0% found this document useful (0 votes)
18 views25 pages

زهراء علي وزهراء قاسم

The document discusses types of human memory, including long-term memory, short-term memory, and sensory memory. It notes that long-term memory can be divided into explicit memory (declarative memory) and implicit memory. Explicit memory includes episodic memory and semantic memory. Short-term memory persists for about 18-30 seconds before information is transferred to long-term memory for indefinite storage. Sensory memory involves initial perception of sensory information.

Uploaded by

Mariam Salah
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
18 views25 pages

زهراء علي وزهراء قاسم

The document discusses types of human memory, including long-term memory, short-term memory, and sensory memory. It notes that long-term memory can be divided into explicit memory (declarative memory) and implicit memory. Explicit memory includes episodic memory and semantic memory. Short-term memory persists for about 18-30 seconds before information is transferred to long-term memory for indefinite storage. Sensory memory involves initial perception of sensory information.

Uploaded by

Mariam Salah
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 25

Ministry of Higher Education

And Scientific Research


University of Al-Qadissiya
College of Education
Department of English

Activated Memory in Language Learning

Submitted By

Zahraa Qassem Sarhan Zahraa Ali

Supervised By

Lect. Ghosoon Mahdi


Dedication

To our dear parents, sisters and brothers.

To all our friends with whom we shared the university life.

iii
Acknowledgements

We owe an immense debt of gratitude to our supervisor Lect.


Ghosoon Mahdi since her sound advice and careful guidance
contributed to the completion of this paper.

We also would like to express our heartfelt gratitude and


appreciation to our friends for providing the needed advice and
encouragement.

Finally, we would remember without mentioning our family for


their love and support.

iv
Contents

Dedication iii

Acknowledgement iv

Contents v-vi

Abstract vii

1. Definitions of Memory 1

2. Types of Human Memory 3

2.1 Long-Term Memory 4

2.1.1 Explicit memory 5

2.1.2 Implicit Memory 5

2.2 Short-Term Memory 5

2.3 Sensory Memory 6

3. Information Processing 7

3.1 Information Processing Stages 8

V
3.2 Working Memory 9

4. Memory and Language Learning 10

4.1 CLT and Memory Activation 11

4.2 Interaction and Activating Memory 13

Conclusion 16

Bibliography 17

Vi
Abstract
Memory is the remembering or recognition of whether or not an item or
event has occurred previously in the past or has not. Students' ability to learn a
second language differs from each other, and each student learns a material better
in a special method. These differences rely on students' ability, or to be specially
mentioned in their memory.
Memory is a built store that must be accessed in some way in order for
effective recall or retrieval to occur. This definition is based on the belief that
memory is a multi-faceted, if not multi-staged, the system of connections and
representations that encompass a lifetime’s accumulation of perceptions.
The paper is divided into four sections. The first section deals with the
definition of memory while the second section examines the components of
memory and the different views about the relationships among working memory,
long-term memory, short-term memory and language processing. The third section
deals with the information processing stages. The final part of the study explores
the effect of the classroom on memory activation. The fourth part studies the
relationship between memory, language learning and the effect of interaction on
language learning. Finally, the conclusion sums up the findings of the study.

Vii
1. Definitions of Memory

Eliasmith (2001:1) defines memory as the “general ability, or faculty, that


enables us to interpret the perceptual world to help organize responses to changes
that take place in the world”. This indicates that there must be a tangible structure
in which to incorporate new stimuli into memory. The form of this structure has
been the source of much debate, and there seems to be no absolute agreement on
what shape a memory structure actually takes, but there are many theories on what
constitutes both the memory structure and the knowledge unit.

Schacter and Tulving (as cited in Driscoll, 2001:283) state that “a memory
system is defined in terms of its brain mechanisms, the kind of information it
processes, and the principles of its operation”. Memory is a built store that must be
accessed in some way in order for effective recall or retrieval to occur. This
definition is based on the belief that memory is a multi-faceted, if not multi-staged,
the system of connections and representations that encompass a lifetime’s
accumulation of perceptions.

In other word, “the amount of previously learned material that has been
retained.” The memory could be referred to as retention. Retention is another name
for memory. There are two basic forms of retention: procedural and declarative.
Procedural memory is how to perform an action, in sequence. Athletic skills
represent an example of procedural memory. One learns the fundamentals, practice
them over and over, and then they seem to flow naturally when we are in a game.
Rehearsing for a dancing or musical performance would be other examples of
procedural memory. Everyday examples might be remembering how to tie our
shoes, drive a car, or get on the internet. Declarative memory involves memory for
facts, concepts and events rather than muscular procedures. Declarative memory is
either episodic or Semantic. Semantic retention is the type of knowledge people
associate with books and school: names, dates, and numbers. But it can also
include personal information, such as telephone numbers and addresses
(Cowan,2001:45).

Stiggins (2002:3) mentions that there is general agreement among most


cognitive psychologists on some basic principles of the information processing
system. First, there is the “assumption of a limited capacity”, these limitations
occur at different points in information processing, but it is widely held in all
models that there are limitations as to how much new information can be encoded,
sorted and retrieved.

Language input moves toward becoming intake or part of the fleeting


memory of the student. This is intentionally taken care of and learned by the
student. Whenever structures and vocabulary move toward becoming uptake, some
portion of the long-term memory, it is viewed as intuitive and obtained. The uptake
is the capability level of the student (Cowan,2001:55).

At the point when students take a test on the new material and do well, it is
on account of they are being evaluated on their transient memory (intake). At the
point when new material tags along and that more seasoned materials have not
proceeded onward to long-term memory (uptake) it is supplanted by the more up to
date material. That is the reason the sentence structure they knew so well for the
test isn't as effectively created a couple of days after the fact and the reason one
needs to invest so much energy assessing for last, most decisive tests toward the
finish of the school year. The more introduction students need to include that is
conceivable to them the more probable the language will move toward becoming
uptake and advance toward the long-term memory. Once more, this is, for the most
part, a subliminal procedure in which language is procured so intelligible inout is
the best device. Memory strategies can be utilized successfully for language
adapting, yet they require more than an information of the systems themselves:
they require a learning of language (Atkinson,1986:82).

2. Types of Human Memory

Driscoll (2001:283) states that “a memory system is defined in terms of its


brain mechanisms, the kind of information it processes, and the principles of its
operation”. So , memory means the total of all mental experiences. In this light,
memory is a built store that must be accessed in some way in order for effective
recall or retrieval to occur. Memory is a having many facets, if not multi-staged,
system of connections and representations that encompass a lifetime’s
accumulation of perceptions.

Eliasmith (2001:1) defines memory as the “general ability, or faculty, that


enables us to interpret the perceptual world to help organize responses to changes
that take place in the world”. By this definition, one can understand that there must
be a sensible structure in which to incorporate new stimuli into memory. The form
of this structure has been the source of much debate, and there seems to be no
absolute agreement on what shape a memory structure actually.

2.1 Long-Term Memory


Long-term memory (LTM) is the stage of the Atkinson–Shiffrin memory
model where informative knowledge is held indefinitely. It is defined in contrast to
short-term and working memory, that persist for only about 18 to 30 seconds.
Long-term memory is commonly labelled as explicit memory (declarative), as well
as episodic memory, semantic memory, autobiographical memory, and implicit
memory (Atkinson,1986:89).

After passing through the short-term memory, relevant information is moved


to long-term storage. At this stage, the brain is less likely to forget important
details. At the same time, the long-term memory can diminish over time if one
doesn't refresh his knowledge. (Morris et al,1977:518).

As opposed to the previous memory constructs, long-term memory has


unlimited space. The crucial factor of long-term memory is how well organized the
information is. This is affected by proper encoding (elaboration processes in
transferring to long-term memory) and retrieval processes (scanning memory for
the information and transferring into working memory so that it could be used)
(Ibid.).

Abbot (2002:1) suggests that long-term memory “is that more permanent
store in which information can reside in a dormant state out of mind and unused
until you fetch it back into consciousness”. In order to incorporate new
information, long-term memory must be in communication with the short-term
memory. There are several categories of long-term memory, and there are many
suggestions as to how memory units are represented in the mind.
Long-Term Memory divided into the following :

2.1.1 Explicit memory

Brown, G. D. A., Preece, (2000:250) admit that explicit memory (declarative


memory) means all memories that are consciously available. These are encoded by
the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, and perirhinal cortex, but consolidated and
stored elsewhere. The precise location of storage is unknown, but the temporal
cortex has been proposed as a likely candidate.

2.1.2 Implicit Memory

Implicit memory (procedural memory) refers to the use of objects or


movements of the body, such as how exactly to use a pencil, drive a car, or ride a
bicycle. This type of memory is encoded and it is presumed stored in the striatum
and other parts of the basal ganglia. Which is believed to mediate procedural
memory and other brain structures and is largely independent of the hippocampus
(Foerde and Poldrack, 2009:1083).

Manelis and Hanson (2011:32) find that the reactivation of the parietal and
occipital regions was associated with implicit memory. Procedural memory is
considered non-declarative memory or unconscious memory which includes
priming and non-associative learning.

2.2 Short-Term Memory

Is the stage that falls under the specialization of working memory, which
temporarily stores information when it is triggered by different stimuli. Short-term
memory can only hold a maximum of (7) items at one time. The time limit, of
short memory which is usually between (10) seconds to a minute. Short-term
memory: A system for temporarily storing and managing information required to
carry out complex cognitive tasks such as learning, reasoning, and comprehension.
Short-term memory is involved in the selection, initiation, and termination of
information-processing functions such as encoding, storing and retrieving data
(Atkinson and Shiffrin,1968:89).

In a typical test of memory span, an examiner reads a list of random


numbers aloud at about the rate of one number per second. At the end of a
sequence, the person being tested is asked to recall the items in order. The average
memory span for normal adults is(7).Short-term memory is also termed recent or
working memory (Ibid.).

3.2 Sensory Memory

When someone senses are triggered by a stimulus, his brain briefly stores the
information. For example, one smells freshly baked bread and can only remember
its scent for a few seconds before it vanishes. Even though the bread is no longer in
front of him, people mind still holds onto its impression for a short period. The
brain then has the option to process it through the memory banks or forget about it.
In learning, sensory memory is triggered by a visually compelling

image, background music, or any other element that utilizes the senses (Adams,
A. M. and Gathercole,2000:241).

In sensory memory, information is gathered via the senses through a process


called transduction, by receptor cell activity, it is altered into a form of information
that the brain could process. These memories, usually unconscious, last for a very
short amount of time, ranging up to three seconds. Our senses are constantly
bombarded with large amounts of information. Our sensory memory acts as a
filter, by focusing on what is important, and forgetting what is unnecessary.
Sensory information catches our attention, and thus progresses into working
memory, only if it is seen as relevant, or is familiar (Baddeley,2001:581).

3. Information Processing

Information processing is the change of information in any manner


detectable by an observer. In processing describes everything that happens in the
universe, from the falling of a rock to the printing of a text file from a digital
computer system. In the latter case, an information processor is changing the form
of presentation of that text file. Information processing may more specifically be
defined as the conversion of latent information into manifest information
(McGonigle and Mastrian, 2011:56).

Information processes are executed by information processors. For a given


information processor, whether physical or biological, a token is an object, devoid
of meaning, that the processor recognizes as being totally different from other
tokens. A group of such unique tokens recognized by a processor constitutes its
basic “alphabet”; for example, the dot, dash, and space constitute the basic token
alphabet of a Morse-code processor. Objects that carry meaning are represented by
patterns of tokens called symbols. (Huitt, 2000:38).

Again, exactly how and where the controls operate is a question of some
debate, but the actuality of some type of system that requires some processing
capacity is generally accepted. The belief in the interaction of new information
with stored information is a third key point of the cognitive study. This is usually
demonstrated with a bottom-up or top-down system or a combination of the two. A
bottom-up system is predicated on the belief that new information is seen as an
initiator which the brain attempts to match with existing concepts in order to break
down characteristics or defining attributes (Gibson, 1979:316).

After that, infants begin to vocalize the sounds of the mother tongue and
omit sounds not found in that language. It has also been discovered that infants
begin to lose the ability to discriminate sounds not in the mother tongue at about
six to seven months of age. All of these factors play a significant role in the
development and understanding of how the mind operates (Jusczyk, 1997:58).

3.1 Information Processing Stages

Anderson and Bower (1973:13) propose there are many suggestions that the
learning process involves three key stages:

In first stage Input, the brain is exposed to stimuli, at which point it analyzes
and evaluates the information. For example, the online learner reads a passage and
determines whether it's worth remembering (Ibid.).
In the second stage output, storage the brain stores the information for later
use. It also adds it to our mental schema and encodes it. If the information is not
reinforced, the brain may simply forget it over time (Ibid:15).

The third stage output, the brain decides what it's going to do with the
information and how it will react to the stimulus. For example, after reading the
passage, the individual uses the information they learned to overcome a challenge
(Ibid.).

3.2 Working Memory

Working memory is a cognitive system with a restricted ability that is


responsible for temporarily holding information available for processing. Working
memory is important for reasoning and the guidance of decision-making and
behaviour. Working memory is used synonymously with short-term memory, but
some theorists consider the two forms of memory distinct, working memory allows
for the manipulation of stored information, whereas short-term memory only refers
to the short-term storage of information. Working memory is a theoretical concept
central to cognitive psychology, neuropsychology, and neuroscience. Working
memory is considered to have limited capacity (Malenka and Nestler,2009:313).

Miyake(1999:56) mentions that working memory means the capacity to


perform some complex tasks depends critically on the ability to retain task-relevant
information in an accessible state over time and to selectively process information
in the environment. In addition, one must be able to selectively attend to the
relevant objects because there is more information in a scene that can be processed
by individuals perceptual systems. The contents of working memory and attention
often, interfere. If the directions stored in working memory guide someone to turn
left after the yellow water tower, then attention may be guided towards objects that
resemble a yellow water tower.

In general, memory span for verbal contents (digits, letters, words, etc.)
depends on the phonological complexity of the content (i.e., the number of
phonemes, the number of syllables), and on the lexical status of the contents
(whether the contents are words known to the person or not). Several other factors
affect a person's measured span, and therefore it is difficult to pin down the
capacity of short-term or working memory to a number of chunks. Nonetheless,
Cowan proposed that working memory has a capacity of about four chunks in
young adults (and fewer in children and old adults) (Cowan,2001:87).

Working memory is a system for temporarily storing and managing the


information required to carry out complex cognitive tasks such as learning,
reasoning, and comprehension. Working memory is involved in the selection,
initiation, and termination of information-processing functions such as encoding,
storing and retrieving data (Service,1998:283).

4. Memory and Language Learning

The medial temporal region is richly connected to widespread areas of the


cerebral cortex, including the regions responsible for thinking and language.
Whereas the medial temporal region is important for forming, organizing,
consolidating, and retrieving a memory, cortical areas are important for the
long-term storage of knowledge about facts and events and for how this knowledge
is used in everyday situations ( Green and Oxford,1995:261).
To serve these functions, the prefrontal cortex also interacts with a large
network of posterior cortical areas that encode, maintain, and retrieve specific
types of information, such as visual images, sounds, and words, as well as where
important events occurred and much more (Anderson,1976:471).

Cairns et al (2011:274). points out the importance of memory and its role in
learning the language. Working memory provides the temporary storage that is
very useful in language comprehension process. In language production, working
memory becomes the place where the pronunciations of the words are put in a
linear order on the basis of the syntactic and semantic relations in the intended
utterance before the construction of a motor program that produces the utterance.

Moreover, through the phonological loop, the new vocabulary is being


stored and rehearsed in working memory before it transfers to the long-term
memory. This information is very useful to second language teachers in formal
class and also to the independent learner who tries to learn the language. In this
case, they can maximize the function of memory in learning language (Ibid.).

4.1 CLT and Memory Activation

Communicative language teaching ( hence CLT ) first emerged in European


countries in the 1970s and was successfully implemented into the English
curriculum in other countries in the 1980s. Following the emergence of CLT in
English-speaking nations, CLT is the most influential language teaching
methodology in the world (Ying, 2010:179).

Due to the needs of international communication in the early 1990s, the CLT
approach was introduced in many countries where English is learnt and spoken as a
foreign language in EFL classrooms.It aims to develop students ability to
communicate meaningfully and to build on accumulated classroom experience as
well as current language teaching theory. More specifically, it attempts to combine
the broader view of communicative competence that formed a major impetus to the
development of CLT (Ying, 2010:67).

Communicative activities include any activities that encourage and require a


learner to speak with and listen to other learners, as well as with people in the
program and community. Communicative activities have real purposes: to find
information, break down barriers, talk about self, and learn about the culture. Even
when a lesson is focused on developing reading or writing skills, communicative
activities should be integrated into the lesson. Furthermore, research on second
language acquisition (SLA) suggests that more learning takes place when students
are engaged in relevant tasks within a dynamic learning environment rather than in
traditional teacher-led classes (Ibid.).

CLT has become popular and widespread in second foreign language


teaching, it emphasises the process of communication and leads learners to roles
different from the traditional approach. The role of the learner is a negotiator
between the self, the learning process, and the object of learning. Learners should
be actively engaged in negotiating meaning within the classroom procedures and
activities (Richards and Rodgers, 2001:78).

Teachers’ roles in the communicative approach are to facilitate the


communication process between all participants in the classrooms. The teacher is
also a co-communicator who engages in communicative activities with the
students. In addition, the teacher acts as an analyst, counsellor, and group process
manager. In the CLT classroom, it seems to be student-centred which does not
depend on their teachers all the time, waiting for instructions, words of approval,
correction, advice or praise (Chang, 2011:17).

Similarly, Ozsevik (2010:24) argues that learners in CLT classrooms are


supposed to participate in classroom activities that are based on a collaborative,
rather than individualistic, approach to learning. They are portrayed as active
participants in the language learning process.

The emphasis in communicative language teaching in the processes of


communication, rather than mastery of language forms, leads to different roles for
learners from those found in a more traditional second language classroom (Breen
and Candlin, 1980 : 110).

Active learning instructional strategies can be created and used to engage


students in (a) thinking critically or creatively, (b) speaking with a partner, in a
small group, or with the entire class, (c) expressing ideas through writing, (d)
exploring personal attitudes and values, (e) giving and receiving feedback, and (f)
reflecting upon the learning process .It should also be noted that active learning
instructional strategies can (a) be completed by students either in-class or
out-of-class, (b) be done by students working either as individuals or in group, and
(c) be done either with or without the use of technology tools (Vogel, and
Machizawa,2004:1)

4.2 Interaction and Activating Memory

Interaction is foundational to effective learning. Often, however, the term


interaction is used without a clear understanding. The goal of interaction is to lead
students to a point of reflection that causes them to evaluate existing assumptions
and then choose to integrate or discard the new information. By itself, interaction
has very little value. It is possible to interact at length with concepts, only to find
that everything read or heard is a blur because active engagement is defined by
reflection and validation of the content being explored. Effective interaction, then,
is a process of awakening a students' internal reflective processes.

Interaction can be defined as: " Interactions occur when these objects and
events mutually influence one another. An instructional interaction is an event that
takes place between a learner and the learner's environment. Its purpose is to
respond to the learner in a way intended to change his or her behaviour toward an
educational goal. Instructional interactions have two purposes: to change learners
and to move them toward achieving their goals." (Alvermann and Phelps,
2005:131)

One way for students to shoulder the responsibility for learning is for them
to be the readers, writers, speakers, listeners, and thinkers in the classroom through
active engagement in social interaction with others (Vacca, Vacca, and Mraz,
2011:57).

Francis (1999:262)admits that interaction between students can include


collaboration and increase students' comfort with each other and with the learning
environment. For example Student↔student interaction-based activities which
are: group projects, group case studies, peer instruction, role playing, synchronous
or asynchronous discussions or debates, collaborative brainstorming, and peer
review of selected work (For more on using Canvas tools to manage peer review.

Any of these examples can be used on a large or a small scale ranging from
semester-long project groups doing research and presenting results to an
optional live meeting where those present discuss a short video case or a
discussion forum where they brainstorm alternatives to a textbook
problem(Ibid.).

Kirby(1999:205) states that depending on the size of the class, the teacher
can encourage student↔student interaction class-wide or in smaller groups or
pairs. When working with smaller groups, it helps to emphasize individual
accountability, positive interdependence, and positive interaction in grading the
group's work. This strategy leads to three grades on a group project emphasizing
the three aspects of group work: Individual contribution to the group projec,
synthesis of the individual parts into a project that shows collaboration, consensus,
and learning and working together to encourage and facilitate each other's efforts
to complete the project
Conclusion

The last decade has witnessed an important step forward in understanding


the relationship between memory and language and how these key features of the
human cognitive system interact. There is a strong interconnected relationship
among the working memory, short-term memory, long-term memory and the
information processing in language acquisition and learning. The study of memory
at large is of extreme importance to the understanding of the human mind.

There are in fact many ways of learning and studying so that more than just
intellect is involved, where remembering is the natural outcome, both within the
classroom and in the real world. the successful learners are the ones who are
engaged in activities that thoroughly engage them. Along with a decent working
memory, learners need to be able to commit what they have learned to their
long-term memory for it to be of any use.

Native and foreign language acquisition appears to be achieved through the


ability to represent serial-order information in working memory, while language
perception and production rely on intentional control functions. The debate
continues about the extent to which each of these factors has the crucial impact on
the process of acquisition and learning language.
Bibliography

Abbot, B. (2002). Human memory. Cambridge, Massachusetts.: The MIT Press.

Adams, A. and Gathercole, S. (2000). “Limitations in Working Memory:


Implications For Language Development. New York: New York University
Press.

Almasi, J., and Gambrell, L.. (1997). Conflict During Classroom Discussions
Can be A Good Thing. New York: International Reading Association.

Anderson, J., and Bower, G. (1973). Human Associative Memory. Washington,


Anderson, M. (1976). Educational Psychology. Portland, OR: Cortland
College.

Atkinson, R., and Shiffrin, R. (1968). Human Memory: A Proposed System and Its
Control Processes. New York: Academic Press.

Baddeley, A. (2001). Preserved Recognition in A Case of Developmental Amnesia:


Implications for The Acquisition of Semantic Memory? New York:
New York University Press.

Breen, M. and Candlin, C. N. (1980). The Essentials of A Communicative


Curriculum in Language Teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T., and Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-Based Memory for
Serial Order. New York: Academic Press.

Chang, M. (2011). EFL Teachers’ Attitudes Toward Communicative Language


Teaching in Taiwanese College. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Cowan, N. (2001). The Magical Number 4 in Short-Term Memory: A


Reconsideration of Mental Storage Capacity. London: Longman.
Driscoll, M. (2001). Psychology of Learning for Assessment (2nd ed). Boston:
Allyn and Bacon.

Eliasmith ,Chris (2001). An Introduction to Contemporary Theories of Content


New York :Oxford University Press.

Francis, W. (1999). Cognitive Integration of Language and Memory in


Bilinguals: Semantic Representation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Gibson, J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston:


Houghton Mifflin.

Green, J. and Oxford, R. (1995). A Closer Look At Learning Strategies, L2


Proficiency, and Gender. Belmont : Wadsworth.

Huitt, W. (2000). The Information Processing Approach. Educational Psychology


Interactive. Valdosta, GA: Valdosta State University.

Jackendoff , Ray(2003). Foundations of Language Brain, Meaning, Grammar,


Evolution .Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Jusczyk, P. (1997). The Discovery of Spoken Language. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Kirby, E. (1999). Building Interaction in Online and Distance Education Courses.


Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Malenka RC, Nestler EJ, Hyman SE (2009). Sydor A, Brown RY, ed. Molecular
Neuropharmacology: A Foundation for Clinical Neuroscience (2 nd ed.). New
York: McGraw-Hill Medical.
McGonigle, Dee and Mastrian ,Kathleen(2011) Nursing Informatics and The
Foundation of Knowledge .UK: Macmillan Education.

Miyake, A., and Shah, P. (Eds.) (1999). Models of Working Memory: Mechanisms
of Active Maintenance and Executive Control. New York: Cambridge
University Press.

Morris, N., Buck-Rolland, C., and Gagne, M. (2002). From Bricks to Bytes:
Faculty and Student Perspectives of Online Graduate Nursing Courses.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Oxford, R. (1990). Language Learning Strategies: What Every Teacher SHOULD


Know. Boston: Heinle and Heinle.

Ozsevik, Z. (2010). The Use of Communicative Language Teaching (CLT):


Turkish EFL Teachers’ Perceived Difficulties in Implementing CLT in
Turkey. Unpublished master dissertation, University of Illinois.

Richards, J. and Rodgers, T. S. (Ed.). (2001). Approaches and Methods in


Language Teaching (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Schacter, D. and Tulving, E. (1994). What Are The Memory Systems of 1994? In
D. L. Schacter and E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Service, E. (1998). Phonology, Working Memory, and Foreign-Language
Learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Striggins, R. (2002). Assessment Crisis: The Absence of Assessment For Learning.
London : longman.
Vacca, R. and Vacca, J.L., and Mraz, M. (2011). Content Area Reading: Literacy
and Learning Across The Curriculum (10th ed.). Boston: Pearson.

Vogel, E. and Machizawa, M. G. (2004). Neural Activity Predicts Individual

You might also like