Discourse Analysis FINAL
Discourse Analysis FINAL
DISCOURSE
Levels of Analysis
Reference
Negotiation of meaning
Introduction to Discourse Analysis
Discourse analysis focuses on knowledge about language beyond the word, clause,
phrase and sentence that is needed for successful communication. It looks at patterns of
language across texts and considers the relationship between language and the social
and cultural contexts in which it is used.
The second kind of language is called discourse; and the search for what gives discourse
coherence is discourse analysis.
It is not accurate to regard discourse analysis as something totally new. The first know
students of language, the scholars of Greece and Rome, divided grammar from rhetoric.
In Britain, J.R. Firth saw language as part of a culture, which is in turn responsive to
the environment.
There are many other disciplines (philosophy, psychology, sociology, etc.) which examine
their object of study through language, and are thus carrying out their own discourse
analysis.
It was a sentence linguist, named Zellig Harris, who both coined the term discourse
analysis and initiated a search for language rules which would explain how sentences
were connected within a text by a kind of extended grammar.
He analyzed an advertisement for hair tonic, and set about searching for
grammatical rules to explain why one sentence followed another. His conclusions
are interesting.
o One of the possible directions for discourse analysis was: continuing
descriptive Linguistics beyond the limits of a single sentence at a time.
o Another possible direction was: correlating culture and language.
“In every language it turns out that almost all the results lie within a relatively short
stretch which we may call the sentence . . . Only rarely can we state restrictions across
sentences”
Zellig Harris
We must look beyond the formal rules operating within sentences, and
consider the people who use language, and the world in which it happens.
Pragmatics
It is a branch of linguistics concerned with the use of language in social contexts and the
ways in which people produce and comprehend meanings through language.
Phonology
Phonology is a branch of linguistics concerned with the systematic organization of
sounds in languages. It has traditionally focused largely on the study of the systems of
phonemes in particular languages, but it may also cover any linguistic analysis either at
a level beneath the word or at all levels of language where sound is considered to be
structured for conveying linguistic meaning.
Phonetics
Phonetics is a branch of linguistics that comprises the study of the sounds of human
speech. It is concerned with the physical properties of speech sounds: their physiological
production, acoustic properties, auditory perception, and neurophysiological status.
Morphology
Syntax
Syntax is the set of rules, principles, and processes that govern the structure of
sentences in a given language, specifically word order. The term syntax is also used to
refer to the study of such principles and processes.
Semantic
Semantics is the study of meaning. It focuses on the relation between signifiers, and
what they stand for; their denotation. Linguistic semantics is the study of meaning that
is used for understanding human expression through language.
It involves the interpretation of what people mean in a particular context and how
the context influences what is said.
It explores how a great deal of what is unsaid is recognized as part of what is
communicated.
o What determines the choice between the said and the unsaid?
Distance.
Closeness, whether is physical, social, or conceptual, implies
shared experience. On the assumption of how close or distant
the listener is, speakers determine how much needs to be said.
Pragmatics is the study of the relationships between linguistic forms and the users of
those forms.
Reference
Acts of communication can be pragmatically effective in three ways. First, the language
can be used to talk about something to express a proposition of some kind. This involves
making a connection with context in such a way as to make an appropriate reference.
The definite article the provides a contextual connection since it signals that what
is being referred to is common knowledge between the people engaged in the
conversation.
The adverb here locates the utterance in a particular context of place.
The prepositional phrase in a quarter of an hour locates the utterance in a
particular time.
o To the extent that the conversationalists recognize the coordinated,
appropriate reference is achieved.
Illocutionary force refers to the intention of the utterance of being a promise, advice, or
anything else. It will depend on the context of knowledge and assumption that speaker
assumes to be share.
Perlocutionary effect happens when the speaker is doing something else than just saying
an expression connected with the context. The speaker is not just acting, but acting
upon the other person, to bring about a certain state of mind or course of action. In
performing an illocutionary act, the speaker is also bringing about a perlocutionary
force.
Context
When we receive a linguistic message, we pay attention to many other factors apart from
the language itself. These are the paralinguistic features of a spoken message. This may
exist in written messages too, influenced by handwriting or typography.
These factors take us beyond the study of language, in a narrow sense, and force
us to look at other areas of inquiry –the mind, the body, society, the physical
world-, at everything.
In linguistics, there have been several schools of thought which believe that context
should be rules out of language analysis as far as possible. In this way, linguists will be
able to make discoveries about the language itself, and its system of rules which exist
quite independently of particular circumstances.
By removing these sorts of features – hesitations, false starts, social or regional dialects –
sentence linguists would argue that we take away what is incidental and variable in
language and leave what is permanent and invariable.
Yet, for the discourse analyst it may be exactly these transient and variable features
which enable us to understand the meaning of what is said, and the reason why the
order of sentences proceeds in the way that it does. The language learner needs to be
able to handle language which is not idealized –language in use.
We have two approaches to language: sentence linguistics and discourse analysis. They
both have an invaluable contribution to make to the understanding of language, and
they need each other.
Schema
The process of making sense by taking schematic bearings applies to the interpretation
of all texts. You cannot make sense of anything without bringing it within the confines of
what is preconceived as familiar. Everything new has to be related to what is given.
Negotiation of Meaning
All but the most constrained interactions are the result of the joint efforts of the
participants to make sense to each other. This is reflected in the amount of negotiation,
which is required in order for the conversational participant to ensure that their
messages are being received in the way they intended, and for the listeners to ensure
that they are interpreting what they hear correctly.
Types of Presupposition
Hedges
Conversational Implicature
Scalar Implicatures
Conventional Implicatures
Person, Spatial, and Temporal Deixis
Deixis is a technical term for one of the most basic things we do with utterances. It
means ‘pointing’ via language. Any linguist form used to accomplish this ‘pointing’ is
called a deictic expression.
Deictic expressions are also called indexicals. They are among the first forms to be
spoken by very young children and can be used to indicate:
Deixis is clearly a form of referring that is tied to the speaker’s context, with the basic
distinction between deictic expressions being ‘near speaker’ or ‘away from speaker’.
Person Deixis
Spatial Deixis concerns with itself with the spatial locations relevant to an utterance.
The locations may be those of the speaker and the addressee or those people or objects
being referred to, such as:
Here
There
Where
Yonder
For example:
The location from the speaker’s perspective can be fixed mentally as well as
physically.
Physically distant: speaker’s continue to use ‘hear’ to mean the home location,
when they are actually not there.
o Speaker’s also project themselves into other locations prior to actually being
in those locations. This is called deictic projection.
e.g. I will come later. (this means that the speaker is not there now)
Psychologically distant: physically closed objects will tend to be treated by the
speaker as psychologically close.
o Something physically distant will generally be treated as psychologically
distant.
e.g. The man over there.
o When a speaker wishes to mark something that is physically close as
psychologically distant.
e.g. I don’t like that.
Temporal Deixis
Temporal Deixis concerns itself with various times involved in and referred to in an
utterance.
e.g. tonight, last week, yesterday, before, after, now, then, soon
It has only two basic forms, the present (proximal) and the past (distant)
The basic distinctions presented for person, spatial, and temporal Deixis can all be seem
at work in one common structural distinction in English grammar: direct and indirect
(reported) speech.
The proximal forms presented in the first case have shifted to the corresponding
distal forms in the second case.
The regular difference in English reported discourse marks a distinction between
the ‘near speaker’ meaning of direct speech and the ‘away from speaker’ meaning
of indirect speech.
Deictic expressions and their interpretation depend on the context, the speaker’s
intention, and they express relative distance.
o Deictic expressions always communicate much more than what is said.
Presupposition and entailment are used to describe two different aspects of what it is
communicated but not said.
An entailment is something that logically follows from what is asserted in the utterance.
Sentence have entailment.
Presupposition in treated as a relationship between two propositions.
o P >> Q Proposition ‘p’ presupposes proposition ‘q’
e.g. Mary’s cat is cute (p) >> Mary has a cat (q)
Constancy under negation: the presupposition of statement will remain constant
even when that statement is negated.
o Mary’s cat isn’t cute (Not p) >> Mary has a cat (q)
Types of Presupposition
Presuppositions are associated with the use of large numbers of words, phrases, and
structures.
This occurs when the meaning of a whole sentence is a combination of the meaning of
its parts.
We expect the presupposition of a simple sentence will continue to be true when those
simple sentences become part of a more complex sentence. However, the meaning of
some presuppositions (as ‘parts’) doesn’t survive to become the meaning of some
complex sentences (as ‘wholes’).
e.g. 1
e.g. 2
TYPES OF ENTAILMENT
Speakers and listeners involved in conversation are generally cooperating with each
other. Collaboration is a necessary factor.
This sense of cooperation is one in which people having a conversation are not
normally assumed to be trying to confuse, trick, or withhold relevant information
from each other.
The cooperative principle: It makes your conversational contribution such as is required,
at the stage at which it occurs, by the accepted purpose or direction of the talk exchange
in which you are engaged.
Implicatures are primary examples of more being communicated than is said, but in
order for them to be interpreted, some basic cooperative principle must first be assumed
to be in operation.
Hedges
There are certain kinds of expressions speakers use to mark that they may be in danger
of not fully adhering to principles. These kinds of expressions are called hedges.
When making a statement, certain expressions are used to indicate the degree of
certainty concerning the information given.
The importance of the maxim of quality may be best measured by the number of
expressions we use to indicate that what we are saying may not be totally
accurate.
o As far as I know, they are married.
o I may be mistaken, but I thought I saw a wedding ring on her finger.
o I couldn’t live without her, I guess.
Caution notes can also be used to show that the speaker is conscious of the
quantity maxim.
o As you probably know, I am terrified of bugs.
o So, to cut a long story short, we grabbed our stuff and run.
Markers tied to the expectation of relevance for the maxim of relation, can be
found in the middle of speakers’ talk.
o I don’t know if this is important, but some of the files are missing.
o This may sound like a dumb question, but whose handwriting is this?
The awareness of the expectations of manner may also lead speakers to produce
hedges.
o This may be a bit confused, but I remember being in a car.
o I’m not sure if this makes sense, but the car had no lights.
Conversational Implicature
The basic assumption in conversation is that the participants are adhering to the
cooperative principle and the maxims.
e.g. Charlene: I hope you brought the bread and the cheese. (=b & c?)
Scalar Implicatures
e.g. I am studying linguistics and I’ve completed some of the required couses.
“Some of the required courses” -> The speaker created an implicature (+> not all/
+> not many/ +> not most)
The basis of the scalar implicature is that when any form in a scale is asserted,
the negative of all forms higher on the scale is implicated.
All the implicatures taken into consideration are part of what is communicated and not
said.
Speakers can always deny that they intended to communicate such meanings.
Conversational implicatures:
Conventional Implicatures
Conventional implicatures are NOT based on the cooperative principles or the maxims.
Speech Acts
Felicity Conditions
Speech Events
Speech Acts
Actions performed via utterances are generally called speech acts and are commonly
given more specific levels, such as:
Apology
Complaint
Compliment
Invitation
Promise
Request
These descriptive terms for different kinds of speech acts apply to the speaker’s
communicative intention in producing an utterance.
The speaker normally expects that his or her communicative intention will be
recognized by the hearer.
On any occasion, the action performed by producing an utterance will consist of three
related acts.
Locutionary act
The basic act of utterance.
The production of a meaningful linguistic expression.
o If people have difficulty in forming the sounds and words to create a
meaningful utterance in a language, then those people fail to produce
a locutionary act.
Illocutionary act
It is performed via the communicative force of an utterance.
We form an utterance with some kind of function in mind.
o We might utter something to make a statement, an offer, an
explanation, or for some other communicative purpose.
It is also known as the illocutionary force of the utterance.
Perlocutionary act
We create an utterance with a function always intending it to have an effect.
People perform utterances on the assumption that the hearer will recognize
the effect they intended.
o It is also known as the perlocutionary effect.
The most discussed dimension is the illocutionary force.
o The illocutionary force of an utterance is what it ‘counts as’.
o The same locutionary act may count as a prediction, a promise, or a
warning.
e.g. I’ll see you later.
(I predict that)
(I promise you that)
(I warn you that)
How can speakers assume that the intended illocutionary force will be recognized by the
hearer?
Performative verbs
Can I talk to Mary?
No, she’s not here.
I’m asking you – Can I talk to her?
And I’m telling you – SHE’S NOT HERE!
o Each speaker has described, and drawn attention to the illocutionary force
(‘ask’ and ‘tell’) of their utterances.
Intonation
a. You’re going!
o (I tell you that you are going)
b. You’re going?
o (I request your confirmation)
Word Order
Are you going?
o (I ask you if you are going)
The Interrogative Mood
It is supposed to indicate that the utterance is (intended as) a question.
The Directive Mood
It indicated that an utterance is (intended as) a directive illocutionary act.
e.g. an order, a request.
Stress, voice quality, the mood of the verb, punctuation
Felicity Conditions
The Felicity Conditions cover expected or appropriate circumstances for the performance
of a speech act to be recognized as intended.
Speech acts are performed via utterances are to assume that underlying every utterance
(U) there is a clause, containing a performative verb (Vp, which makes the illocutionary
force explicit.
For example:
Examples like b and d, normally without ‘hereby’, are used by speakers as explicit
performatives.
Examples like a and c are implicit performatives, sometimes called primary
performatives.
Technical disadvantages:
1. Uttering the explicit performative version of a command (e.g. in the case of b and
d), has much more serious impact than uttering the implicit version.
2. It is difficult to know exactly what the performative verb (or verbs) might be for
some utterances.
e.g.
i. You are dumber than a rock.
ii. ? I hereby insult you that you are dumber than a rock
Although the speaker and hearer might recognize the utterance in i as an
insult, it would be very strange to have ii as an explicit version.
Direct Speech
Move out of the way!
Act
The basic function of all
Indirect
Do you have to stand in front of the TV? utterances is a
Speech Act
command/request
Indirect
You’re standing in front of the TV.
Speech Act
One of the most common types of indirect speech act in English has the form of an
interrogative, but it is not typically used to ask a question (i.e. we do not expect
only an answer, we expect an action)
e.g.
Could you pass the salt?
Would you open this?
Indirect speech acts are associated with greater politeness in English than direct
speech acts.
Speech Events
It may include an obvious central speech act, such as ‘I don’t really like this’, as in
a speech event of ‘complaining’, but it will also include other utterances leading up
to and subsequently reacting to the central action.
For example:
Cohesion
Referential Cohesion
Conjunction
Lexical Cohesion
Information Structure
Genre
Rhetorical Patterns
Propositional Analysis
Cohesion
Cohesion is the grammatical and lexical relationship within a text or sentence. Cohesion
can be defined as the links that hold a text together and give it meaning.
It is the first standard of textuality; it refers to the surface relations between the
sentences that create a text and to create connected sentences within a sequence It
helps the reader /hearer to sort out the meaning and uses.
1. Reference
2. Substitution
3. Ellipsis
4. Conjunction
5. Lexical cohesion.
Referential Cohesion
The term reference is traditionally used in semantics to define the relationship between a
word and what it points to in the real world.
In the textual sense, though, reference occurs when the reader/listener has to retrieve
the identity of what is being talked about by referring to another expression in the same
context.
Types of Reference
Substitution refers to the action of replacing a word, phrase or clause with another one.
The words that are used to replace can only be interpreted in relation to what has gone
before the substitution. Also, it is largely limited to the immediately preceding clause.
Types of Substitution
Nominal Substitution
o For example:
There are some new tennis balls in the bag. These ones’ve lost their
bounce.
Verbal Substitution
o For example:
A: Annie says you drink too much.
B: So do you.
Clausal Substitution
o For example:
A: Is it going to rain?
B: I think so.
Ellipsis occurs when some essential structural element is omitted from a sentence or
clause and can only be recovered by referring to an element in the preceding text.
Types of Ellipsis
Nominal Ellipsis
o For Example:
My kids play an awful lot of sport. Both (my kids) are incredibly
energetic.
Verbal Ellipsis
o For example:
A: Have you been working?
B: Yes, I have (been working).
Clausal Ellipsis
o For example:
A: Why’d you only set three places? Paul’s staying for dinner, isn’t he?
B: Is he? He didn’t tell me (he was staying for dinner).
Conjunction
It is a cohesive device because it signals relationships that can only be fully understood
through reference to other parts of the text.
Types of Conjunction
Types of
Description Examples
conjunction
Adversative The information in the second Yet
sentence of each text moderates or Though
qualifies the information in the But
first. However
Nevertheless
Whereas
In fact
Actually
Additive They signal the presentation of And
additional information. In addition
Moreover
For example
Such as
Temporal Temporal relationships exist when To start with
the events in a text are related I Next
terms of the timing of their In conclusion
occurrence. Previously
At this point
From now on
In the meantime
As a result
For this purpose
Under the circumstances
Causal The relationship is one of cause and As a result
consequence. In consequence
That being the case
Otherwise
Therefore
Lexical Cohesion
Lexical cohesion occurs when two words in a text are semantically related in terms of
their meanings.
Reiteration
Collocation
Collocation includes all those items in a text that are semantically related. Sometimes,
this makes difficult to decide whether a cohesive relationship exists or not.
The problems arise because collocation is expressed through open rather than closed
class items: “Closed” lexical items include all grammatical words – such as pronouns,
conjunctions and preposition – membership of which is finite. In contrast, there is no
limit to the items that can be used to express collocation. This means that it is difficult
to establish set of regularly co-occurring word and phrases.
An additional problem is that lexical relationships are texts and context bound.
This means that words and phrases that are related in one text may not be related
in another.
o For example:
My neighbor has just left one of this tree falls into my garder. And the
scoundrel refuses to pay for the damage he has caused.
Neighbor and scoundrel are not related at all, but, in this case,
they are synonyms.
The background knowledge of the reader or listener plays a more obvious role in the
perception of lexical relationships than in the perception of other types of cohesion.
For example: Collocational patterns will only be perceived by someone who knows
something about the subject at hand.
One problem that arises in analyzing these relations in text has to do with how many
“steps” away an item can be in a word class and still contribute to cohesion.
Rose and flower seem more closely related than rose and plant.
Mosquito and insect can be accepted, but one wonders about mosquito and animal.
Various lexical relationships between the different sentences making up a text
provide a measure of the cohesiveness of the text.
o The centrality and importance to the text of any particular sentence within
the text will be determined by the number of lexical connections that
sentence has to other sentences in the text.
Information Structure
Information structure refers to the ways in which information is organized within and
beyond the sentence. It concerns with the way in which information is arranged within a
sentence will be affected by the pattern of the sentences within the text as a whole.