Reading Rockets
Reading Rockets
The development of oral language is one of the child's most natural – and impressive – accomplishments. Get an introduction to when and how
language is learned.
This article presents an overview of the process and mechanics of language development, along with implications for practice.
The environment itself is also a significant factor. Children learn the specific variety of language (dialect) that the important people around them
speak.
Children do not, however, learn only by imitating those around them. We know that children work through linguistic rules on their own because
they use forms that adults never use, such as "I goed there before" or "I see your feets." Children eventually learn the conventional forms, went
and feet, as they sort out for themselves the exceptions to the rules of English syntax.
As with learning to walk, learning to talk requires time for development and practice in everyday situations. Constant correction of a child's
speech is usually unproductive.
Children seem born not just to speak, but also to interact socially. Even before they use words, they use cries and gestures to convey meaning;
they often understand the meanings that others convey. The point of learning language and interacting socially, then, is not to master rules, but to
make connections with other people and to make sense of experiences (Wells, 1986).
In summary, language occurs through an interaction among genes (which hold innate tendencies to communicate and be sociable), environment,
and the child's own thinking abilities.
As with other aspects of development, language acquisition is not predictable. One child may say her first word at 10 months, another at 20
months. One child may use complex sentences at 5 1/2 years, another at 3 years.
The phonological component involves the rules for combining sounds. Speakers of English, for example, know that an English word can end,
but not begin, with an -ng sound. We are not aware of our knowledge of these rules, but our ability to understand and pronounce English words
demonstrates that we do know a vast number of rules.
The semantic component is made up of morphemes, the smallest units of meaning that may be combined with each other to make up words(for
example, paper + s are the two morphemes that make up papers), and sentences (Brown, 1973). A dictionary contains the semantic component of
a language, but also what words (and meanings) are important to the speakers of the language.
The syntactic component consists of the rules that enable us to combine morphemes into sentences. As soon as a child uses two morphemes
together, as in "more cracker," she is using a syntactic rule about how morphemes are combined to convey meaning.
Like the rules making up the other components, syntactic rules become increasingly complex as the child develops. From combining two
morphemes, the child goes on to combine words with suffixes or inflections (-s or -ing, as in papers and eating) and eventually creates questions,
statements, commands, etc. She also learns to combine two ideas into one complex sentence, as in "I'll share my crackers if you share your
juice."
Of course speakers of a language constantly use these three components of language together, usually in social situations. Some language experts
would add a fourth component: pragmatics, which deals with rules of language use.
Pragmatic rules are part of our communicative competence, our ability to speak appropriately in different situations, for example, in a
conversational way at home and in a more formal way at a job interview. Young children need to learn the ways of speaking in the day care
center or school where, for example, teachers often ask rhetorical questions. Learning pragmatic rules is as important as learning the rules of the
other components of language, since people are perceived and judged based on both what they say and when they say it.
However, if a child appears not to hear what others say to her; if family members and those closest to her find her difficult to understand; or if she
is noticeably different in her communicative abilities from those in her age range, adults may want to seek advice from specialists in children's
speech, language and hearing.
Teachers can help sustain natural language development by providing environments full of language development opportunities. Here are some
general guidelines for teachers, parents, and other caregivers:
Understand that every child's language or dialect is worthy of respect as a valid system for communication. It reflects the identities, values,
and experiences of the child's family and community.
Treat children as if they are conversationalists, even if they are not yet talking. Children learn very early about how conversations work
(taking turns, looking attentively, using facial experiences with conversing adults.
Encourage interaction among children. Peer learning is an important part of language development, especially in mixed-age groups.
Activities involving a wide range of materials should promote talk. There should be a balance between individual activities and those that
nurture collaboration and discussion, such as dramatic play, block-building, book-sharing, or carpentry.
Remember that parents, caregivers, teachers, and guardians are the chief resources in language development. Children learn much from
each other, but adults are the main conversationalists, questioners, listeners, responders, and sustainers of language development and
growth in the child-care center or classroom.
Continue to encourage interaction as children come to understand written language. Children in the primary grades can keep developing
oral abilities and skills by consulting with each other, raising questions, and providing information in varied situations. Every area of the
curriculum is enhanced through language, so that classrooms full of active learners are hardly ever silent.
References
References
Click the "References" link above to hide these references.
Brown, R. A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard, 1973.
Cazden, C.B., Ed. Language in Early Childhood Education. Washington, DC: NAEYC, 1981.
Fletcher, P., and M. Garman, Eds. Language Acquisition, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge, 1986.
Genishi, C., Children's Language: Learning Words from Experience. Young Children 44 (Nov., 1988): 16-23.
Genishi, C. and A. Haas Dyson. Language Assessment in the Early Years. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 1984.
Heath, S.B. Ways with Words: Language, Life, and Work in Communities and Classrooms. New York: Cambridge, 1983.
Hough, R.A., J.R. Nurss and D. Wood. "Tell Me a Story: Making Opportunities for Elaborated Language in Early Childhood Classrooms."
Young Children 43 (Nov., 1987): 6-12.
Lindfors, J.W. Children's Language and Learning, 2nd ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1987.
Wells, G. The Meaning Makers: Children Learning Language and Using Language to Learn. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann, 1986.
Genishi, C. (1998). Young Children's Oral Language Development. ERIC Digest. ERIC Clearinghouse on Elementary and Early Childhood
Education.
Reprints
You are welcome to print copies for non-commercial use, or a limited number for educational purposes, as long as credit is given to Reading
Rockets and the author(s). For commercial use, please contact the author or publisher listed.
Related Topics
Early Literacy Development
Oral Language
Speech, Language, and Hearing
"You may have tangible wealth untold. Caskets of jewels and coffers of gold. Richer than I you can never be — I had a mother who read to
me." — Strickland Gillilan