Chapter 8: Cognitive Views of Learning
Chapter 8: Cognitive Views of Learning
Information is encoded in sensory memory, where attention determines what will be held in short-term memory
for further use. In short-term memory, new information connects with knowledge from long-term memory.
Thoroughly processed and connected information becomes part of long-term memory and can be activated to
return to short-term memory
● According to this model, stimuli from the environment (input) flow into the sensory registers, one
for each sensing modality (seeing, hearing, tasting, etc.)
○ From there, some information is encoded and moves to short-term memory
○ Short-term memory holds information very briefly, combines it with information from
long-term memory, and with enough effort, moves some information into long-term
memory storage
○ Short-term memory is also responsible for generating responses or output
● This model proved helpful, but also incomplete
○ For example, in the model, information moved through the system mostly in one way,
from sensory registers to long-term memory, but research indicated many more
interactions and connections among the processes
○ The model could not explain how out-of-awareness memories or knowledge could
influence learning or how several cognitive processes could happen
simultaneously—like many small computers operating in parallel
● A more recent cognitive science information processing model retains some of the features of
the old approach, but emphasizes the role of working memory, attention, and the interactions
among the elements of the system, as shown in Figure 8.2, which is based on several sources
(Ashcraft & Radvansky, 2010; Bruning et al., 2011; Sternberg & Sternberg, 2012)
Information is encoded in sensory memory, where perception and attention determine what will be held in
working memory for further use. In working memory, executive processes manage the flow of information and
integrate new information with knowledge from long-term memory. Thoroughly processed and connected
information becomes part of long-term memory, and when activated again, becomes part of working memory.
Implicit memories are formed without conscious effort. All three elements of the system interact with each other
to guide perception; represent, organize, and interpret information; apply and modify propositions, concepts,
images, schemas, and strategies; construct knowledge; and solve problems. Attention has a role in all three
memory processes and in the interactions among them.
II. Memory
a. Sensory Memory (pp. 320-323)
- Sensory memory is the initial or first step in the processing of incoming stimuli from
the environment (sounds, sights, scents, etc.) into information that individuals can
understand. Sensory memory is also known by the terms sensory buffer, iconic
memory (for pictures), and echoic memory (for sounds).
i. Capacity, Duration and Contents
● The capacity of sensory memory is tremendous, and it can store far more
information than humans can manage at any given time. However, this vast
amount of sensory information is fast. It only lasts around 3 seconds.
ii. Perception
● Perception as we can recall from General Psychology is the process of
perceiving a stimulus and assigning meaning to it. This meaning is created
by combining physical world representations with our prior knowledge. For
example, consider the following symbols: 13. You would reply "B" if asked what
letter it is. You'd reply "13" if someone asked you what number it is. The symbols
themselves stay the same; their meaning changes depending on your
expectation to recognize a letter or a number, as well as your understanding of
how Arabic numbers and the Latin alphabet appear.
a. Bottom Up
i. The process from sensory input to recognized objects probably
goes through several stages. The initial phase involves extracting
or analyzing characteristics to create a rough sketch. The stimulus
must be examined into features or components and organized into
a meaningful pattern "from the bottom up," this feature analysis
is known as data-driven or bottom-up processing.
b. Gestalt Theorists
i. As perception continues, the features are organized into patterns.
Gestalt theorists, a kind of psychologist that investigated these
processes in Germany early in the twentieth century (and later in
the United States). Gestalt refers to people's ability to arrange
sensory information into patterns or connections. It comes from
the German word "pattern" or "configuration."
c. Top-Down Processing
i. Learning would be pretty slow if all perception was based only on
feature analysis and Gestalt concepts. At the last stage of
perception, the features and patterns detected are combined in
light of the context of the situation and our existing knowledge or
called Top-Down Processing or Conceptually Driven Processing.
So to recognize patterns rapidly, in addition to noting features, we
use context and what we already know about the situation or our
knowledge about words or pictures or the way the world generally
operates. In Figure 8.2, the role of knowledge in perception is
represented by the arrows between long-term memory (stored
knowledge), working memory, and sensory memory.
iii. Role of Attention
● Life would be impossible if every variation in color, movement, sound, smell,
temperature, and other characteristics ended up in working memory.
Fortunately, attention is selective. We restrict the possibilities of what we will
perceive and comprehend by paying attention to certain stimuli while
ignoring others. Because what we pay attention to is influenced to some extent
by what we already know and what we need to know. All three memory
processes indicated in Figure 8.2 are involved in and influenced by attention.
But attention takes effort and is a limited resource. I imagine you have to
work a bit to pay attention to these words about attention! We can pay attention
to only one cognitively demanding task at a time (Sternberg & Sternberg,
2012). For example, when I was learning to drive, I couldn’t listen to the radio
and drive at the same time. After some practice, I could listen, but I had to turn
the radio off when traffic was heavy. After years of practice, I can plan a class,
listen to the radio, and carry on a conversation as I drive. This is possible
because many processes that initially require attention and concentration
become automatic with practice. Actually, automaticity is probably a matter of
degree; depending on how much practice we've had, the scenario, and whether
we're consciously concentrating our attention and guiding our own cognitive
processes, we're more or less automatic in our performances. For example,
even experienced drivers might become very attentive and focused during a
blinding blizzard—and no one should text or talk on a cell phone while driving.
But the AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety found that over half of the adult drivers
in the United States admit to using a phone while driving, even though research
has shown that driving while chatting is equivalent to driving while drinking.
● Also, the content of the tasks makes a difference. Some tasks, such as
walking and chewing gum, call on different cognitive and physical resources; and
both walking and chewing are pretty automatic. But other complex tasks, such as
driving and talking on the phone, require some of the same cognitive
resources—paying attention to traffic and paying attention to what the caller is
saying. The problem with multitasking comes with simultaneous, complex tasks.
● For tasks that are at all complicated, no matter how good you have become at
multitasking, your performance of the task will suffer (Hamilton, 2009). As
soon as you turn your attention to something else, the brain starts to lose
connections to what you were thinking about, like the answer to question 4 in
your math assignment. To find that brain pathway again (to spread activation
toward the needed information) means repeating what you did to find the path in
the first place, so finding the answer to question 4 takes more time. In fact, it can
take up to 400% longer to do a homework assignment if you are
multitasking (Paulos, 2007).
● Terry Judd (2013) summarizes the effects of multitasking for most people:
“While there is some evidence that multitasking efficiency—that is, the mechanics
of multitasking—can be improved through practice (Dux et al., 2009) any
advantage this confers would appear to be more than offset by a reduction in the
encoding of information acquired during multitasking into both shorter and
longer-term memory” (p. 366). In complicated situations, the brain prioritizes and
focuses on one thing. You may be able to listen to quiet instrumental music in the
background while you study, but favorite songs with words will steal your
attention away and it will take time to get back to what you were doing.
● Many factors in the classroom influence student attention. These factors can
all be used to gain attention.
a. Bright colors,
b. Underlining, highlighting of written or spoken words
c. Calling students by name
d. Surprise events
e. Intriguing questions
f. Variety in tasks and teaching methods
g. Changes in voice level, lighting, or pacing
b. Working Memory
● the “workbench” of the memory system
● the interface where new information is held temporarily and combined with
knowledge from long-term memory to solve problems or comprehend a lecture,
for example.
● This information held briefly in working memory points your thinking toward the
knowledge you need to retrieve from long-term memory to understand and solve
problems, so working memory “contains” what you are thinking about at the
moment (Demetriou, Spanoudis, & Mouyi, 2011).
● Unlike sensory memory or long-term memory, working memory capacity is
very limited—something many of your professors seem to forget as they race
through a lecture while you work to hold on to and make sense of their words and
PowerPoints.
The Difference between short term memory and working memory
● Short-term memory is not exactly the same as working memory.
● Working memory includes both temporary storage and active processing—the
workbench of memory—where active mental effort is applied to both new
information and old information from your store of knowledge—your long-term
memory.
● But short-term memory usually means just storage, the immediate memory for
new information that can be held for about 15 to 20 seconds (Baddeley, 2001).
● Early experiments suggested that the capacity of short-term memory was only
about 5 to 9 (the “magic 7,” + or -2) separate new bits of information at once (G.
A. Miller, 1956).
● Alan Baddeley and his colleagues are responsible for the model of working
memory that is central to our current understanding of human cognition
(Baddeley, 2007; Eysenck, 2012; Jarrold, Tam, Baddeley, & Harvey, 2011).
c. Long-Term Memory
● Storage of information over an extended period.
● This type of memory tends to be stable and can last a long time.
1. Procedural Memory
● Memory for procedural knowledge
● Memory for skills, habits, and how to perform tasks
● It may take a while to learn a procedure but once learned, this
knowledge tends to be remembered for a long time
a. Scripts
● Action sequences or plans for actions stored in memory
● For very young children, scripts seem to help them organize and
remember the predictable aspects of their world
● In terms of human survival, it is probably useful to remember what
is likely to keep happening and to notice when something is out of
place
b. Productions
● Specify what to do under certain circumstances. If A occurs, then
do B.
● The more practiced the procedure, the more authentic the action
and the more implicit memory
2. Priming Effects
● Activating information that already is in long term memory through
some out-of-awareness process
● It may be the fundamental process for retrievals as associations
are activated and spread through the memory system
● Occurs when an individual’s exposure to a certain stimulus
influences his or her response to a subsequent stimulus, without
any awareness of the connection.
III. Teaching for Deep, Long-Lasting Knowledge: Basic Principles and Applications
Challenge Solution
1. Automated Basic Skills - Skills that are applied without conscious thought
● Cognitive Stage - when we are first learning, we rely on declarative knowledge
and general problem-solving strategies to accomplish our goal
(1) “Think about” every step
(2) Heavy cognitive load on working memory
(3) Trial-and-error phase
● Associative Stage
(1) Individual steps of a procedure are combined or “chunked” into larger
units.
(2) One step smoothly cues the next.
● Autonomous Stage - the whole procedure can be accomplished without much
attention.
(1) Holds for the development of basic cognitive skills in any area, but
science, medicine, chess, and mathematics have been most heavily
researched.
(2) Requires many hours of successful practice to make skills automatic
2 Critical Factors:
a) Prerequisite knowledge - If students don’t have the essential prior knowledge
(concepts, schemas, skills, etc.), the cognitive load on working memory will be
too great.
b) Practice with feedback - allows you to form associations, recognize cues
automatically, and combine small steps into larger condition-action rules, or
productions
i) practice should include a simplified version of the whole process in a real
context. Practice in real contexts helps students learn not only how to do
a skill but also why and when.
2. Domain-Specific Strategies - Consciously applied skills that organize thoughts and
actions to reach a goal.
● Not all procedures can be automatic, even for experts in a particular domain.
(1) For example, no matter how expert you are in driving, you still have to
consciously watch the traffic around you because conditions are
constantly changing.
● Teachers need to provide opportunities for practice in many different
situations—for example, practice reading with package labels, magazines, books,
letters, operating manuals, web pages and so on.
C. Guidelines: Helping Students Understand and Remember
1. Make sure you have the students’ attention.
2. Help students separate essential from nonessential details and focus on the most
important information.
3. Help students make connections between new information and what they already know.
4. Provide for repetition and review of information.
5. Present material in a clear, organized way.
6. Focus on meaning, not memorization.
REFERENCES:
ASAPScience. (2020, October 22). The Muscle Song (Memorize Your Anatomy) | SCIENCE SONGS [Video].
YouTube. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=VmcQfCcGScY
Miss Rachel. (2017, August 6). The Periodic Table set to The Chainsmokers’ Closer [Video]. YouTube.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=gEUY4PWgRPo&ab_channel=MissRachel
Sprouts. (2017, July 16). The Memory Palace : Can You Do It? [Video]. YouTube.
https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=p9IOqd1LpkA&ab_channel=Sprouts
Tiny Medicine. (2021, February 22). Visual Memory Techniques: A Step by Step guide for fast memorization.
[Video]. YouTube. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=IxsOv1j20NE&ab_channel=TinyMedicine
Woolfolk, A. (2019). Educational Psychology (14th ed.). Singapore: Pearson Education