Deep Summary For TS
Deep Summary For TS
(introduction)
What is a paradigm
:The normative way that things are done – the agreed concerns, concepts, methods, ways of
reasoning, and standards that make contributions to the dsicipline recognisable as such
Chapter 2 (Lec 2
research design)
2.3 Epistemological considerations
epistemological issues associated with social research, in other words, the question of what is
(or should be) seen as acceptable knowledge in a discipline. In the social sciences, a central
issue is whether the social world can and should be studied according to the same principles,
procedures, and ethos as the natural sciences
What is positivism?
Positivism is an epistemological position that argues for the use of natural science methods to
study social reality and beyond.
1. Only phenomena, and therefore knowledge confirmed by the senses, can genuinely be
considered as knowledge (a principle known as phenomenalism).
2. The purpose of theory is to generate hypotheses that can be tested so that they enable
explanations of laws —patterns and regularities—to be assessed (the principle of
deductivism).
3. Knowledge is reached by gathering together facts that provide the basis for laws (the
principle of inductivism).
For example, positivism involves elements of both a deductive approach (principle 2) and an
inductive approach (principle 3).
Interpretivism
the view that the social world cannot be studied using a scientific model. This is because the
subject matter of the social sciences—people and their institutions—is fundamentally
different from that of the natural sciences. The logic of social science research is, therefore,
also different, and the methods used in social science research need to reflect the
distinctiveness of human consciousness and experience.
What is interpretivism?
It is based on the view that there are fundamental differences between people and the objects
of the natural sciences. Therefore, social scientists need distinct research methods that respect
the differences between the natural world and the human one. These methods require the
researcher to grasp the subjective experience of social action, what these experiences mean in
practice, how those experiences and meanings are understood by others, and why they are
interpreted in such ways
Taking an interpretative stance can mean that the researcher comes up with surprising
findings, or at least findings that appear surprising outside the particular social context being
studied.
The key question for social scientists is whether social entities can and should be considered
as
Objectivism
What is objectivism?
Objectivism is an ontological position that claims that social phenomena, their meanings, and
the categories that we use in everyday discourse have an existence that is independent of, or
separate from, social actors.
Provide examples
a tree falls in the forest and it does make a sound, even if no one hears it
everyone else is living a life just as vivid and detailed as yours whether you know it or
not
Constructionism
is an ontological position that asserts that social phenomena and their meanings are
continually being created by social actors. It implies that social phenomena are not only
produced through social interaction but are in a constant state of revision
The authors argue that if we focus on the formal properties of organizations (rules,
organizational charts, regulations, roles), we tend to neglect the degree to which order in
these entities have to be accomplished in everyday interaction—
However, they do stress the active role of individuals in the construction of social reality.
Constructionism essentially invites the researcher to consider the ways in which social reality
is an ongoing creation of social actors rather than something external to actors and that totally
constrains them.
Constructionism also suggests that the categories people use to understand the world around
them are in fact social products. They are not external to us; their meaning is constructed in
and through interaction.
So a category such as ‘masculinity’ is not treated as a distinct entity, but as something whose
meaning is built up during interaction.
Deductive and inductive theory
The deductive approach
In this approach, the researcher draws on what is known about a particular domain and on
relevant theoretical ideas in order to deduce (come up with) a hypothesis (or hypotheses): a
speculation that they can test empirically. Embedded within the hypothesis will be concepts
that will need to be translated into researchable entities (often referred to as variables).
Provide examples of a questionarrie linking to a specific concept
It is worth noting that deductive approaches are more commonly used in quantitative
research; the language of hypotheses, variables and testing does not usually apply to
qualitative research
The last step in this process, revision of theory, it involves induction, as the researcher
reflects on the implications of their findings for the theory that prompted the whole exercise
For example, the findings of a study involving Twitter data by Williams et al. (2017a) built
upon the knowledge base relating to the ‘broken windows’ theory (Wilson and Kelling 1982),
which suggests that visible evidence of low-level crime, such as broken windows, encourages
more crime. In the study conducted by Williams and colleagues, tweets containing certain
terms relating to low-level disorder were found to be associated with rates of recorded crime,
offering an insight into how the theory manifests in social media:
The inductive approach
Here, theory is the outcome of research and is formed by drawing generalizable inferences
out of observations. In this way, induction represents an alternative strategy for linking theory
and research
just as deduction involves an element of induction, the inductive process is likely to involve a
degree of deduction. Once the researcher has carried out some theoretical reflection on a set
of data, they may want to collect further data in order to establish the conditions in which a
theory will and will not hold. This is often called an iterative strategy and it involves moving
back and forth between data and theory.
Example: O’Reilly’s
theoretical idea
(the idea of the ‘service
silo’ as a purposely designed managerial control strategy) comes from her data rather than
being formed before she had collected her data.
-front-line employees working in service silos (i.e. separate areas from which they could not
easily break out)
Considering deduction and induction together
-the deductive approach is in favour of a strict process of theory testing using hypotheses, but
it doesn’t address how researchers should select a theory in the first place. Inductive
reasoning is also criticized because empirical data does not inevitably allow researchers to
build theory.
Abduction acknowledges that the conclusions arising from an observation are plausible, but
not completely certain.
It can also be thought of as inference to the best explanation—for example, you start with an
observation or set of observations, such as smoke in your kitchen, and reason that the most
likely explanation is that you have burnt the dinner you had in the oven. There could be other
reasons for having smoke in your kitchen (perhaps someone left a window open, and smoke
from an outside fire has entered the room), so you cannot be certain of the source of the
smoke based on the initial observation alone, but the simplest and most probable explanation
is that it is coming from the remains of your meal.
Lecture 3 (Quantitative
research)
This book is about the research methods (see Key concept 1.1)— that is, the tools and
techniques—that social scientists use to explore such topics
Research methods
Quantitative research
- Ontology: The social world is independent, real and observable
- Objectively observed & measurable, General causal relationships
- Epistemology: Positivistic
- Discovering the truth, explaining social patterns & behaviour
it takes a deductive view of the relationship between theory and research, meaning that
theory comes first, driving the research, rather than emerging out of it;
it has a preference for the natural science approach (and positivism in particular);
it has an objectivist conception of social reality, implying that social phenomena and
their meanings have an existence that is independent of social actors.
The main methods of data collection associated with quantitative research are
Measurement
Causality
Quantitative researchers are rarely focused on just describing how things are; they want to say
why things are the way they are. For example, if a researcher was studying racial prejudice,
they would generally want to go further than describing how much prejudice exists in a
certain group of individuals, or what proportion of people in a sample are prejudiced. They
would probably want to examine the causes of variation in racial prejudice, perhaps
explaining it in terms of personal characteristics (such as levels of authoritarianism) or social
characteristics (such as education or social mobility).
Generalization
is whether they can generalize their findings beyond the context of their study—in other
words, whether their research has sufficient external validity
Replication
In order to minimize the influence of these potential problems, scientists may seek to
replicate (to reproduce) each other’s experiments. If the findings repeatedly couldn’t be
replicated, serious questions would be raised about their validity. As a result, scientists are
often very explicit about their procedures so that their experiments can be replicated.
Quantitative researchers in the social sciences also regard the ability to replicate as an
important part of their activity. It is therefore often seen as important that researchers clearly
present their procedures so they can be replicated by others, even if the research does not end
up being replicated.
-
Sources of quantitative data
- Perfect information: i.e. election results
- Experiments: Medicine vs placebo (high level of control)
- Sampling in surveys, involvement of variables
- Observations: Smokers/nonsmokers, little background knowledge
Types of variables
- Textual: Education level, Opinions, ethnicity etc
- Measurement: Nominal and Ordinal
- Numerical: quantities
- Measurement: Ratio
Research designs
- Quasi-Experiments
- Cross sectional research
- Research done at single point in time
- Many observations in a single or multiple spatial units
- Residents of one or many neighbourhood
- Good for describing & measuring associations
The four most common criticisms of quantitative approaches to research are as follows.
1. Researchers treat people and social institutions in the same way as ‘the world of
nature’.
3. The reliance on instruments and procedures limits the connection between research
and everyday life.
4. The analysis of relationships between variables creates a static view of social life as if
it were independent of people’s lives.
.
Lecture 4 (Qualitative
research)
Slides
16.1 Introduction
Qualitative research is a type of research strategy that emphasizes words, images, and
objects when collecting and analysing data. It is broadly inductivist, constructionist, and
interpretivist, but it can take a wide variety of forms;
Qualitative research aims to generate deep insights concerning particular topics, and it does
this through a considered engagement with places and social actors. This might include
people, communities, organizations, or institutions.
It tends toward an inductive view of the relationship between theory and research,
with the former generated out of the latter (though see the discussion in Section 2.2 on
abductive reasoning, in which we qualify this view).
We might summarize the more recent themes and ‘movements’ associated with qualitative
research as follows.
• The use of creative and participatory approaches. Creative research and participatory
research methods attempt to rebalance power relations in research by conducting studies ‘by’
or ‘with’ rather than ‘on’ participants.
Creative methods include collage, storytelling, sandboxing, puppetry, drawing, theatre, and
various other forms or artistic expression, as well as everyday activities such as walking
The main methods of data collection associated with qualitative research are
ethnography/participant observation;
qualitative interviewing;
focus groups;
Strauss and Corbin define grounded theory as ‘theory that was derived from data,
systematically gathered and analysed through the research process. In this method, data
collection, analysis, and eventual theory stand in close relationship to one another’ (1998:
12). There are two central features of grounded theory:
2. the approach is iterative (sometimes called recursive), meaning that data collection
and analysis proceed in tandem, repeatedly referring back to each other.
Correlation vs Causality
- Correlation: Two things appearing together regularly
- Causality: One thing being caused by another
- Taking place in chronological order
Qualitative research
- Goals of qualitative research
- Describing / Understanding
- Explaining (how?)
- Interpreting meanings & narratives
Basic principles
Induction
-What relationships emerge from the data
Operationalization
-are the concept clearly defined
Sampling
-What sample is needed? Typical or extreme
Generalizing
-Focusing on diversity rather than on the average
Qualitative:
For example: data shows that Paris & Amsterdam have expensive city centers
- Deduction: because ALL city centres are generally expensive,
Amsterdam city centre is relatively expensive
- Induction: Ams & Paris city centers are expensive, therefore ALL city
centres are relatively expensive
Karl Popper was a prominent philosopher of science known for his contributions to
the philosophy of knowledge and the scientific method. One of his key ideas was the
concept of falsificationism, which challenges the traditional view that scientific
theories can be proven true through empirical evidence. Instead, Popper argued that
scientific theories can only be falsified, but never definitively proven true. Here's a
breakdown of Popper's falsificationism:
1. Falsifiability:
Popper proposed that for a theory to be considered scientific, it must
be potentially falsifiable. This means that it must be possible, at least in
principle, to conceive of evidence that could prove the theory wrong.
According to Popper, theories that cannot be falsified, such as those
that make vague or unfalsifiable claims, are not scientific but belong to
the realm of metaphysics or pseudoscience.
2. Empirical Testing:
Scientists, according to Popper, should not aim to confirm their
theories but rather to test them rigorously by seeking evidence that
could potentially refute or falsify them.
Scientific inquiry involves formulating hypotheses or conjectures and
subjecting them to empirical testing through observation and
experimentation.
If empirical evidence contradicts a theory's predictions or hypotheses,
then the theory is considered falsified or refuted.
3. Deductive Reasoning:
Popper emphasized the role of deductive reasoning in scientific inquiry,
particularly in formulating hypotheses and making predictions.
Scientists use deductive reasoning to derive specific predictions or
hypotheses from general theories or hypotheses. These predictions are
then tested against empirical evidence to determine their validity.
4. Testing Hypotheses:
Popper's approach to scientific inquiry involves formulating hypotheses
that are specific and testable and then subjecting them to rigorous
empirical testing.
When testing hypotheses, scientists strive to design experiments or
observations that have the potential to falsify the hypotheses if they are
incorrect.
5. Falsification and Progress:
Popper argued that science progresses not by accumulating evidence
in support of theories but by eliminating or falsifying false or
inadequate theories.
Each instance of falsification leads to refinement or replacement of
theories, resulting in the advancement of scientific knowledge.
Pseudo-Science: Science that lacks scientific backing (Astrology/horoscope,
Religion etc.)
- Pseudoscience makes claims that cannot be falsified
ChatGPT
Pseudoscience refers to practices, beliefs, or claims that are presented as scientific but lack
empirical evidence, scientific methodology, or credibility within the scientific community.
Pseudoscientific ideas often rely on unfalsifiable claims, lack rigorous testing, and fail to adhere
to the principles of the scientific method. Here's how pseudoscience is characterized and
examples of pseudoscientific beliefs:
Researchers who take the position that research cannot be value-free exercise reflexivity
This involves trying to identify and recognize the impact of their social location—that is,
their gender, age, sexual orientation, ethnicity, education, background, and so on—on the
kind of data that they produce and analyse during their research process. These researchers
argue that our values, preferences, and inclinations can exert an influence at many stages in
the process of social research, including
choice of method;
analysis of data;
interpretation of data;
conclusions.
The researcher’s values can also intrude into their work if they develop an affection or
sympathy for the people being studied.
As we will explore in Chapter 18, this is quite a common issue for researchers working
within a qualitative research strategy, particularly when they use participant observation
or very intensive interviewing, and they can find it difficult to disentangle their stance as
social scientists from their subjects’ perspective(s).
Turnbull (1973) studied an African tribe known as the Ik and perceived them as a loveless
(and for him unlovable) tribe that left its young and very old to die. Turnbull identified the
conditions that had led to this culture but he was very honest about his strong disapproval for
what he witnessed.
This reaction was the result of his Western values about the family, and it is likely, as he
acknowledged, that these values influenced his perception of what he witnessed. He wrote
(Turnbull 1973: 13): ‘the reader is entitled to know something of the aims, expectations,
hopes and attitudes that the writer brought to the field with him, for these will surely
influence not only how he sees things but even what he sees.’
Ethical approaches
- Teleological (utilitarian): Bentham (18th century) & Stuart-Mill (19th)
- Consequentialism: end justifies the means
- Taking the solution that maximises social pleasure, minimises
social pain
The teleological or utilitarian ethical approach, advocated by philosophers such as
Jeremy Bentham and John Stuart Mill, is a consequentialist theory that emphasizes
the outcomes or consequences of actions as the basis for moral judgment.
Utilitarianism holds that the right action is the one that maximizes overall happiness
or pleasure and minimizes suffering or pain for the greatest number of people.
Here's an example to illustrate the utilitarian approach:
Benefits of Vaccination:
Vaccination can prevent the spread of the disease, reducing the number of
infections and associated deaths.
Immunizing a significant portion of the population can lead to herd immunity,
protecting even those who cannot be vaccinated due to medical reasons.
A decline in disease prevalence can alleviate strain on healthcare systems and
reduce healthcare costs.
Economic benefits may be realized through increased productivity and
reduced healthcare expenditures.
Harms of Vaccination:
Some individuals may experience adverse reactions or side effects from the
vaccine, though these are typically rare and outweighed by the benefits.
There may be logistical challenges in distributing and administering vaccines
to large populations.
Public resistance or skepticism towards vaccines may undermine the
effectiveness of the vaccination program.
Utilitarian Analysis: In this scenario, a utilitarian approach would involve weighing the
aggregate benefits and harms of implementing the vaccination policy. If the
expected benefits, such as reduced disease burden, saved lives, and societal well-
being, outweigh the anticipated harms, such as potential side effects and logistical
challenges, then the policy would be deemed ethically justified.
The ethics of care, rooted in feminist theories and developed in the 20th century,
emphasizes the moral significance of relationships, empathy, and responsiveness to
the needs of others. This ethical approach challenges traditional moral theories, such
as deontology and utilitarianism, which prioritize abstract principles or rules. Instead,
the ethics of care highlights the importance of caring and interconnectedness in
moral decision-making. Here's an example to illustrate the ethics of care:
Ethics of Care Analysis: From the perspective of the ethics of care, the family member
would prioritize the relational and compassionate aspects of caregiving in making
this decision. Rather than solely focusing on abstract principles or rules, such as
autonomy or utility, the ethics of care emphasizes the importance of empathy,
compassion, and responsiveness to the needs of the elderly relative and their
caregivers.
In this scenario, the family member would consider factors such as the elderly
relative's preferences, comfort, and well-being, as well as the quality of the caregiving
environment and the availability of support services. The decision-making process
would involve engaging in empathetic understanding, listening to the concerns and
perspectives of all involved parties, and striving to ensure that the chosen course of
action promotes the elderly relative's dignity, autonomy, and quality of life.
Unconscious biases
- Conscious brain operates on facts and data
- Unconscious brain operates on patterns, rules & what is known based on
experience
Such as…
- Affinity bias: positive response to people similar to oneself
- Categorization based on existing ideas: good vs bad
Stereotype threat
- When discriminated against, individual may tend to confirm one’s own
stereotypes (learnt in DP psych)
1. Research Performance:
Researchers who belong to groups that are stereotypically perceived as
less competent or capable in certain domains may experience
stereotype threat when conducting research or presenting their
findings.
Example: A female researcher in a male-dominated field of science may
experience stereotype threat when presenting her research at a
conference. Due to the stereotype that women are less competent in
STEM fields, she may feel pressure to perform exceptionally well to
prove her competence, leading to heightened anxiety and potentially
impacting her presentation skills or confidence.
2. Perceptions of Competence:
Stereotype threat can influence how researchers perceive their own
competence and abilities, which may impact their willingness to pursue
certain research topics or career opportunities.
Example: A researcher from an ethnic minority background may
internalize stereotypes about their group's intellectual abilities. As a
result, they may hesitate to apply for prestigious research grants or
academic positions, fearing that they will be judged based on negative
stereotypes rather than their qualifications and achievements.
3. Retention and Advancement:
Stereotype threat can contribute to feelings of alienation, imposter
syndrome, and reduced sense of belonging among researchers from
underrepresented groups, which may hinder their retention and career
advancement in academia.
Example: A researcher who is the only person of color in their
department may experience stereotype threat, feeling pressure to
represent their entire racial group. This heightened awareness of their
minority status and the fear of confirming negative stereotypes may
lead to feelings of isolation and disengagement from academic
activities, ultimately impacting their career progression.
4. Bias in Peer Review:
Stereotype threat can also affect the evaluation of research and
scholarly contributions, with reviewers potentially being influenced by
stereotypes when assessing the work of researchers from marginalized
groups.
Example: A study conducted by Moss-Racusin et al. (2012) found
evidence of gender bias in peer review, where identical research papers
were rated lower when attributed to female authors compared to male
authors. This bias may be influenced by stereotypes about women's
competence and credibility in academic settings, contributing to
disparities in publication rates and career advancement.
Positionality:
- Unique social, cultural, and personal perspectives that shape an
individual's understanding and interpretation of the world around them.
- Plays a role in forming biases/ideas: acknowledging one's positionality
involves recognizing how personal background, beliefs, and experiences
might shape the research process and findings.
Positionality is a concept that acknowledges the influence of an individual's social, cultural, and
personal perspectives on their understanding and interpretation of the world, including their
approach to academic research. In the context of conducting academic research, positionality
recognizes that researchers bring their own unique backgrounds, experiences, values, and biases
to the research process, which can shape the formulation of research questions, the design of
studies, the interpretation of findings, and the dissemination of results. Here's how positionality
operates in academic research: