.
with different types of dementia. J Neurol Sci 1994;121:139-146. 171. Marder K, Maestre G, Cote L, et al. The apolipoprotein ε4 allele
159. Aotsuka A, Weate SJ, Drake ME, Jr, Paulson GW. Event-related in Parkinson’s disease with and without dementia. Neurology
potentials in Parkinson’s disease. Electromyogr Clin Neuro- 1994;44:1330-1331.
physiol 1996;36:215-220. 172. Polymeropoulos MH, Lavedan C, Leroy E, et al. Mutation in the
160. Tachibana H, Aragane K, Miyata Y, Sugita M. Electrophysiolog- a-synuclein gene identified in families with Parkinson’s disease.
ical analysis of cognitive slowing in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Science 1997;276:2045-2047.
Sci 1997;149:47-56.
173. Singleton AB, Farrer M, Johnston J, et al. ␣-Synuclein locus
161. Sohn YH, Kim GW, Huh K, Kim JS. Dopaminergic influences on
triplication causes Parkinson’s disease. Science 2003;302:841.
the P300 abnormality in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci 1998;
158:83-87. 174. Zarranz JJ, Alegre J, Gémez-Esteban J, et al. The new mutation,
162. Wang L, Kuroiwa Y, Kamitani T, Takahashi T, Suzuki Y, Ha- E46K, of ␣-synuclein causes Parkinson and Lewy body dementia.
segawa O. Effect of interstimulus interval on visual P300 in Ann Neurol 2004;55:164-173.
Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:497- 175. Wszolek ZK, Vieregge P, Uittis RJ, et al. German-Canadian
503. Family (Family A) with parkinsonism, amyotrophy, and demen-
163. Pirtosek Z, Jahanshahi M, Barrett G, Lees AJ. Attention and tia: longitudinal observaton. Parkinsonism Related Disord 1997;
cognition in bradykinetic-rigid syndromes: an event-related po- 3:125-136.
tential study. Ann Neurol 2001;50:567-573. 176. Golbe LI, Di Iorio G, Sanges G, et al. Clinical genetic analysis of
164. Wang H, Wang Y, Wang D, Cui L, Tian S, Zhang Y. Cognitive Parkinson’s disease in the Contursi kindred. Ann Neurol 1996;
impairment in Parkinson’s disease revealed by event-related po- 40:767-775.
tential N270. J Neurol Sci 2002;194:49-53. 177. Marder K, Tang MX, Alfaro B, et al. Risk of Alzheimer’s disease
165. Perriol MP, Dujardin K, Derambure P, et al. Disturbance of in relatives of Parkinson’s disease patients with and without
sensory filtering in dementia with Lewy bodies: comparison with dementia. Neurology 1999;52:719-724.
Parkinson’s disease dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol 178. Kurz M, Alves G, Aarsland D, LArsen JP. Familial Parkinson’s
Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76:106-108. disease: a community-based study. Eur J Neurol 2003;10:159-
166. Harhangi BS, de Rijk MC, van Duijin CM, Van Vroeckhoven C, 163.
Hofman A, Breteler MMB. APOE and the risk of PD with or 179. Levy G, Louis ED, Mejia-Sanatana H, et al. Lack of familial
without dementia in a population-based study. Neurology 2000; aggregation of Parkinson disease and Alzheimer disease. Arch
54:1272-1276. Neurol 2004;61:1033-1039.