Declarative and Procedural Memory
Declarative and Procedural Memory
• Cognitive abilities, including attentional control and working memory, contribute to differing levels of L2
aptitude.
• Long-term memory, specifically declarative and procedural memory, may also serve as an individual
difference factor in L2 learning.
• Knowledge in declarative memory can occur rapidly, often after a single trial of learning.
• Knowledge in declarative memory can be used flexibly with other knowledge in declarative memory and with
knowledge from other memory systems.
• Learning abilities in declarative memory mature later than procedural memory learning abilities, improve until
early adulthood, remain relatively stable during middle adulthood, and then decline in older adulthood.
• Cognitive tasks used to assess declarative memory and its role in L2 acquisition include the Modern Language
Aptitude Test, Part V, the Continuous Visual Memory Task, the LLAMA-B, and the visual-auditory learning
subtest of the Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Ability.
• Evidence for the existence of declarative memory as a distinct memory system has been found in animal
models, amnesic patients, and healthy humans.
• Declarative memory is often supported by effortful encoding and retrieval in working memory but is also a
flexible memory system that can support learning on a wide variety of stimuli.
• Learning in procedural memory does not involve conscious awareness and is not supported by attention.
• Knowledge development occurs gradually and improves over multiple learning trials.
• Knowledge in procedural memory is typically encapsulated, unavailable for use by other memory systems and
inflexible with respect to the contexts in which it can be applied.
Development and Anatomical Support
• Learning abilities in procedural memory tend to be stable during childhood and adulthood.
• Evidence for procedural memory has been found in animal models, neuropsychology, and cognitive and
neuroimaging research with healthy participants.
• Studies suggest a dissociation between memory of cognitive and motor skills such as mirror drawing and
memory of facts or episodes.
• Procedural memory is supported by a different system from that which supports factual or episodic knowledge.
• Three theories have discussed various roles for declarative and procedural memory and knowledge in L2
acquisition: DeKeyser’s Skill Acquisition Theory, Paradis’ declarative/procedural model, and Ullman’s
declarative/procedural model.
• Declarative and procedural memory play crucial roles in all theories, but less is known about the role of these
long-term memory systems in L2.
• Declarative knowledge is characterized as knowledge 'that' that can be acquired quickly and via observation,
without performance.
• Procedural knowledge involves the performance of complex skills and is informationally encapsulated.
• Procedural knowledge allows the learner to 'chunk' steps from declarative knowledge into a single routine.
• Skill learning progresses through three stages: the declarative stage, the procedural stage, and the automatic
stage.
• DeKeyser's model suggests that declarative knowledge does not interface with procedural knowledge, but
plays a causal role in the development of procedural knowledge.
• The power law of learning, a mathematical formalization of how competency increases with practice, is
represented as a series of the three qualitatively distinct stages.
• For L1, declarative memory supports all non-grammatical aspects of language, including vocabulary, and any
grammatical aspects that are under explicit control.
• For L2, declarative memory will dominate learning of most aspects of the L2.
• Paradis argues against any sort of interface between these two memory systems.
• Fluency in Paradis' model may be attained either through learning explicit knowledge in declarative memory
or through acquiring implicit competence in procedural memory.
• Learning in declarative memory involves "speeded-up controlled use" of grammar, whereas learning in
procedural memory involves implicit competence through the internalization of grammar.
• Defined as learning arbitrary information such as word meanings, lexical subcategorization specifications, and
phonological forms.
• Also stores irregular and possibly higher frequency grammatical forms, especially in L2.
Procedural Memory
• Predicted to learn rule-governed sequences and probabilistic information in language, such as rules in the
mental grammar.
• The redundancy hypothesis suggests that the two memory systems often acquire the same or analogous
knowledge.
• The competition hypothesis suggests that acquiring knowledge in one system may inhibit learning in the other
system.
• Age of acquisition and the learning context can affect whether declarative and/or procedural memory primarily
underlies acquisition.
• The degree to which learners rely on declarative and procedural memory to process grammatical forms varies
depending on the learning context.
• Paradis’ model predicts declarative memory is responsible for most aspects of L2 acquisition, but in rare cases
L2 learners may internalize grammatical and/or lexical aspects of L2 and process them in procedural memory.
• Ullman’s model predicts different roles for declarative and procedural memory in L2 acquisition, and
hypotheses for the relationship between declarative and procedural memory.
• Declarative and procedural memory are expected to play a role in Language Acquisition (L2) acquisition.
• Evidence on this role is gathered from laboratory studies with one training condition, those with multiple
training conditions, and naturalistic learning studies.
• Two studies indirectly provided evidence for these theories by using tasks associated with procedural memory,
although no claims were made about procedural memory in these studies.
• Morgan-Short, Faretta-Stutenberg, Brill-Schuetz, Carpenter, and Wong (2014) examined L2 acquisition under
implicit training conditions.
• Findings indicated that declarative memory ability predicted L2 syntactic development at early stages of
acquisition, and procedural memory ability predicted L2 syntactic development at later stages.
• Neuroimaging data analysis showed a link between learners who were strong in declarative memory and the
use of procedural memory neural circuits in L2 processing at early stages of development.
• Strong declarative memory may lead to rapid acquisition of declarative knowledge that indirectly facilitates
proceduralization.
Ettlinger, Bradlow, and Wong (2014) examined the relationships between declarative memory, procedural
memory, and L2 acquisition.
• Findings indicated that participants who learned both the pattern and analogistic rules had high declarative and
procedural memory, simplifyors had high procedural memory but low declarative memory, and nonlearners had
low declarative and procedural memory.
Role of Declarative and Procedural Memory in Learning Syntactic and Morphophonological Rules
• Morgan-Short et al. (2014) and Ettlinger et al. (2014) examined the role of declarative and procedural memory
in implicit but intentional learning conditions.
• Hamrick (2015) used an incidental learning condition to examine the role of declarative and procedural
memory in L2 acquisition.
• After exposure to a semi-artificial language, declarative but not procedural memory abilities were correlated
with performance on the surprise recognition test.
• After a 1-3 week period of no exposure, procedural but not declarative memory abilities were correlated with
performance on the test.
• Antoniou, Ettlinger, and Wong (2016) extended Ettlinger et al.’s (2014) research with morphophonological L2
learning under a passive, exposure-based condition.
• Declarative memory was associated with learning the analogistic rule and procedural memory with learning
the pattern rule in all conditions, except when the analogistic rule was presented before the pattern rule.
• Brill-Schuetz and Morgan-Short (2014) found that procedural memory ability interacted with training
condition, resulting in more accuracy on a grammaticality judgment task in implicit training conditions.
• Tagarelli, Ruiz, Vega, & Rebuschat (2016) found that procedural memory was negatively associated with L2
acquisition of syntax in incidental but not the instructed learning condition.
• Suzuki (2017) examined the role of procedural memory in explicit training conditions, indicating that
procedural memory may also play a role in explicit and instructed conditions when reaction time is considered
as opposed to accuracy.
• Faretta-Stutenberg and Morgan-Short (2017) studied the role of declarative and procedural memory in L2
acquisition of Spanish syntax.
• They found no link between declarative memory and L2 learning in either context, despite L2 improvements.
• Procedural memory was related to changes in behavioral performance and neurocognitive processing in the
second language, but this relationship only held in the study-abroad context.
• Other studies provide additional evidence for the role of declarative and procedural memory in learning
syntactic, morphological, and morphophonological grammatical rules.
• Studies implicate a positive role for declarative memory at earlier stages of L2 learning, in implicit, exposure-
based, incidental, and classroom contexts, in learning analogistic rules when L2 input is not ordered, and in
learning pattern rules when preceded by analogistic rules.
• Procedural memory is positively associated with L2 learning at later stages of learning, in implicit, exposure-
based, incidental, and immersion contexts, but not in classroom contexts or explicit contexts.
• More replication and extension work is needed to fully understand the roles of declarative and procedural
memory in L2 learning.
• DeKeyser (2015), Paradis (2009), and Ullman (2015) predict roles for declarative and procedural memory in
Language Acquisition (L2) acquisition).
• Declarative memory supports learning of grammatical structures in L2 at early learning phases, while
procedural memory plays an increasingly important role as L2 proficiency develops.
Research Limitations
• Previous research is limited to syntactic, morphological, and morphophonological grammatical structures, and
most studies have examined learning in a laboratory-based, implicit or exposure-based context.
• Future research should study a wider range of linguistic structures and L2 acquisition in explicit instruction
contexts.
Unexplored Questions
Conclusion
• Defined as domain-general, cognitive, long-term memory constructs, declarative and procedural memory
should potentially be considered a part of L2 aptitude.
Source: Implications of the declarative/ procedural model for improving second language learning: The
role of memory enhancement techniques
Learner-Level Approaches
• Sleep (important for consolidating new declarative and/or procedural memories).
• Aerobic exercise (may augment hippocampal volumes and aspects of declarative memory).
• Diet (e.g., flavanols may improve declarative memory in older adults).
• Mindfulness (may improve declarative but inhibit procedural learning).
Limitations in Research
• The testing effect has been less studied than the spacing effect in language learning.
• Few studies have examined the effects of retrieval practice on word learning in both L1 and L2.
• There are no studies investigating retrieval practice effects specifically in grammar learning, in either L1 or
L2.
Non-invasive Interventions
• Non-invasive interventions can be item-level (e.g., spaced repetition, retrieval practice, deep encoding,
enactment effect, method of loci) or learner-level (e.g., sleep, exercise, diet, mindfulness).
• Spaced repetition (the spacing effect) and retrieval practice (the testing effect) are well-studied and effective,
relying on declarative memory and procedural memory.
• Both techniques are particularly effective for language retention, the goal of language learning.
Pedagogical Predictions
• The DP model makes several pedagogical predictions about spaced repetition.
• Language educators should space out coverage of topics, rather than introducing and completing each one
before moving on to the next.
• Language learners should space their study out over weeks, months, or years, continually coming back to the
same material.
• Spacing is generally most effective at longer retention intervals, making it useful for learners and educators.