Vinod Goel (2007) - Anatomy of Deductive Reasoning
Vinod Goel (2007) - Anatomy of Deductive Reasoning
10
Much of cognitive research on deductive reasoning has explanatory framework to organize and contextualize
been preoccupied with advocating for or against visuos- the data within a cognitive neuroscience framework is
patial (mental model theory) or linguistic/syntactic provided.
(mental logic theory) models of logical reasoning. Neu-
roimaging studies bear on this issue by pointing to both Overview of reasoning studies
language-based and visuospatial systems being engaged Categorical syllogisms
during logical reasoning, and by raising additional issues Syllogisms involve reasoning about quantification and
not anticipated by these cognitive theories. Here, the negation. Because they come in several logical forms
literature on the neural basis of deductive reasoning from and so are available for multiple presentations without
the past decade is reviewed. Although these results might repetition, they have constituted the stimuli in a half-
seem chaotic and inconsistent, we identify several inter- dozen neuroimaging studies. Early positron emission tom-
esting patterns and articulate their implications for cog- ography PET (Oxygen 15 [O15]) studies of reasoning by
nitive theories of reasoning. Cognitive neuroscience data Goel et al. [8,9] used syllogisms (of the form ‘some officers
point away from a unitary system for logical reasoning are generals; no privates are generals; some officers are not
and towards a fractionated system dynamically reconfi- privates’) and reported activation in the left hemisphere
gured in response to specific task and environmental (LH) frontal–temporal system. Another early PET (O15)
cues. study by Osherson et al. [10] used similar stimuli (of the
form ‘none of the bakers play chess; some of the chess
Introduction players listen to opera; some of the opera listeners are not
Two major theories have dominated the cognitive bakers’) and reported activation in right occipital lobe,
literature on deductive reasoning. The major issue of con- right basal ganglia and left prefrontal cortex (PFC). More
tention between the two theories is whether deduction is recent event-related functional magnetic resonance ima-
underwritten by a system of (linguistic) rules sensitive to ging (fMRI) studies by Goel et al. [11,12] have identified a
the logical form of the argument (mental logic theory [1]), large neural network including bilateral PFC (left to right
or whether a visuospatial representation of the argument [L > R]), left temporal lobe and bilateral parietal lobule
is constructed and evaluated (mental model theory [2]). (L > R) as underlying logical reasoning. More interest-
Until recently, this hotly debated issue relied largely upon ingly, they have demonstrated that manipulation of the
behavioral data. During the past decade findings from content of arguments leads to systematic reorganization of
neuroimaging studies and lesion studies with neurological this neural system. This issue is addressed further below.
patients have generated interesting results that need to be
considered in developing a theory of logical reasoning. Simple conditional reasoning
There is, however, some disagreement within the scientific Conditional arguments involve the sentential connective
community as to the relevance and interpretation of these ‘if-then’ and constitute an important component of human
data. Some have argued that the results are not particu- reasoning. Noveck et al. [13] used maximally simple forms
larly helpful or even relevant to evaluating cognitive of if-then arguments (‘if there is a black square than there
theories [3]. Some have been perplexed by the apparent is a yellow circle; there is a black square; there is a red
lack of consistency in neural activation across different circle’) to examine the functional neuroanatomy of modus
studies of reasoning [4,5]. Still others see compelling evi- ponens (if p then q, p; therefore q) and modus tollens (if p
dence for their favorite theory of reasoning [6,7]. then q, not q; therefore not p) reasoning. They reported
This review begins by organizing the various studies by activation in the left lateralized parietal–frontal network
the type of logical arguments tested (categorical syllo- for both inference forms, with increased amounts of right
gisms, conditionals, transitive arguments) and summar- prefrontal activity during modus tollens. Similar results
izing the findings associated with the main effect of have been reported by Prado and Noveck [14]. More com-
reasoning (i.e. the neural activation associated with plex forms of conditional reasoning have also been used in
reasoning over and above the baseline task; Box 1). The the literature.
data are indeed overwhelming and perplexing at first
glance. The similarities in the results across different Complex conditional reasoning
studies are identified and reasons are proposed for The Wason card selection task [15] is perhaps the most
the differences in results across the studies. Finally, an widely used task in the cognitive literature to assess con-
Corresponding author: Goel, V. ([email protected]).
tent effects in reasoning [16–20]. In the original version of
Available online 2 October 2007. the task, four cards are placed on a table, each having a
www.sciencedirect.com 1364-6613/$ – see front matter . Crown Copyright ß 2007 Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.003
436 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.10
the cat; the ape is dirtier than the cat; the dog is cleaner indicates that logical reasoning involves large networks
than the ape’) and reported similar activation in a bilateral encompassing much of the brain. (The overall network
parietal–frontal system (LH > RH) plus bilateral temporal activated in one of these studies [11] is presented in
systems. Fangmeier et al. [31] used visually presented Figure 1a.) It is also apparent that the activations from
nonlinguistic stimuli and reported activation in bilateral no two studies are identical. This has caused some concern
frontal–parietal systems for the reasoning phase of the and confusion in the literature. For example, if one is
task. Goel et al. [32] used transitive arguments involving searching for a ‘logic module’ these results are bound to
familiar geographic relations (e.g. ‘Paris is north of Cairo’) seem chaotic and unsatisfactory, and lead to worries about
and found activation in the bilateral hippocampus, in poor design and experimental controls [4,5]. Alternatively,
addition to the frontal–parietal system. if one is searching for evidence for a favorite theory (for
example, mental models theory [7]) with a selective eye,
Interpreting results there is much to choose from. It is suggested here that
Table 1 summarizes the neural activations of the main both of these approaches are overly constrained by,
effect of reasoning reported by the above studies. It and self-serving to, cognitive criteria at the expense of
Table 1. Summary of particulars of 19 neuroimaging studies of deductive reasoning and reported regions of activation
corresponding most closely to the main effect of reasoning
Studies Scanning Stimuli Occipital lobes Parietal lobes Temporal Basal Cingulate Frontal lobes
(organized by method modality lobes ganglia
tasks)
RH LH RH LH RH LH RH LH RH LH RH LH
Transitivity (explicit)
Goel et al. PET Visual, 19 37 Yes 24, 32 45, 46
(1998) linguistic
Goel & Dolan fMRI Visual, 17, 18, 19 7, 40 7, 40 Yes Yes 6 6, 9
(2001) linguistic 19
Knauff et al. fMRI Auditory, 7 7 21 21, 38 6 46, 47
(2003) linguistic
Goel et al. fMRI Visual, 18, 19 18, 19 7, 40 7 21, 22, 21, 22, 11, 47 6, 9, 46,
(2004) linguistic Hi Hi 11
Fangmeier et fMRI Visual, 7 40 32 6 6, 9
al. (2006) nonlinguistic
Transitivity (implicit)
Acuna et al. fMRI Visual, 7, 39, 40 39, 40 Yes 6, 8, 9, 46 6, 8, 9, 46
(2002) nonlinguistic
Heckers et al. fMRI Visual, 40 40 37, Hi 37, 21 Yes 24 PSMA, 6 6, 47
(2004) nonlinguistic
Categorical syllogisms
Goel et al. PET Visual, 21, 22 24, 32 45, 46, 47
(1998) linguistic
Osherson et al. PET Visual, 18 Yes 6
(1998) linguistic
Goel et al. fMRI Visual, 18, 19 18 7 21/22 Yes Yes 45 44, 45
(2000) linguistic
Goel & Dolan fMRI Visual, 17, 18 17, 18 21, 22, Yes 6 6, 44
(2003) linguistic 38
Goel & Dolan fMRI Visual, 18 18, 19 7 37 39 Yes Yes 6 6, 44, 45
(2004) linguistic
Conditionals (Simple)
Noveck et al. fMRI Visual, 19 7 37 32 6, 47
(2004) linguistic
Prado & fMRI Visual, 18 17 39, 40 40 6, 45, 46 9, 46
Noveck (2006) linguistic
Conditionals (complex)
Houde et al. PET Visual,
(2000) * nonlinguistic
Parsons et al. PET Visual, 18 18 21, 37, Yes Yes 24 31 10, 44, 9
(2002) linguistic 39
Canessa et al. fMRI Visual, 19 19 7, 39, 40 7, 39, Yes 32 32 6, 8, 9, 6, 8, 9, 46
(2005) linguistic 40 10, 46
Mixed stimuli
Goel et al. PET Visual,
(1997) linguistic
Knauff et al. fMRI Auditory, 19 19 7, 40 7, 14 21, 22 21, 22 32 32 6, 9 6, 9
(2001) linguistic
Numbers denote Brodmann Areas; RH = right hemisphere; LH = left hemisphere; Hi = hippocampus; PSMA = pre-sensory-motor area.
Blank cells indicate absence of activation in region. ‘‘Stimuli modality’’ refers to the form and manner of presentation of the stimuli.
Cerebellum activations are not noted in the table.
*
Brodmann Areas not provided by authors.
www.sciencedirect.com
438 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.10
absence of a commitment to a specific cognitive theory of inhibit the prepotent response associated with the
reasoning suggests that human reasoning is underwritten belief-bias. These belief-biased responses activate ventro-
by a fractionated system that is dynamically configured in medial PFC (VMPFC) (BA 11, 32), highlighting its role in
response to specific task and environmental cues. Three of non-logical, belief-based responses. The correct response
these lines of fractionation are reviewed below. indicates that subjects detected the conflict between their
beliefs and the logical inference, inhibited the prepotent
Systems for dealing with familiar and unfamiliar response associated with the belief-bias, and engaged the
material formal reasoning mechanism. The detection of this conflict
As noted above, cognitive theories of reasoning disagree requires engagement of right lateral/dorsal lateral PFC
whether logical reasoning is underwritten by the linguistic (BA 45, 46) [11,12,14] (Figure 2). This conflict detection role
system or a visuospatial system. According to the results of of right lateral/dorsal PFC is a generalized phenomenon
imaging studies, both systems are involved and their that has been documented in a wide range of paradigms in
engagement can be systematically manipulated by manip- the cognitive neuroscience literature [43–45]. The robust
ulating the content and context of the stimuli [11,12,32]. activation of right PFC in Acuna et al. [27] might have
More specifically, a left lateralized frontal–temporal con- resulted from the conflict introduced by the switching of
ceptual/language system (Figure 1b) processes familiar, spatial order in stimulus pairs (corresponding to the inver-
conceptually coherent material whereas a bilateral parie- sion of relations in linguistic arguments, e.g. Mary is taller
tal visuospatial system (Figure 1c) processes unfamiliar, than John versus John is shorter than Mary).
nonconceptual or incoherent material. It has been pro-
posed [34] that the frontal–temporal pathway (Broadman Systems for dealing with certain and uncertain
area [BA] 47, 21/22) corresponds to a heuristic system information
whereas the parietal pathway (BA 7, 40) corresponds to Cognitive theories of reasoning do not typically postulate
a formal/universal system, along the lines developed by different mechanisms for reasoning with complete and
Newell and Simon [35] or dual mechanism theory [6,36,37]. incomplete information. For example, the major cognitive
A proper treatment of the relationship between these theories of reasoning assume that the same cognitive system
various theoretical accounts and these data are beyond would deal with the following inferences [46,47]:
the scope of this review. But, the general idea is that Mary is smarter than John; John is smarter than
reasoning about familiar situations automatically utilizes George; Mary is smarter than George (determinate,
situation-specific heuristics, which are based on back- valid trial)
ground knowledge and experience. Where no such heur- Mary is smarter than John; John is smarter than
istics are available (as in reasoning about unfamiliar George; George is smarter than Mary (determinate,
situations), universal/formal methods must be used to invalid trial)
solve the problem. This latter system might rely on decon- Mary is smarter than John; Mary is smarter than
textualized or even visuospatial representations given the George; John is smarter than George (indeterminate,
involvement of the parietal system. invalid trial)
There is even some evidence to suggest that the However, patient and neuroimaging data suggest that
response of the frontal–temporal system to familiar situ- different neural systems underwrite these inferences. Goel
ations is content specific to some degree (in keeping with et al. [48] tested neurological patients with focal unilateral
some content specificity in the organization of temporal frontal lobe lesions (Figure 3) on a transitive inference
lobes) [38]. For example, although middle temporal lobe task while systematically manipulating completeness of
regions are activated when reasoning about categorical information regarding the status of the conclusion (i.e.
statements such as ‘all dogs or pets’, in the case of making determinate and indeterminate trials). The results demon-
transitive inferences about familiar spatial environments, strated a double dissociation such that patients with left
reasoning is mediated by posterior hippocampus and para-
hippocampal gyrus, the same structures that underwrite
spatial memory and navigation tasks [32]. Perhaps the
most robust example of content specificity in the organiz-
ation of the heuristic system is the ‘theory of mind’ reason-
ing system identified by a number of studies [39,40].
www.sciencedirect.com
440 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.11 No.10
will move us beyond the sterility of mental models versus 30 Knauff, M. et al. (2003) Reasoning, models, and images: behavioral
measures and cortical activity. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 559–573
mental logic debate and further the development of cogni-
31 Fangmeier, T. et al. (2006) FMRI evidence for a three-stage model of
tive theories of reasoning (See Box 3). deductive reasoning. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 320–334
32 Goel, V. et al. (2004) The hippocampal system mediates logical
References reasoning about familiar spatial environments. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
1 Rips, L.J. (1994) The Psychology of Proof: Deductive Reasoning 16, 654–664
in Human Thinking, MIT Press 33 Shallice, T. (1988) From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure,
2 Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1994) Mental models, deductive reasoning, Cambridge University Press
and the brain. In The Cognitive Neurosciences (Gazzaniga, M.S., 34 Goel, V. (2005) Cognitive neuroscience of deductive reasoning. In
ed.), pp. 999–1008, MIT Press Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning (Holyoak, K. and
3 Fodor, J. (1999) Let your brain alone. London Review of Books 21, http:// Morrison, R., eds), pp. 475–492, Cambridge University Press
www.lrb.co.uk/v21/n19/print/fodo01_.html, 30 September (1999) 35 Newell, A. and Simon, H.A. (1972) Human Problem Solving, Prentice-
4 Parsons, L.M. and Osherson, D. (2001) New evidence for distinct right Hall
and left brain systems for deductive versus probabilistic reasoning. 36 Stanovich, K.E. and West, R.F. (2000) Individual differences in
Cereb. Cortex 11, 954–965 reasoning: implications for the rationality debate. Behav. Brain Sci.
5 Monti, M.M. et al. (2007) Functional neuroanatomy of deductive 22, 645–665
inference: a language-independent distributed network. Neuroimage 37 Sloman, S.A. (1996) The empirical case for two systems of reasoning.
37, 1005–1016 Psychol. Bull. 119, 3–22
6 Evans, J.S. (2003) In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning. 38 McCarthy, R.A. and Warrington, E.K. (1990) Cognitive
Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 454–459 Neuropsychology: A Clinical Introduction, Academic Press
7 Knauff, M. et al. (2002) Spatial imagery in deductive reasoning: a 39 Fletcher, P.C. et al. (1995) Other minds in the brain: a functional
functional MRI study. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 203–212 imaging study of ‘‘theory of mind’’ in story comprehension. Cognition
8 Goel, V. et al. (1997) The seats of reason: a localization study of 57, 109–128
deductive & inductive reasoning using PET (O15) blood flow 40 Goel, V. et al. (1995) Modelling other minds. Neuroreport 6, 1741–1746
technique. Neuroreport 8, 1305–1310 41 Wilkins, M.C. (1928) The effect of changed material on the ability to do
9 Goel, V. et al. (1998) Neuroanatomical correlates of human reasoning. formal syllogistic reasoning. Arch. Psychol. 16, 5–83
J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10, 293–302 42 Evans, J.S.B.T. et al. (1983) On the conflict between logic and belief in
10 Osherson, D. et al. (1998) Distinct brain loci in deductive versus syllogistic reasoning. Mem. Cognit. 11, 295–306
probabilistic reasoning. Neuropsychologia 36, 369–376 43 Fink, G.R. et al. (1999) The neural consequences of conflict between
11 Goel, V. et al. (2000) Dissociation of mechanisms underlying syllogistic intention and the senses. Brain 122, 497–512
reasoning. Neuroimage 12, 504–514 44 Stavy, R. et al. (2006) Intuitive interference in quantitative reasoning.
12 Goel, V. and Dolan, R.J. (2003) Explaining modulation of reasoning by Brain Res. 1073, 1074, 383–388
belief. Cognition 87, B11–B22 45 Caramazza, A. et al. (1976) Right-hemispheric damage and verbal
13 Noveck, I.A. et al. (2004) The neural basis of conditional reasoning with problem solving behavior. Brain Lang. 3, 41–46
arbitrary content. Cortex 40, 613–622 46 Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1993) Human and Machine Thinking, Erlbaum
14 Prado, J. and Noveck, I.A. (2007) Overcoming perceptual features in Associates
logical reasoning: a parametric functional magnetic resonance imaging 47 Osherson, D. (1975) Logic and models of logical thinking. In Reasoning:
study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 642–657 Representation and Process in Children and Adults (Falmagne, R.J.,
15 Wason, P.C. (1966) Reasoning. In New Horizons in Psychology (Foss, ed.), pp. 81–92, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
B., ed.), pp. 135–151, Penguin 48 Goel, V. et al. (2007) Hemispheric specialization in human prefrontal
16 Cosmides, L. (1989) The logic of social exchange: has natural selection cortex for resolving certain and uncertain inferences. Cereb Cortex 17,
shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. 2245–2250
Cognition 31, 187–276 49 Wolford, G. et al. (2000) The left hemisphere’s role in hypothesis
17 Fodor, J. (2000) Why we are so good at catching cheaters. Cognition 75, formation. J. Neurosci. 20 (6), 1–4
29–32 50 Corballis, P.M. (2003) Visuospatial processing and the right-
18 Gigerenzer, G. and Hug, K. (1992) Domain-specific reasoning: social hemisphere interpreter. Brain Cogn. 53, 171–176
contracts, cheating, and perspective change. Cognition 43, 127–171 51 Gazzaniga, M.S. (1985) The Social Brain, BasicBooks
19 Griggs, R.A. and Cox, J.R. (1982) The elusive thematic-materials effect 52 Gazzaniga, M.S. (1998) The Mind’s Past, University of California Press
in Wason’s selection task. Br. J. Psychol. 73, 407–420 53 Gazzaniga, M.S. (2000) Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric
20 Stenning, K. and Lambalgen, M. (2001) Semantics and psychology: communication: does the corpus callosum enable the human condition?
Wason’s selection task as a case study. J. Log. Lang. Inf. 10, 273–317 Brain 123, 1293–1326
21 Canessa, N. et al. (2005) The effect of social content on deductive 54 Read, D.E. (1981) Solving deductive-reasoning problems after
reasoning: an fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, 30–43 unilateral temporal lobectomy. Brain Lang. 12, 116–127
22 Houde, O. et al. (2000) Shifting from the perceptual brain to the logical 55 Langdon, D. and Warrington, E.K. (2000) The role of the left
brain: the neural impact of cognitive inhibition training. J. Cogn. hemisphere in verbal and spatial reasoning tasks. Cortex 36, 691–702
Neurosci. 12, 721–728 56 Whitaker, H. et al. (1991) Inference deficits after brain damage. J. Clin.
23 Houde, O. and Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2003) Neural foundations of Exp. Neuropsychol. 13, 38
logical and mathematical cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 507–514 57 Varley, R. and Siegal, M. (2000) Evidence for cognition without
24 Houde, O. et al. (2001) Access to deductive logic depends on a right grammar from causal reasoning and ‘theory of mind’ in an
ventromedial prefrontal area devoted to emotion and feeling: evidence agrammatic aphasic patient. Curr. Biol. 10, 723–726
from a training paradigm. Neuroimage 14, 1486–1492 58 Adolphs, R. et al. (1996) Neuropsychological approaches to reasoning
25 Delius, J.D. and Siemann, M. (1998) Transitive responding in and decision-making. In Neurobiology of Decision-Making (Damasio,
animals and humans: exaption rather than adaption? Behav. A.R., ed.), pp. 157–178, Springer-Verlag
Processes 42, 107–137 59 Goel, V. et al. (2004) Asymmetrical involvement of frontal lobes in
26 Acuna, B.D. et al. (2002) Cognitive mechanisms of transitive inference. social reasoning. Brain 127, 783–790
Exp. Brain Res. 146, 1–10 60 Golding, E. (1981) The effect of unilateral brain lesion on reasoning.
27 Acuna, B.D. et al. (2002) Frontal and parietal lobe activation during Cortex 17, 31–40
transitive inference in humans. Cereb. Cortex 12, 1312–1321 61 Deglin, V.L. and Kinsbourne, M. (1996) Divergent thinking styles of the
28 Heckers, S. et al. (2004) Hippocampal activation during transitive hemispheres: how syllogisms are solved during transitory hemisphere
inference in humans. Hippocampus 14, 153–162 suppression. Brain Cogn. 31, 285–307
29 Goel, V. and Dolan, R.J. (2001) Functional neuroanatomy of three-term 62 Goel, V. (2003) Evidence for dual neural pathways for syllogistic
relational reasoning. Neuropsychologia 39, 901–909 reasoning. Psychologica 32, 301–309
www.sciencedirect.com