A Grammar of The Great Andamanese Language
A Grammar of The Great Andamanese Language
Series Editors
John Peterson, University of Kiel
Anju Saxena, Uppsala University
Eitorial Board
Anvita Abbi, Jawaharlal Nehru University
Balthasar Bickel, University of Zurich
George Cardona, University of Pennsylvania
Carol Genetti, University of California, Santa Barbara
Geoffrey Haig, University of Bamberg
Gilbert Lazard, cnrs & École Pratique des Hautes Études
Harold F. Schiffman, University of Pennsylvania
Udaya Narayana Singh, Visva-Bharati, Shantiniketan, India
VOLUME 4
By
Anvita Abbi
LEIDEN • BOSTON
2013
Cover illustration: Strait Island, Andaman Islands. Picture courtesy of the author.
PL7501.A6A34 2013
495.9—dc23
2013018345
This publication has been typeset in the multilingual “Brill” typeface. With over 5,100 characters
covering Latin, IPA, Greek, and Cyrillic, this typeface is especially suitable for use in the
humanities. For more information, please see www.brill.com/brill-typeface.
ISSN 1877-4083
ISBN 978-90-04-23527-4 (hardback)
ISBN 978-90-04-24612-6 (e-book)
All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, translated, stored in
a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical,
photocopying, recording or otherwise, without prior written permission from the publisher.
Authorization to photocopy items for internal or personal use is granted by Koninklijke Brill NV
provided that the appropriate fees are paid directly to The Copyright Clearance Center,
222 Rosewood Drive, Suite 910, Danvers, MA 01923, USA.
Fees are subject to change.
Acknowledgements ......................................................................................... xv
Preface . ............................................................................................................... xvii
List of Maps, Figures and Tables ................................................................ xxi
Abbreviations and Symbols .......................................................................... xxv
VI. Possession
Introduction . ............................................................................................. 137
6.1 Possessive Classification ............................................................ 137
6.2 Primary Possession ...................................................................... 138
6.2.1 Body Part Terms ............................................................ 139
6.2.1.1 Mouth Cavity (a=) . ....................................... 140
6.2.1.2 Major External Body Parts (ɛr= ~ er=) . .. 141
6.2.1.3 Extremities of the Body (uŋ= ~ oŋ=) ....... 142
6.2.1.4 External Body Products or Extension
(ɔt= ~ ut= ~ ot=) ............................................. 143
6.2.1.5 Internal Organs (e= ~ i-) . ............................ 144
6.2.1.6 Nodular or Curved Structure (ara=, ra=) . .. 145
6.2.1.7 Lower Parts of Body (o= ~ ɔ=) . ................. 146
6.2.2 Is There a Hierarchy? ................................................... 148
6.3 Kinship Terms ............................................................................... 149
6.3.1 Parallels between the Body Part Terminology and
Kinship Terms ................................................................ 150
6.4 The Twin Levels . .......................................................................... 150
6.5 Secondary Possession . ................................................................ 151
6.5.1 Juxtaposition/Compounds .......................................... 151
6.5.2 Complex Structures . ..................................................... 152
6.5.2.1 Lexical Compounding with
Class Markers . ................................................ 152
6.5.2.2 Double Marking and Clitic Sequencing . 153
6.5.2.3 Adverbial Function ....................................... 153
6.5.2.4 Double Class Markers .................................. 155
6.5.2.5 Summary .......................................................... 156
6.6 Animate vs. Inanimate Possessor and the Semantics of
Inalienability . ................................................................................ 156
6.6.1. Inanimate ......................................................................... 156
6.6.2 Alienated but Inherent ................................................ 157
6.6.3 Part-to-Whole or Part-to-Component ..................... 159
6.6.4 Inalienable Possessed Nouns: A Conspectus . ....... 160
6.7 Alienable Nouns ........................................................................... 163
6.7.1 Possessed Nouns ............................................................ 163
6.7.2 Twelve Different Varieties . ......................................... 163
6.8. Attributive Modification and Possession . ............................ 165
6.9. The “possessive” Relationship: The ‘Have’ Construction ..... 166
6.10 Conclusion . .................................................................................... 167
xii contents
X. Syntactic Organisation
Introduction . ............................................................................................. 243
10.1 Word Order .................................................................................... 243
10.1.1 Adjectives and Nouns ................................................. 243
10.1.2 Genitive ........................................................................... 245
10.1.3 Case Markers ................................................................. 245
10.1.4 Numerals and Quantifiers ......................................... 246
10.1.5 Degree Words ................................................................ 246
10.1.6 Manner Adverbs ........................................................... 247
10.1.7 Temporal Adverbs . ...................................................... 247
10.1.8 Negative Verb ................................................................ 248
10.1.9 Relative Markers ........................................................... 248
10.1.10 Interrogatives . ............................................................... 249
10.1.11 Adpositional Phrases . ................................................. 249
10.1.12 Variability in Word Order . ........................................ 250
10.1.13 Deviation from the Standard SOV Pattern . ......... 250
10.1.14 Variability in Proclitics ............................................... 250
xiv contents
Books like this one cannot be completed without the assistance and
generosity of the funding agency, viz. ELAR, SOAS, University of London,
which granted me funds for the mega project Vanishing Voices of the Great
Andamanese (VOGA) www.andamanese.net and http://elar.soas.ac.uk/
deposit/abbi2006greatandamanese.
This grammar was written at three different places while I was on long
leave from my university. I would like to express my gratitude to Bernard
Comrie who provided me the right atmosphere at the Max Planck Insti-
tute of Evolutionary Anthropology, Germany where I commenced writing
this grammar. I am thankful to Alexandra Aikhenvald who invited me as
a Visiting Professor at James Cook University at Cairns, Australia so that
I could work on this grammar. I am grateful to her and Robert Dixon for
the discussion on the main tenets of the grammar of Great Andamanese.
I am also thankful to Barbara Lotz for providing me a peaceful environ-
ment at the University of Würzburg, Germany, to give the final touches
to the manuscript.
I am greatly indebted to Peter Austin of SOAS, University of London,
UK where I was associated as the Leverhulme Professor for nine months
for the provision of both physical and mental space to think and complete
the grammar. But for the help that he and his staff offered I could not have
accomplished the task at hand. I dedicate this work to him.
I will be indebted all my life to Satish Abbi, my husband, who not only
encouraged me to plunge into this venture but also accompanied me to
the field as much as possible. His help, understanding, and cooperation
kept the fire burning in me despite many official, physical and psychologi-
cal hurdles.
I feel very happy and satisfied that I could accomplish the task of writ-
ing this grammar before the journey of the language into oblivion.
Maps
Figures
Tables
6.6. Body part terms with e=, i= possessive class marker ................. 145
6.7. Body part terms with ara= possessive class marker .................. 146
6.8. Body part terms with o=, ɔ= possessive class marker . .............. 147
6.9. Frequency of occurrence of body division class markers with
body part terminology . ....................................................................... 148
6.10. Kinship terms . ....................................................................................... 149
6.11. Parallels between body parts and kinship terms . ...................... 150
6.12. Double marking and secondary possession ................................. 154
6.13. Terms for the parts of a tree or a plant ......................................... 160
6.14. Possessive class markers defining primary possession and
classes of nouns . ................................................................................... 161
6.15. Varieties of possession ........................................................................ 165
7.1. Pronominal forms in PGA . ................................................................ 169
7.2. Pronominal clitics . ............................................................................... 174
7.3. Indefinite/interrogative forms .......................................................... 181
8.1. Body division classes in adjectives .................................................. 194
8.2. Edge and side of an object . ............................................................... 197
8.3. Multiplicity of temporal deixis ......................................................... 202
8.4. Terms for direction and days of a month ..................................... 203
8.5. Names of flowers and hunting time ............................................... 204
8.6. Levels of the sea .................................................................................... 205
8.7. Landscape and coastal terms ............................................................ 205
8.8. Spatial deixis describing parts of a Great Andamanese
house . ....................................................................................................... 206
8.9. Spatial adverbs and their morphemic composition .................. 208
8.10. Body division classes designating spatial relations .................... 209
8.11. Body division classes in adverbs ...................................................... 210
8.12. Demonstrative pronouns . .................................................................. 211
9.1. The body division class markers with verbs ................................ 215
9.2. Verbs with Formative affixes, tense and mood suffixes ........... 239
9.3. Verbal affixes in PGA . ......................................................................... 241
Abbreviations and symbols
1 first person
2 second person
3 third person
A agent
abl ablative
abs absolutive
acc accusative
adj adjective
adv adverb
agt agentive
appl applicative
c consonant
caus causative
cl 1, cl 2, etc. body division class 1, 2, etc.
class classifier
clt clitic
com comitative
comp complementiser
compr comparative
conj conjunct
cop copula
cond conditional
conv converb
d possessed
dat dative
dem demonstrative
dir directional
dis.vis distant visible
dis.invis distant invisible
dst.pst distant past
do Direct object
du dual
emph emphatic
erg ergative
excl exclusive
xxvi abbreviations and symbols
exist existential
F female
fa formative affix
gen genitive
h, hon honorific
hab habitual
imm.pst immediate past
imp imperative
io indirect object
ina inalienability
incl inclusive
instr instrument
int intimate
loc locational
M male
mod modifier
mt mother tongue
N noun
neg negative
nmlz nominaliser
nom nominal
np noun phrase
npst non-past tense
nrr.pst narrative past
O object
obj object clitic
PGA Present-day Great Andamanese
pcpl participial
pl plural
pp postpositional phrase
poss possessive
pro pronoun
prohb prohibitive
prox proximate
prox1 proximate, nearer to the speaker
prox2 proximate, nearer to the hearer
pst past
q question
R possessor
abbreviations and symbols xxvii
rec reciprocal
rel relativiser
refl reflexive
result resultative
S subject
sg singular
Sp speaker
spec specific
stat stative
subj subjunctive
tam tense, mood and aspect
tr transitiviser
V verb
vt verb transitive
vi verb intransitive
X any entity
= clitic boundary
Chapter one
Introduction
The Andaman Islands and their people have been the centre of interest for
a multitude of scientific and cultural reasons since their discovery in the
18th century. During the 18th and 19th centuries, researchers studied these
islands to classify and catalogue the various human races in the world,
with the underlying desire to scientifically confirm the racial superiority
of the white race over the others. The 20th century witnessed a change
in this attitude. Subsequent scientific, anthropological, linguistic and cul-
tural studies revealed that the islands and the people were unique from
an evolutionary perspective. These preliminary studies have revealed that
investigations concerning these small islands are important for under-
standing significant aspects of human evolution, migration and even the
peopling of the continents.
13○
Strait I.
Bangladesh 12○
China
Burma Andaman
(Myanmar) Islands
India 11○
N. Sentinel I.
Thailand Little Andaman
Bay of
Bengal A n daman 10○
S ea
Car Nicobar
Sri Lanka 9○
Malaysia Nicobar
Indonesia
Islands
8○
Great 7○
0 50 Miles Nicobar
0 50 Kilometres
The Great Andaman: the area covered by the Andaman Islands is made
up of island clusters. From north to south, the various islands are North
Andaman, Middle Andaman, South Andaman, Baratang, Ritchie Archipel-
ago and North and South Sentinel. Collectively, these are called the Great
Andaman. The close proximity of these islands to each other creates the
impression of one island, a fact that unfortunately motivated the govern-
ment to build the Andaman Grand Trunk road, which robbed the tribes
of their basic resources due to rampant deforestation. The capital city of
the Andaman Islands is Port Blair, located in the south of the islands at a
distance of 1255 km from Kolkata. Approximately 65 km south from the
city of Port Blair in the Great Andaman is the island called Little Anda-
man, which is a home to Onges.
1.2 Population
above can be grouped into four major groups: the Great Andamanese, the
Jarawa, the Onge and the Sentinelese. Barring the Sentinelese, the other
tribes have been exposed to the mainlanders. Jangil, a language closely
associated with Sentinelese, and several other Great Andamanese lan-
guages have become extinct within the last eighty years.
The ten distinct languages of the Great Andaman are known by the
names of the respective tribes that speak them. The history of contact
with outsiders varies from tribe to tribe. Chronologically, the first group
to come into contact with the mainlanders was the Great Andamanese,
followed by the Onge and finally the Jarawa. All attempts to establish con-
tact with the Sentinelese have failed so far. The Jarawa came to be known
to the mainlanders only recently. It is generally believed that first con-
tact was established in 1998. The demographic scale of these islanders is
inversely related to the period of contact with the mainlanders; the longer
and deeper the contact, the smaller the population. It is reported that the
6 chapter one
Islands. Wild pig was the only large mammal until fifty years ago. There
are no monkeys, squirrels or other small mammals except bats. Deer and
dogs were only introduced recently. Among the fauna the Andamanese
recognise a large variety of birds, crabs, turtles, fish, snakes, ants and mos-
quitoes. Among the flora, the largest lexicon is that of trees and plants
which are used for either hunting, everyday use or for medicinal purposes.
It perhaps justifies the utilitarian view of the folk biology that people have
a more diverse vocabulary for those categories of biological entities which
are more useful to them. For information on these aspects, readers may
refer to A Dictionary of the Great Andamanese Language (Abbi 2012).
The Great Andamanese, especially those who visit Port Blair frequently,
have a functional knowledge of Hindi and Bangla. Some of them also
understand a few words of English. Children below the age of sixteen do
not understand or speak the heritage language. All of the members com-
municate in Andamanese Hindi among themselves. See §1.8 below.
state of the men in the tribe. It is a vicious circle from which men find it
difficult to break out.
A large part of the island is occupied by the Public Work Department
(PWD) and the AAJVS (the Andaman Tribal Welfare Society) after the
last tsunami of 2004. There is a nursery school for children and a small
dispensary for primary health care. The island is lush green consisting of
coconut, banana, tamarind, mango, gurjan, palm, and several indigenous
trees. No vegetables are grown on the island: they are all transported from
the city of Port Blair. Solar powered equipment is seen to be used by the
Great Andamanese people. The local non-tribal population is larger than
the indigenous tribal population, a glaring fact obvious to anyone visiting
the Reserve. We made several trips to this island as the government does
not give permission to visit the island for longer than three weeks at a
time.
When we first visited Strait Island in 2001 there were nine speakers
(of PGA), among them three Jirake, the Chief (multilingual in several
languages/dialects of the Great Andamanese language family, but profi-
cient in Jeru), Boa Sr., (the last speaker of Bo) and Nao Jr. (the most flu-
ent speaker of Jeru) served us as the main repositories of information.
Unfortunately, they all died between 2005–2010. Another speaker Boro,
who came from a Khora background also died during our fieldwork.
At present, we have, in order of fluency in the language: Lico, Peje, Noe,
Surmei and Golat. They are all in the range of fifty to fifty-eight years of
age and speak the koiné variety of present-day Great Andamanese. Alas,
the language is breathing its last.
the andaman and the great andamanese 9
Apart from Great Andamanese, the other three languages are vibrant
despite the small population base, as they are still passed on to the next
generation. Except for Sentinelese, we have fair amount of knowledge
about the Andamanese languages.
The following is a brief account of a linguistic study undertaken on the
Andamanese languages with special emphasis on the new perspectives
emerging from research undertaken in the last ten years. While discussing
the results of these investigations, we will be keeping the issues of genetic
classification and typology of the languages in mind. In this way the his-
torical analysis proceeds in tandem with the descriptive analysis.
3 There is one speaker who claims to hail from a Sare background though she speaks
the modern version of the Great Andamanese language.
4 The last few generations of Great Andamanese speakers are descendants of inter-
marriages among North Andamanese tribes. The Government of India encouraged this
practice in order to preserve their dwindling numbers when the entire population was
settled on Strait Island.
10 chapter one
Andamanese
Western Eastern
Jarawa [300]
Figure 1.1. Two distinct language families in Andaman (Adapted from Abbi 2003)
Jeru, Sare, Khora and Bo, but is primarily based on the grammar of
Jeru. As the present form is the amalgam of four languages, the author
has deliberately avoided naming this form of the language ‘Jeru’ as this
will unnecessarily place an arbitrary emphasis on one language over the
others. PGA is spoken on Strait Island. A recent study shows that it shares
a large percentage of its vocabulary with Aka-Kede, the central variety of
the Great Andamanese language (Mayank 2009).
the andaman and the great andamanese 11
Aka-Khora
Aka-Bo
Aka-Kede
Great
Andamanese
Aka-Jowoi
Central Variety
Aka-Pucikwar
Aka-Kol
Aka-Bale
Southern Variety
Aka-Bea
7 A detailed grammar was included in the Census Report of 1901: 98–121, which has
been left out from the reprint version undertaken by the Government of India, and hence
is not accessible to researchers.
14 chapter one
8 The latter got its name because both Jarawa and Onge call themselves ‘Ang’, pro-
nounced as əŋ. Justin (2000) had also proposed that they should be called Ung because of
the andaman and the great andamanese 15
the similar reason. The name Jarawa is given to the tribe by Âkà-Bêa to refer to them as
‘stranger’, ‘one you should be afraid of ’.
9 She uses the term ‘Ongan’ for these.
16 chapter one
Aka - Bo
Aka - Kede Jarawa Onge - Jarawa
Aka - Juwoi
Jarawa
Aka - Pucikwar
the andaman and the great andamanese
Jangil
Sentineli Sentineli
NORTH
Onge
10 This dictionary is prepared in the specially designed Toolbox program of SIL, with
an accompanying Lexique Pro on a CD Rom, which gives in addition, etymology of words
wherever possible and sources of information.
11 The works cited here were the results of the project Vanishing Voices of the Great
Andamanese (VOGA) supported by the Hans Rausing Endangered Language Project, Uni-
versity of London (2005–2009).
the andaman and the great andamanese 19
phrases and not in lengthy conversations. This too is the case with the
‘best speakers’ of the language. The continuum of language competence
among the Great Andamanese that I discuss below should be understood
against this background. I use the term ‘best speaker’ for speakers with the
competence level 5 (refer to §1.8.2) and the speakers at level 4 are termed
‘semi-speakers.’ The term ‘best speakers’ is used for those who were found
to be best among the few members who still use the language although
not very fluently and not in all domains. Apart from language loss, the
Great Andamanese people also suffer from a loss of cultural heritage, of
ancient practices and rituals, as well as a loss of the art of narration. The
few people who now speak the language did not remember any native
stories. It was noticed that storytelling as an activity does not exist
anymore.12 Neither the young mothers nor the old people of the commu-
nity ever narrate stories to their children. The loss of the mother tongue
has very serious implications as the very genre of narration has also been
lost in the contact language, i.e. Andamani Hindi. Thus, the present gen-
eration of Great Andamanese have never heard any story from their elders
neither in their heritage language nor in Andamani Hindi.
A thorough investigation of the linguistic behaviour of the Great Anda-
manese towards their own language and culture presents a bleak picture
and points towards a future when they will become a group of people
who would have totally lost their land, language, tales, songs, art of body
painting and many facets of indigenous cultural identity. These are the
perils of language loss.
The following is a sociolinguistic sketch based on initial fieldwork con-
ducted during 2005–2007, when the total population of the Great Anda-
manese was fifty. The number of children under the age of ten years was
twenty. There were thirteen members in the age range between twenty
and thirty. There were only ten members aged between thirty and eighty.
The number of infants, i.e. between the ages of zero to five years, was also
thirteen. See Figure 1.3.
The increase in population in the Great Andamanese tribe has been
tremendous especially in the last few years. There were only thirty-six
12 However, Nao Jr., one of the best speakers of the language, did manage to narrate ten
folk tales with great effort, although not all in the heritage language. These are submitted
for publication with NBT, India. The first folk tale narrated by Nao Jr., An Ancient Tale from
Andaman, was published in 2012. He narrated folk tales in Hindi after much prodding. It
took him, at times, many days to complete a story as he had never narrated one in his life
time and had heard them from his elders only when he was a child. Other old members
of the community reported loss of memory regarding folk tales. It was a very sad state of
affairs as a couple of speakers wanted to oblige us but could not remember anything.
20 chapter one
14
13 Number of
12 tribe members
12
10
8
8
7
Number
6
5
4
2 2
2
1
0
0 to 5 6 to 9 10 to 20 21 to 30 31 to 40 41 to 50 51 to 60 61 to 80
years years years years years years years years
Age
Figure 1.3. The number and age of the Great Andamanese people (2005–2006)
members in 2001 when the author conducted her first fieldwork on the
islands but at present there are fifty-six. This indicates that the commu-
nity is thriving.
Table 1.3. The lineage of the oldest and the best speakers
Speakers Claimed MT Mother’s MT Father’s MT Spouse’s MT
Sp 1 (58) M Jeru/Bo Sare Bo Jeru
Sp 2 (80) F Jeru Bo Jeru Jeru
Sp 3 (74) F Khora Khora Khora Jeru
Sp 4 (60) M Jeru Khora Jeru Jeru
Sp 5 (45) F Jeru Jeru Jeru Jeru/Khora
Sp 6 (48) F Jeru Jeru Jeru Bo
Sp 7 (43) F Jero Khora Bo Jeru
(The number in brackets indicates the age of the speakers)
Interestingly, speaker no. 14 is not the only one who has married out-
side the community. One of the female members of the community, aged
twenty-six (who has a mixed parentage of Jeru & Bo) has recently mar-
ried a Bengali person. She is not a fluent speaker of the language; her
competence level is 3. Hence, an average Great Andamanese could be a
Khora speaker, married to a Sare, and speaks Jeru to her neighbours and
Andamani Hindi to her children. It is a complex web of multilingualism
that an average Great Andamanese builds around her/him.
Marriage with outsiders has resulted in interactions with other com-
munity speakers and in the learning of their languages. Although marriage
and cohabitation with members outside the community is a recent trend,
intermarriages among different sub-tribes of the Great Andamanese is
nothing new, as can be seen in Table 1.3. However, with almost every-
one claiming Jeru as their mother tongue, the other heritage languages
are clearly marginalised and ignored, indicating a ‘superiority’ com-
plex assigned to the heritage language Jeru. Members are proud to call
themselves Jeru speakers even when few of them actually speak it. This
22 chapter one
13 A similar situation is observed in the mainland northern belt of India where speakers
of most of the marginalized languages claim ‘Hindi’ as their mother tongue.
the andaman and the great andamanese 23
5 7 Number of
speakers
4 7
Competence
3 8
2 3
1 10
0 6
0 2 4 6 8 10 12
Number of speakers
Figure 1.4. Number of speakers on the scale of competence
70 Mean Age
60 58
50
Mean age
40 34
30 23
20 16
10 8.8
4
0
0 1 2 3 4 5
Competence level
5 58 7
4 34 7
competence
3 23 9
2 16 3
1 9 10 Mean Age
No. of speakers
0 4 6
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70
Mean age
Figure 1.6. The number and mean age of the speakers on the competence scale
process of language erosion. This also clearly indicates that the language
is not transmitted from the older generation to the younger generation
effectively. The reluctance of the younger members of the community to
learn and use the heritage language is also responsible for this decline in
their competence level. The graph below (Figure 1.5) clearly indicates the
decline in competence level from the older to the younger generation.
Figure 1.6 given below sums up the situation of language competence
among the Great Andamanese with respect to the age of the speakers and
their number in the community. The members with the highest compe-
tence also have the highest mean age. However, the strength of level 5
speakers was very small in 2007 and it has since been further reduced
by the passing away of four elderly speakers. The members with a low
competence level have a low mean age and their strength is consider-
ably higher than the previous group, indicating the moribund status of
the language. Competency level is positively related to age: the higher the
age group, the higher the proficiency; the lower the age group, the lower
the proficiency in Great Andamanese.
14 It is interesting to note that tribes associate themselves by father’s tongue and not
by mother’s tongue.
the andaman and the great andamanese 27
15 The etymology of the words is given in A Dictionary of the Great Andamanese Lan-
guage English-Great Andamanese-Hindi’ (Abbi 2012).
28 chapter one
very need and domains in which to use their heritage language are vanish-
ing. Alongside education and the culture of the work place, daily contact
with mainstream people must surely have a direct effect on language ero-
sion. By their own admission, code-switching and code-mixing between
Hindi and PGA is far higher than the occasional mixture of a few words
that takes place between the other heritage languages.
16 First language attrition has been researched by many linguists in the past; Kees de
Bot (2004), Herbert W. Seliger, Robert Michael Vago (1991), and Monika S. Schmid (2011)
among others.
the andaman and the great andamanese 29
her life based on the names of flowers. Men undergo name changes more
often than women in their life cycle. Consider the following table describ-
ing different stages of naming a person.
Each life form category is usually divided into intermediates, which is the
rank between life form and folk-generic categories.
The Great Andamanese people recognise different folk specifics and
a subclass of folk generics, mainly on visible attributes. One can also
see that there is a high level of correspondence between folk generics in
Great Andamanese ethno-taxonomy and scientific taxa, (for details see
Pande & Abbi 2011). Our detailed study shows that the Great Andamanese
classification system is consistent with ethno-biological classifications of
other non-literate tribal societies (Berlin 1992; Forth 2004), indicating that
humans everywhere recognise nature’s structure in essentially the same
way (Bulmer 1974). It can be seen that the Great Andamanese ethno-
taxonomy is consistent with the general principles and ideology of sci-
entific classification, as observed analytically for several other non-tribal
societies (Berlin 1992).
The present study analyses PGA as spoken on Strait Island and in parts of
Port Blair. There are ten chapters to this grammar, followed by the appen-
dices including maps, graphs and a sample text. As certain structures of
the language were found to be unique, Chapter 3, which is a ‘Grammar
overview’, familiarises the reader with the basics in addition to the axi-
oms necessary to comprehend what follows in the subsequent chapters.
In this respect, it is the most important chapter of the present grammar.
It would be beneficial if the reader were to read this chapter before mov-
ing on to the others. Readers may find phonetic variation in forms within
and across chapters as our data comes from various speakers who have
retained some of the sounds/phones of their original heritage language.
These variations have been explained in the chapter on phonology. The
only text with interlinear translations that was available to us, after a long
period of working with the community members,17 is the Creation Myth,
the story of Phertajido, the first Great Andamanese or the first human
being. This is given at the end of the grammar.
17 For details on the elicitation of folk tales and the difficulties in acquiring them, one
can consult A Dictionary of the Great Andamanese Language. English-Great Andamanese-
Hindi (Abbi 2012).
Chapter two
Introduction
2.1 Vowels
/i/ : /ɛ/
/iboi/ ‘cooked food’
/ɛ boi/ ‘spouse’
/ɛ/ : /e/
/bɛŋ/ ‘muck’
/beŋ/ ‘forehead’ ~ [be:ŋ]
/en/ ‘a kind of leaf ’
/ɛn/ ‘wound’
phonetics and phonology 39
/ɛ/ : /ɑ/
/ɑrɑ-cɛ/ ‘sting’
/ɑrɑ-ca/ ‘nest’
/ɑ/ : /ɔ/
/phɑr/ ‘boil/pimple’
/phƆr/ ‘bamboo’
/kɑbo/ ‘a deity’
/kƆbo/ ‘skin’
/o/ : /ɔ/
/kobu/ ‘umbrella’
/kɔbu/ ‘plates made of silai leaf ’
/ʈole/ ‘pluck’ (imp)
/ʈɔle/ ‘flowers’
/ɛrphɔke/ ‘take out from hot fire’
/ɛrphoke/ ‘kill’ (imp)
/ɛrkɔtho/ ‘nose’
/ɛrkotho/ ‘mix’
/u/ : /o/
/ɑrɑpho/ ‘fell a tree’
/ɑrɑphu/ ‘latrine/stool’
2.1.2 Length
Length at the phonemic level could only be attested for a few vowels.
However, it was noticed that the speakers occasionally varied between
the absence and presence of vowel length. Vowel length has been speci-
fied where it is important for the pronunciation of the word, especially in
the words which are derived from the Bo variety. Words derived from the
Sare variety also show distinctive vowel length. The last speaker of the Bo
language, Boa Sr., had distinctive length in her speech. Vowel length has
been specified by a colon mark [:] placed after the long vowel. It can be
postulated that the earlier version of the language had phonemic length,
as was documented by Manoharan (1989), although this is apparently no
40 chapter two
longer the case. Some examples demonstrating vowel length are given
below:
/ɑ/ : /ɑ:/
/ɑrɑttɑ/ ‘convince’
/ɑrɑ:ttɑy/ ‘mensuration’
/cɑo/ ‘dog’
/cɑ:o/ ‘rain fish’
/e/ : /e:/
/jibeʈ/ ‘small bat’
/jibe:ʈ/ ‘swallow’ (N)
/i/ : /i:/
/jili/ ‘snail’
/ji:li/ ‘aunt’ (father’s brother’s wife)
/o/ : /o:/
/bol/ ‘fish’
/bo:l/ ‘cane, fresh water snake’
/u/ : /u:/
/burul/ ‘white fish’
/bu:rul/ ‘a kind of fish’
/bun/ ‘sea shell’, ‘a kind of snail’
/bu:n/ ‘spoon’
Although there is a minimal pair [bun] and [bu:n] available in the lan-
guage, distinction in length is neutralised for some speakers as we also
find [bun] ~ [bu:n] ‘sea shell.’
It is observed that no two identical vowels occur in any position. For fur-
ther information refer to section 2.6 on morphophonemics.
2.2 Semi-vowels/Glides1
1 I am using y for the palatal semi-vowel and j for the palatal voiced stop throughout
the text.
phonetics and phonology 45
2.3 Consonants
Table 2.9 gives an overview of variation among the speakers. The claimed
father tongue(s) by each speaker is given in parentheses.3 The sounds in
free variation vary from speaker to speaker, thus confirming the hypoth-
esis that the existing speakers of the language are not descendants of the
speakers of one language, but instead are the descendants of speakers of
different varieties of the same language family. In other words, PGA rep-
resents inter‐group free variation. However, the substitution of sounds in
free variation does not disturb the comprehension of the word under con-
sideration. One can see that the speech of the speaker Noe is influenced
by her husband, Peje, who comes from a mixed background of Khora
and Jeru.
3 The name of the language(s) and the names of the original sub-tribe that a speaker
belongs to are the same.
4 As mentioned in The World Atlas of Language Structures (Haspelmath et al., 2005).
48 chapter two
while our speakers mostly used the voiceless bilabial aspirated plosive
[ph]. Some speakers also used the voiceless bilabial fricative [Φ]. Specifi-
cally, Peje and his wife Noe produced this sound.5 The voiced counterpart
[β] was also sporadically attested in the speech of one or two consultants.6
One can safely say that the bilabial fricatives are on their way to extinc-
tion as our database has very few words with these sounds. They seem to
be replaced by bilabial aspirates. Similarly, the voiceless velar fricative [x]
has changed over the last forty years to a voiceless velar aspirated plo-
sive [kh] due to Hindi influence. We have tried our best to capture both
of these sounds (refer to Abbi 2012). Only one speaker, a woman aged
76 called Boro, who was originally a Khora speaker, used the retroflex
trill (we use the symbols [ɽ] for this in the text): this also has been docu-
mented. Sounds such as [Φ, β, ɽ, x, lw, w] that occur least often are given
in parentheses. All other sounds occur at phonemic level.
5 My recent visit to Andaman in 2009 showed that Noe had lost [Φ] in favour of [ph]
due to contact with her fellow community members who also use [ph] under the influ-
ence of Hindi.
6 Our consultant Golat used [β] although he was the son of Boro Sr., the Khora speaker
who had [b] in her verbal repertoire.
phonetics and phonology 49
the language is dying fast and speakers no longer use it in their daily life.
This lack of use of the language and indifferent attitude towards the use
of the heritage language seem to be a significant reason for the genesis of
sound variation in the lexicon as well as in syntactic constructions.7
2.3.1.1 Nasals
There are four distinct nasal consonants at the phonemic level. /m/: /n/:
/ŋ/ : /ɲ/
These four nasal consonants contrast with each other in initial, medial
and final positions.
kʰuŋi ‘a fish’
kʰun ‘bring’
kʰum ‘side shoulder’
biŋo ‘think’
bino ‘sleep’
ben ‘back shoulder bone’
beŋ ‘forehead’
ŋa 2sg
ɲa ‘eat’
ɛkʈɛŋe ‘sputum’
ekʈɛne ‘pull it’
7 For further information on the speakers and instances of use of the above variation,
readers should consult A Dictionary of the Great Andamanese Language (Abbi 2012) which
contains all the citations for the aforementioned variations.
50 chapter two
stops. Only the voiceless sounds are aspirated. We will first consider ante-
rior and coronal sounds. /t/ : /th /: /ʈ / : /ʈh/: /d/
tec ‘leaf ’
theca ‘intoxicated’
tɛkho ‘speech’
dekhɔ ‘now’
ʈhu 1sg
thu ‘reach’
bathe ‘hind of an animal’
bate ‘extinguish’
eka-ʈoke ‘necklace’
eka-ʈʰoke ‘close it’
kata ‘a piece’
kaʈa ‘girl’
dun ‘they’
ɖum ‘earthworm’
dakar ‘basket’
ɖakar ‘potato’
The velar sound exists only in its voiceless form. As we discussed in §2.3
the voiced velar [g] is conspicuous by its absence in the present form
of the language but the possibility of its existence in the earlier form can-
not be ruled out.
/kh/ : /k/
/kh/ voiceless aspirated velar stop
/k/ voiceless unaspirated velar stop
khu ‘drink’
ku ‘burn’
ɛr=ʈɔko ‘slowly’
ɛr=ʈɔkʰo ‘fishing with bamboo’
kharae ‘crab, found only in sand’
karai ‘black ant’
phonetics and phonology 51
kɔrɔ ‘palm’
khɔrɔ ‘sand’
/b/
[b] voiced bilabial stop
[β] voiced bilabial fricative
The following examples help us establish the phonemic status of /p/, /ph/
and /b/, the latter varying with its fricative counterpart.
puro ‘whetstone’
phuro ‘a kind of owl’
buro [buro] ~ [βuro] ‘hand movement’
bol ‘rope’
phol ‘love someone’
/c/ : /j/
Aspirated and unaspirated versions vary freely between speakers. The aspi-
rated version was observed in the speech of Boa Sr., our only Bo speaker.
Sporadically the Khora speaker, Boro Sr. also used the sound [ch] where
other speakers used [c].8 This has been noted in Table 2.9 above.
2.3.1.3 Fricatives
/s/ : /ʃ/
/s/ voiceless alveolar sibilant
/ʃ/ voiceless palatal sibilant
sorobul ‘enemy’
ʃoro-kom ‘kill with an arrow’
Speakers, such as Boa Sr., were observed using [ch] instead of [ ʃ ] word-
initially as in chime ‘soak’ while others varied between [sime] and [ʃime].
As observed in Table 2.9 above, the sounds [s], [ ʃ ], [c] and [ch] varied
freely between the members of the Great Andamanese community as
each of them had descended from a different linguistic background.
2.3.1.4 Liquids
/l/ : /r/
/l/ voiced alveolar lateral. This sound has an alternant in
[lw] which is a voiced labialised lateral.
luro [luro] ~ [lw uro] ~ [luɽo] ‘fire’
phal [phɑl] ~ [phɑ lw] ‘sea wave’
8 It was observed that the two speakers were intimate friends and spent most of their
time together. Influence of Boa’s speech on Boro’s cannot be ruled out.
phonetics and phonology 53
Boro Sr., who hailed from the Khora tribe, used the retroflex version of the
trill more often than not. The trill /r/ contrasted with lateral /l/.
juro ‘goddess’
julu ‘clothes’
le ‘crab’
re ‘flower’
trera ‘shining’
trɛpho ‘much’
trai ‘fever’
khriŋkoʃo ‘Strait Island’
2.4 Syllables
σ σ σ σ σ σ σ σ σ
/ɛr li u e/
σ σ σ σ
Figure 2.1. Syllable breaks
2.4.2 Constraints
All consonants can appear in the onset of a word. The presence of initial
[ɽ] was observed in a few words in Khora. All consonants, except aspi-
rated consonants and the sibilant [ ʃ ], can end a word. There are only
three words in our database that end in the voiceless sibilant [s]. The
large inventory of consonants that appear word-initially and word-finally
can be ascribed to the fact that the language is formed from four different
varieties and has included several kinds of words in its lexicon (see Abbi
2012). As mentioned in §2.3.4, initial consonant clusters appear sporadi-
cally in [tr-], as in trɛpho ‘much’ and in [khr-] as in khriŋkoʃo ‘Strait Island.’
Medial consonant clusters are in abundance.
a-ka-ker-tot-lɔcoŋ ‘uvula’
v-cv-cvc-cvc-cv-cvc
1 2 3 4 5 6
Names of birds, fish, insects, reptiles and other jungle creatures provide
most of the complex words with long syllable structures.9 At times, two
consonant clusters may appear within the same word, as in trɔkʈɔimo
‘long-tail cricket’ or kulʈɛʈmo ‘wasp’ (Vespa affinis).
2.5 Morphophonemics
2.5.1 Gemination
We have seen that geminates occur intervocalically and that they also
conform to the syllable division given above. Geminates are created in
PGA by progressive assimilation. Thus:
v + ccv → v: + cv
bi + ikko > be:ku-abs + burn ‘burnt’
2.5.3 Homorganicity
Nasals acquire the features of the next consonant across the morpheme
boundary. That is, nasal consonants assimilate to the place of articulation
of the following obstruents. This can be stated in a rule:
+ consonant
α anterior + consonant
+ nasal
β coronal α anterior
γ back + β coronal
γ back
For example,
bi + ikko > be + ikku abs + burn ‘burnt’
bi + ijiom > be + ijiom abs + eat ‘eats’
ʈhu + ut cone > ʈho + ut cone ‘I go’
phonetics and phonology 61
When the absolutive -bi combines with the object proclitic, it follows
the same rules as in §2.5.7 (a) and (b). Thus:
Refer to §10.1.14 also to see how this works for all parts of speech. The
constituents of this phonological word may or may not have an indepen-
dent meaning, e.g. ʈhɛm which is derived from the first singular pronomi-
nal proclitic ʈh= and the reflexive prefix ɛm- means ‘myself.’ However, ʈhot
which is derived from the first singular pronominal proclitic ʈh = ‘I’ and
possessive proclitic ot= carries the meaning of only the first morpheme ʈhu
‘I’ or may represent possessive ‘mine’ if followed by a possessed noun as
in ʈhot bo ‘my back.’ The attachment of clitics to various nouns and verbs
is discussed in the next chapter.
2.5.9 Metathesis
Sporadic examples of metathesis of the semi-vowel y and the locative
marker -l have been noticed in the speech of Jero and Khora speakers.
Thus:
As noted above, one of the speakers, Peje,11 always used a labialised lateral
instead of the simple alveolar lateral [l]. An acoustic study was carried out
to analyse the sound.
The labialised lateral could have been an idiosyncratic phenomenon in
Peje’s speech. To rule out language contact effects, other languages of the
Andaman were also investigated. However, no other language spoken in
the Andaman was noticed to contain this particular sound. Despite this,
I undertook the acoustic study of laterals so as to give the benefit of the
doubt for establishing an archaic trait of the language. Peje had reported
to us that he had spent some time of his childhood with some Burmese
tribes. We, however, could not verify this.
Please refer to Appendix B for the spectrograms and further analysis
of laterals.
Chapter three
Grammar overview
3.1.1 General
As discussed in the previous chapter the language demonstrates large
phonetic variation, and contains varying vocabulary items used to express
the same meaning; this is likely because of its mixed nature or its status
as an endangered and moribund language. This chapter is an overview
of grammar highlighting the basic tenets of the grammatical structure of
PGA. This is done so as to prepare the readers to comprehend what fol-
lows in the subsequent chapters.
PGA has a dual semantic system for body part categorisation: one that
is expressed in concrete nouns and another more abstract one that is
expressed in grammaticalised morphemes indicating body divisions. These
are represented by body division possessive classes that classify body part
terms based on the area of the body they refer to. These classes also clas-
sify other inalienables, with some semantic connection to the body part
system. Furthermore, body part semantics pervade the lexical and gram-
matical system of the language as this dual system is extended to other
form classes, viz. verbs, adjectives and adverbs. The body division class
markers occur as proclitics attached to all classes of content words. Thus,
66 chapter three
all content words in PGA can easily be divided into bound and free, the
former necessarily imbued with the semantics of ‘inherency’ and ‘depen-
dency.’ This is a unique and important feature of the language: all parts
of speech occur as dependent and non-dependent pairs, the former being
obligatorily marked. Refer to §3.6 below.
Most of the nouns that refer to the typically inalienable possessed
items as well as those which refer to the objects or results of an action,
e.g., ik-jira ‘it-story.tell’, i.e. ‘a story’, are marked for dependency. All body
part terms, kinship terms, part-to-whole, part-to-component, as well as
nouns referring to time, direction, and depth are dependent nouns. The
dependent nouns are further divided into seven classes, each defined by
a distinct body division inalienability marker appearing as a class marker
proclitic.
There are three numbers encoded in pronominal forms. However,
nouns in general are not marked for duality and plurality. Number is
marked for plurality on a few common nouns designating animacy fea-
tures, e.g. ‘dogs’, ‘children’, etc.
PGA is agglutinative in terms of its treatment of morpheme boundar-
ies, but polysynthetic in its nature of word morphology. A large number
of morphemes, affixes, phonological words, clitics, and incorporation can
constitute a single word. The incorporation of reflexives and nouns is seen
in verb complexes. These verb complexes may constitute a verb phrase.
Thus, verbs are much more versatile and elaborate than nouns. PGA is a
prototypical ‘double marking’ language where the head or possessed noun
is obligatorily marked in inalienable possession, but it is the possessor, the
dependent noun, which is marked in alienable possession. In addition,
case markings are suffixed to the nouns. The verb complex includes a large
amount of information in multi-morphemic strings that include object clit-
ics, incorporated nominals in causative constructions, reflexive and recip-
rocal prefixes, as well as suffixes expressing tense, aspect, and mood. Overt
external NPs are present in addition to the verb complex. However, these
are optional and often dropped in discourse. PGA is a verb-final language.
Following are few sentences from PGA to exemplify the verb complex and
the pervasive body division class markers (symbolised as cl). One can see
that body class markers are also attached to verbs.
Some of the other typological features are: (1) While the genitive phrase
precedes the head noun (as is typical of verb-final languages), other modi-
fiers follow the modified elements. (2) Nouns in general are poor in affix-
ation compared to verbs. (3) Cross-referencing proclitics occur on verbs
as well as on temporal adverbs.
The most striking feature of the language is the heavy occurrence of
proclitics. Most of the free personal pronouns exist in reduced forms as
simple proclitics and when they occur with other clitics in the language,
such as body class proclitics, they offer the possibility of clitic sequenc-
ing. In a large number of words, the clitics have fused with the noun or
verb and are no longer transparent. This can be seen as a result of the
process of grammaticalisation (discussed in detail later in the chapter in
§3.7) or from concrete lexicon to abstract grammatical concepts that nor-
mally takes place in the course of language change but more significantly,
during language evolution (Givon 2002; Heine & Kuteva 2007).
Proclitics are symbolised by the = symbol in the examples given in the
text. We will consider these in detail later in the chapter.
Plural subject nouns are not marked for their argument functions. As
stated earlier, pronouns are unmarked for S and A functions (11).
1 There is no equivalent in English for this local fruit known as ‘Tendu’ in the Anda-
manese Hindi.
grammar overview 69
The following sentence gives both functions of homophonous -bi, viz. the O and
A functions. It is not clear why the subject of the transitive is marked by -bi case
marking. Is it an instance of case alternation?
(23) e=bɔr-ɔm
cl 5=wind-npst
‘Wind is blowing.’
(26) e=cɛr-om
cl 5=rain-npst
‘It is raining/will/may rain.’
(37) ɖu ɛ=ʈɔl-ot= co
3sg-dem cl 5=flower-cl 4=seed
‘That is the pollen of the flower.’
(39a) bolo be
defecate imp.
‘go defecate’
The dichotomy between noun and verb does not appear to be rigid
because the same lexeme can be used as a verb or as a noun depending
upon the process involved in forming a lexical unit. Verbality is assigned
to prototypical noun categories by attaching tense markings, and con-
versely, nominality can be assigned to verbs by attaching case suffixes and
by other means such as showing it as an object of possession. The same is
true for adjectives. That is, any content word in PGA is capable of moving
from its pivotal slot and can occupy the predicate position with appropri-
ate tense, mood and aspect inflections. Thus each of the following word
classes can be used as a predicate.
Words for the phases of the moon and the sun can occupy the predicate
position by adding TAM categories. The word for ‘sunset’ is ɖiu-birate
which takes class 4 as well as verbal categories such as tense marking.
(49) ʈh=ut=ɖiu-birate-k-ɔm
1sg=cl 4=sun-set-fa-npst
‘It will take me the whole day (to finish the job).’
Hence, the absolute separation between the nominal and verbal domain
has weakened in the language.3 Practically any root word in the language
has the potential to be predicated.
In the beginning of the chapter in §3.1.2, we said that there was a dual
semantic system in the language. This system cannot be understood with-
out the discussion on the concept of inalienability and inherency as per-
ceived by the Great Andamanese people. Let us begin with ‘inalienability’
and ‘grammaticalisation.’
3.3.1 Introduction
The concept of inalienability (henceforth ina) is very old in grammar
(Hyman et al. 1970; Allan 1975–1976; Voeltz 1976; Hawkins 1981; Hinne
busch & Kirsner 1981; Diem 1986; Chappell & McGregor 1996, to name
a few) and has been much debated by linguists, anthropologists, phi-
losophers and ethnographers for many years. It is still a major field for
research and debate. One of the reasons for such a wide and sustained
interest in the topic has been that there are some yet unresolved issues
regarding the definition and identification of ina elements that become
encoded in language. Then, there is also the problem of finding the appro-
priate placements for ina elements in grammar.
There is a fairly general agreement among linguists that there are five
areas of inalienability:
3.3.2 Anthropocentrism
PGA exhibits a hierarchy within most of the ina domains such as ‘body
divisions.’ There are seven distinct divisions/areas that are recognised
within the human body and each one is assigned a hierarchically des-
ignated place on a scale. The specific area that a particular body part
belongs to is designated by a specific inalienability [ina] or inherency
feature represented by body division classes. These are grammaticalised
morphemes that attach to several nouns pertaining to body part or body
division terms. These serve the grammatical function of a prototypical
possessive association between two nouns that stand in a relationship of
possessor and the possessed, or R and D. Since these class markers are
obligatorily attached to the head noun, D, the nouns under category D are
dependent nouns. The class markers by their distinct nature behave like
proclitics (see below). As mentioned earlier, PGA maintains seven body
divisions. It then further extends the notion of these seven divisions to
other terms, i.e. kin terms, spatial relational terms, closely related object
terms, and furthermore, attributes (human propensity), objects, action
and state (denoted by verbs) with reference to the body. This implies that
the Great Andamanese people visualise their world from a point of view
centered in the ‘self’ or ‘ego.’ The perspective that human beings are the
centre governs the structure of the modules of grammar. However, each
division within the realm of inalienability is designated on a hierarchical
scale. We will see in Chapter 6 that inalienable areas other than those rep-
resented by nominals such as (i)–(v) mentioned above, are also preceded
by these body division class markers.
As mentioned above, verbs, adjectives and adverbs are capable of being
attached to the same seven division class markers. In other words, these
class markers play a significant role in word formation processes as
grammaticalised units and can attach to a large number of vocabulary
items signifying diverse meanings. As there is a distinct marker used for
all typical ‘alienable’ objects -ico or -iʃo, the language formally maintains
the distinction between the two kinds of possession, i.e., alienable and
78 chapter three
a) Inseparability of r and d
b) Inherent relationship between r and d
c) Permanency of the relation between r and d
d) Intimacy of the relations between r and d (language- and culture-
specific)
e) Conceptual dependency
f) Inextricably linked entities
As Langacker (1991, 1995) observed, one can say that inherently relational
nouns are characterised by a high degree of conceptual dependency. They
are conceptually dependent in the sense that they must be understood in
relation to something given (Velazquez-Castillo 1996: 34). It is difficult to
define and identify what is inherently related and what is not; just as it
is challenging to establish a correlation between inherent relations and
conceptual dependency. The biggest challenge that PGA poses is this:
how should one distinguish one kind of conceptual dependency from the
other? How is a body part term such as ‘head’ conceptually more depen-
dent or less dependent than terms like ‘stomach’ or ‘tongue’? Or, how
can one correlate this dependency with the kinship terminology so as to
establish a one-to-one relationship between two inalienable nouns, the
body part and the kinship term such as ‘head’ and ‘child’ respectively?
grammar overview 79
(50) ʈh=ot=bo
1sg=cl 4.poss=back
‘My back.’
(51) ra tot= bo
pig cl 4.poss=back
‘Pig’s back.’ (For consumption)
grammar overview 81
The body division class markers help us to classify nouns into eight divi-
sions: seven inherently related or representing relational nouns, and one
non-inherently related or representing independent nouns. The objects
that can be transferred, or which are expected to be transferred, such as
goods in the market, household goods not of an intimate nature, objects
4 The class marker is preceded by a dental sound t- in this and the following example
because of the inanimate nature of the modified nominal.
82 chapter three
belonging flora and fauna (see Pande & Abbi 2011) and other objects of the
natural environment do not obligatorily take class markers. These may be
considered as independent nouns. These are discussed in detail in Chap-
ter 6 on possession.
As seen in (51), the use of t- prefixed to the body division class
marker makes it clear that inalienability is not perceived as a physi-
cal non-separation of an object from its host but as an inherent part of
the host. The conceptual dependency between the two is absolute and
is thus recognised as being intimately bonded to each other. The lan-
guage has developed formal devices in the form of proclitics (see below)
that reflect the dependencies, both conceptual and inherent, between
the related objects and events. Furthermore, the variety of formal cat-
egories present in the inalienable category implies varying degrees of
inalienability present in the conceptual world of the Great Andamanese.
Between the varying degrees of inalienability lie various kinds of inher-
ent relations. The inherent relation is signified by the use of proclitics
not only with nouns but also with other content words from different
grammatical categories. This phenomenon can be captured better by
Figure 3.1 given below indicating varying degrees of grammaticalisation of
content words.
addition, they can be seen as modifiers of the action because they encode
the manner of action.
Some transitive and intransitive verbs are attached to class markers
which account for a world of action and state that can be considered on
a par with the distinctions made on the scale of inalienability with regard
to the body part terms. Thus:
(a) ut=ʃile
class 4=aim class 5=aim
(b) e=ʃile
‘aim from above’ ‘aim to pierce’
The case for adjectives is similar to the case for the verbs. A kind of
semantic transparency, drawn on the basis of the original classificatory
meaning assigned to the body part terms by the class marker, can be seen
between these and the host adjectives to a large extent. This, at times,
leads to the possibility of determining the choice of adjective with a par-
ticular class marker. For instance, it was observed that the class marker
i- ~ e- is attached to those terms for body parts which are inside the body,
e.g., ‘blood’, ‘intestines’, etc., and that the same class marker is attached to
adjectives defining internal human traits such as e=liu-ʃɔŋɔ ‘brave’, e=cay
‘bad’, e=ɖirim ‘black’ or ‘dark’, and ɛ=bopho ‘stupid.’ It may also signify
internal quality of an inanimate object such as in e=kokʰela ‘blunt’; i=boe
‘boiled’; e=mɔʈello ‘thick’ and i=phuŋ ‘fully ripe.’
One can explain that inherently relational elements are conceptually
dependent on each other and thus also define attributes. The inherent
attribute may include inherent personal attributes such as qualities,
nature, weight, height, size, shape, state of health, temperature, blood
pressure, energy, bodily functions (for example, movement, reaction, uri-
nation), consciousness, courage, fear, name, among others. Refer to Chap-
ter 8 on modification and deixis and Figure 3.2 at the end of this section.
Body parts and attributes of an object are both considered to be inalien-
able possession. Each class marker proclitic from the set of seven decides
the nature of the modifier. Please refer to Chapters 6 and 8 for the full
spectrum of the phenomenon.
The following sentences indicate attribution or inherent quality of the
argument, but are marked by different body division class markers signify-
ing different kinds of attributes.
Since body division class markers are attached to all form classes, each
with a specific meaning, they appear to be grammaticalised to a large
extent. It is not easy to establish a one-to-one correspondence between
these class markers and the class markers used for body division/body
part terms. However, native speakers of the language have no problem
in assigning an appropriate class marker in the case of new adjectives,
new verbs and new nouns. Although it is very difficult to distinguish one
specific kind of meaning from another while analysing each of the seven
divisions represented in verbs, adjectives, adverbs and nouns, one can
represent the process of grammaticalisation for each category as follows.
The length of each arrow signifies the degree of grammaticalisation. The
longer the arrow, the higher the grammaticalisation. Refer to Figure 3.1.
V: hand-related activities
Class 3
‘hand’ ADJ: attributes related to limbs
body part’
oŋ=, toŋ=
ADV: ‘haste’, ‘hurriedly done action’
86 chapter three
V: resultative action
Class 7
‘legs’ ADJ: ‘shape’, ‘texture’
o=, to=
ADV: temporal deixis
We have been using the terms ‘class markers’ and ‘markers’ throughout
the above discussion, without giving a particular name to this form or
structure from the available range of grammatical categories, because we
first wanted to discuss the various ways in which these markers or class
marker forms appear in the language and the varied functions they per-
form. It was observed that the class markers not only define and clas-
sify nouns but also modify other word classes that they are associated
with. As observed above, they pervade adjectives and verbs defining and
distinguishing the semantic types of the verbs and adjectives concerned.
These are grammaticalised forms of the inalienability markers and oper-
ate more like clitics than prefixes. In the absence of any written record
one can speculate that these must be diachronically derived from full lexi-
cal items. Over time, each has developed its own semantics which gov-
erns the larger meaning of the dependent form class, D. This has become
evident as each part of speech form has the potential to take different
class markers designating distinct meanings. Conversely, the same class
marker can be attached to various forms belonging to the same part of
speech designating similar meanings. Morpho-syntactically, they seem to
be closer to the category of ‘clitics.’ We shall now discuss in detail why
these class marker forms operate as proclitics in the language and how
they are different from simple affixes.
These examples illustrate that the same verb root may take a variety of
class markers depending upon the nature of the action and its effects on
the object concerned. The inherent semantics of each of the class markers
determines the specific meaning of the host.
Seen from a structural point of view, the fact that there is no obligatory
rule that they immediately attach to a verb in all constructions and the
fact that they can be moved to a position before a verb phrase as shown
in (55a, 56, 58, 59, and 60) below, makes them good candidates for pro-
clitics. This argument has been widely accepted as the indicator of clitics
(Aikhenvald 2002; Heggie & Ordóñez 2005). They are moveable and are
attached to the subject as in the following case, distancing themselves
from the verbs they are classifying. This allows incorporation of the nomi-
nal object. Affixes certainly do not behave in this manner. In the following
examples the (a) form shows a distanced verbal clitic (the class marker)
and the (b) form represents the immediate attachment of the clitic to its
host.
The semantic difference between (a) and (b) is that of focus, viz. the focus
is on the verb when the proclitic is adjacent to it.
It was discussed in §3.5 that these class markers may also attach to
intransitive verbs. These can also be distanced from their heads, i.e. the
verbs, as the subject noun attracts the class marker or object clitic towards
itself as in sentence (57). Intransitive verbs that are experiential in nature
or those of motion such as, ‘exit’ or ‘leave’ also allow the markers to be
attached to the subject noun (58, 59).
(iii) The third argument for naming these class markers as clitics and not
affixes comes from a prosodic phenomenon of the language. Pronomi-
nal clitics combined with the class markers are rendered as one or two
syllables—simple phonological words in fast speech. Hence, (a) and (c)
are common, but (b) and (d) are not (Note: a word boundary is shown by
the symbol #):
90 chapter three
However, two prefixes combined together do not form a word. The assign-
ment of the status of the word is decided by the prosodic phenomenon of
pause and stress. For further details see §2.5.7 and §2.5.8.
(a) Pronominal clitics (for details refer to §7.3) which are phonetically
reduced forms of pronouns. These are ‘simple clitics’ (Anderson
2005).
(b) The ‘special clitics’, the body division class marker proclitics which
specify classes of various form classes and are attached to the left of
the hosts.
Although all class markers have some distinctive features, they share most
of the properties of being clitics. Since they attach to various form classes
they may be termed as: (i) Nominal proclitics, (ii) Verbal proclitics, and
(iii) Modifier (adjectival/adverbial) proclitics. These are special clitics. All
of them are unstressed monosyllabic words. When added to their hosts,
they operate as compounds.
Out of the six well recognised criteria that Zwicky & Pullum (1983: 305)
suggest for the identification of clitics, four of them are very relevant for
PGA clitics.
(a) Clitics and not affixes are least selective and can attach to any word
of a major word class, such as noun, adjective, adverb and verb. By
this criterion, class markers in PGA are clitics. PGA class markers also
meet at least three other criteria. These are:
(b) There are no unexpected forms or irregularities in clitics. Hosts are
unaffected by clitics.
grammar overview 91
1. Selectivity: they can attach to nouns, verbs, modifiers, and to other clit-
ics. The class markers, as discussed in the sections above, can attach
to all form classes. Moreover, pronominal clitics attach to other class
marker proclitics. The latter quality gives rise to the possibility of clitic
sequencing.
2. The direction of proclitics is prefixing whilst the language is suffixing
in nature. This is the reason they have been termed ‘proclitics’ in this
grammar.
3. Phonotactic restrictions are such that they are monosyllabic in nature,
except one, i.e., class 6 ara=.5 The class marker proclitics always start
with a vowel, unless specified for inanimate nouns. Despite the fact
that the language has ample examples of VV sequences and medial
consonant clusters, the proclitics have the shape of V(C). Pronominal
clitics are constituted of single sounds, either a vowel or a consonant.
4. Two clitics can combine and result in a lexeme, e.g., a pronominal clitic
can combine with body class marker proclitics as in ʈh + ɛr ‘1sg + class
2’ meaning ‘mine.’ PGA thus has ‘clitic only’ words. Also refer to §3.8.3
below. The possibility of lexicalisation of class marker proclitics gives
us constructions like tottaral ‘on’ comprising of tot class 4, tara class 6
and -l ‘locative’ marker. There are other deictic categories which are
derived by lexicalising the proclitics. These will be considered in sub-
sequent chapters.
5 The hypothesis is that this particular proclitic is derived from the combination of two
proclitics, i.e., cl 1 a= and cl 6 ra=.
92 chapter three
dictates the occurrence of these two clitics as one phonological word also
qualifies the phenomenon to be that of clitic sequencing (Spencer 2000:
368). Reconsider the phrases given earlier in §3.8.1 using the criterion (iii)
given there for clitic sequencing. To recall, we repeat two such examples
to show clitic sequencing. We use the symbol = twice to show double
clitics.
The verb ‘cut’ is ut=phoke but the class marker can move and attaches
to the pronominal clitic.
The verb ‘get’ is i=coŋ but as one can see below i= can move next to the
subject.
Although the class markers are compatible with nouns, modifiers and
verbs as lexically governed categories, they move towards pronominal
clitics. This phenomenon is known in PGA as the syllable attraction
rule (refer to §2.5.8). In a construction with a proper noun or a common
noun as a subject, or when the pronominal is in its full form (see below)
the class marker proclitic is not attracted to the pronoun, but is instead
attached to its host as in ut=cuʃu ‘to kill someone with a gun’ or ot=cobi ‘to
kill a large crowd with a gun’, and i=rube ‘to kill by stoning’:
We prefer to name the ina markers proclitics and not prefixes, not only
because they precede the host category, but also because of the reasons
mentioned above in §3.8.1–3.8.3, which exhibit characteristic features that
are never shared by affixes. It appears that these class markers function as
proclitics and are the nucleus to which all the other grammatical catego-
ries such as nouns, verbs and modifiers are attached as satellites.
We saw that the class markers are grammaticalised in the language and
represent an inherent part of the cognitive world of the speakers. While
eliciting these words, whether in the areas of nouns, adjectives or verbs, it
was observed that the native speakers always attached the content words
with these class markers. Thus, when asked for the word ‘slap’, speakers
gave the form ut=bate and not bate. Similarly, the word ‘good’ was not
rendered in isolation as nɔl but i=nɔl. Thus, the various class markers can
be analysed morphologically as:
Proclitics (i) to (iv) may be long distanced from their host as they have the
tendency to be attracted by the pronominal proclitics (v). Figure 3.2 and
Table 3.2 summarise our findings.
To summarise, the class markers define and classify nouns, attributes,
states and actions in the language. The relationship between two nominal
categories or between a nominal and a verbal category (including those
defining a state) is seen as inherent where physical separation, if it exists
at all, does not make them ‘alienable’, as seen in the case of terms for cut
off body parts of animals. The notion of inalienability in the language rep-
resents a conceptual dependency between the object and its possessor. It
pervades transitive verbs where it establishes an inextricable link between
the mode of operation and the effect of an action; or in the case of intran-
sitive verbs it signifies ambience, the resultant experience, or the inherent
patient/object noun which cannot be alienated from the verb. It appears
with modifiers designating an inherent attribute which cannot be sepa-
rated from its locus. Table 3.2 represents the semantics of various proclit-
ics and host nouns, modifiers, and verbs. Some markers, if not all, retain
the basic meaning of body division classes across form classes.
grammar overview 95
Inalienability
INA
Classification
by
modification
SEMANTIC
FUNCTION
MORPHO-SYNTACTIC Proclitics
FUNCTION
Table 3.2. Semantics of body division class markers and bound form classes
Class Body Body Verbs Adjectives Adverbs
division class
classes markers
1 mouth and a= mouth-related mouth-related, deictic meaning
its semantic activity, origin quality of a of front or back,
extension person anteriority of an
action
2 major ɛr= activity in attribute of deictic meaning of
external which the size, external adjacency or front,
body parts front part of beauty uncontrollable
the body is actions/emotions
involved
3 extreme oŋ= hand-related attributes manner:
ends of the activity, action related to ‘hurriedly’
body like to do with limbs
toe and extremities of
fingernails body
4 bodily ut= directional, quality of an emerging out
products and away from X after a part of something,
part-whole the self, is taken out deictic meaning
relationship experiential of it of ‘towards X’, ‘up’
96 chapter three
Introduction
4.1 Affixation
PGA has a number of prefixes and suffixes that attach to roots giving rise
to new words with different meanings from the original un-affixed root.
Sometimes one has to take recourse to a longer translation because equiv-
alents in English are not expressed in two morphemes. The affixes listed
below do not include those affixes which are used as case markers or for
forming plurals. Nor does the list include TAM suffixes as they do not,
prototypically, form new words. Refer to §5.2.2 for various case marking
suffixes and to Chapter 9 on verbs for further information on how the
verbal inflection suffixes operate. The following prefixes and suffixes are
used to form words in the language.
1. /ɛm- ~ em- ~ un- ~ um-/ is a reflexive, as in phe ‘drown’, but ɛm-phe ‘dive’,
bele ‘flow’ and ɛm-bele ‘overflow’, bolo ‘scratch’ and ɛm-bolo ‘scratch oneself ’,
phorol ‘turn’, but em-phorol ‘turn over while sleeping’, cɛkho ‘face’ and em-cɛkho
‘agreed’, etc. If the reflexive is prefixed to an inanimate noun it is preceded
by t-. The reflexive prefix can be infixed between a verbal proclitic and the
verb root, e.g., ɛr=khole ‘to laugh’ can be rendered as ɛr=ɛm-khole ‘to laugh at
98 chapter four
oneself.’ Refer to §7.5 also. For details on marking inanimate nouns refer to
§3.3.4. Consider the following sentence:
The following two affixes are constituted of two elements: a body division
class marker proclitic and a reflexive affix. Each of them, i.e. the combina-
tion of a proclitic and an affix, operates as a single prefix.
2. /ɑrɑm-/ is derived from two bound morphemes, the body division class marker
proclitic ara= and the reflexive affix ɛm- respectively. This is a reflexive mor-
pheme which is attached to verbs designating that the action is directed to
‘self.’ For example laʈ ‘fear’, but aram-laʈ ‘be scared of ’, ben ‘sleep’, but aram-
ben ‘to take rest/lie down/sleep.’ More often than not, this is attached to action
verbs which involve the side of the body. For details refer to §6.2.1.6 and Table
6.14.
If the finite verb is transitive and is preceded by the object clitic it= the
nominalisation marker assimilates to it and the combination of the two,
i.e., clitic and affix contraction pronounced as [bit], constitutes a single
word. Please refer to §2.5.7 and §2.5.8 for details. Consider the following
sentences:
Thus, the word for ‘call’ tere becomes tere-bit ‘for calling’ and ‘burn’ has a
verbal nominal in irulu-bit ‘for burning.’
8. /-iʃo ~ -ico/ is a genitive suffix and indicates alienable possession, e.g. ʈh=ico
julu ‘my clothes’, surmei-iʃo bindi ‘Surmei’s bindi’, niʃo ‘theirs’, reya-ico bol
‘Reya’s rope’, etc. These also describe modification by possession. Since these
are on the borderline of morphology and syntax, they can be considered as
forming new phrases and not new words. These are discussed at length in
Chapter 6 on possession.
100 chapter four
4.2.1 Proclitics
We have so far established that the language offers two types of word
classes, dependent and independent, which are observable in nouns, adjec-
tives and verbs. The dependent classes are marked by the body division
class markers representing special proclitics. Refer to §3.8.2. The com-
bination of various proclitics and content words gives us a large lexicon
spanning various and different semantic fields. For example, the lexeme
tei ‘blood’ when combined with different proclitics designates different
sources of blood. Thus oŋ=tei ‘blood from finger’, er=tei ‘blood from head’,
o=tei ‘blood from legs’, ut=tei ‘oozing out of blood’, e=tei ‘blood inside the
body’ etc. Similarly, the word khir means ‘hot’ but with proclitic 5 as in
e=khir it signifies the ‘state of being hot.’ However, the same lexeme with
proclitic 4 as in ut=khir changes it into a transitive verb meaning ‘heat it.’
These examples can be taken as being constituted of two phonological
words. Thus, ɛr=-co ‘head’ is constituted of two phonological words: ɛr=
‘class 2.poss’ and co ‘head’ and together they constitute a grammatical
word, a noun designating a body part term. Similarly, the word ot=cone
‘go’ is constituted of two phonological words: ot= class 4 and the stem
cone and together, they constitute the verbal category meaning ‘go’ or
‘exit.’ This process of attaching proclitics to dependent nominal, verbal
and modifier categories is a very productive process of deriving words in
the language. We can symbolise this structure as: proclitic + n/v/mod.
The readers may recall that these proclitics are the various body division
class markers that were considered earlier in §3.3. Hence, lea ‘small’, but
a=lea ‘slowly’; ara=karap ‘waist’, but a=karap ‘behind someone’; e=buŋoi
‘beautiful, inner beauty’, but ɛr=buŋoi ‘beautiful to look at’; kaʈa ‘young
girl’, but ara=kata ‘dwarf ’; luk ‘lap’, but ara=luk ‘weigh’; jire ‘speak/speech’,
and a=jire ‘abuse’; ɖelo ‘ball’ and ara=ɖelo ‘pregnant’; and tujuro ‘shake’,
but uŋ=tujuro ‘trembling of hands’, etc. Many adjectives can be derived
from nouns by attaching the special proclitics or object clitics. Consider:
4.2.2 Enclitics
There is one example of an enclitic =n in the language which indicates
plurality. It co- varies with the word nu ~ no for ‘people’ which is also used
as a second constituent of the compound indicating plurality. Thus, cao
nu ‘dogs’ can co-vary with the form cao=n ‘dogs’ without any change in the
meaning. Refer also to §5.1.
(9) ŋo ik=caʈ-o
2sg obj=work-dst.pst
‘What did you do?’
To summarise, PGA has the following clitics, most of them are used to
form new words.
1 They also seem to be derived from some of the body division classes, but it is difficult
to trace their origin due to the limited data available from the last few speakers.
word formation processes 103
• Body division class marker proclitics: seven in all: a=, ɛr=, oŋ=, ut=, e=,
ara=, and o=. Each of these has a number of allomorphs as stated ear-
lier in Chapter 3.
• The causative or applicative proclitic a= ~ ta= can derive transitive
verbs from intransitive or causative from transitive verbs. It can be used
to generate a nominal category as we saw above in §4.2.1 for the word
for ‘chair.’
• Object clitics: ik= and it= and their variant forms.
• Enclitics: only one, which is =n to indicate plurality. It does not form
new words.
PGA forms new words by combining affixes and proclitics, especially with
words in the deictic categories (refer to §2.5.8). For example, the locative
case marker, -l, which is suffixed to a locational noun, can be combined
with a variety of proclitics and affixes to form a word which specifies the
exact location of the object under consideration. Consider Table 4.1.
There are phonological words that are derived from two bound mor-
phemes, each of them bound by the preceding and the following bound
stems. However, the stress pattern and the pause between the words
render these bound morphemes as one phonological word. For instance,
we witnessed earlier in §4.1 that the absolutive -bi, which is suffixed to a
noun, and the object clitic, which is attached to the following verb, can
combine together to form one phonological word, viz. bit as in sentences
6 and 7 above. Similarly, other phonological words that can be derived by
combining various clitics and affixes are the following. Sometimes a word
is derived by combining two clitics and sometimes by combining a clitic
and an affix, and further, rarely by the combination of two affixes:
Table 4.1. Spatial words and the combination of clitics and affixes
English gloss Andamanese form Morphology
on tut=ara:=l cl 4=cl 6=loc
inside k=ut=ara:=l obj=cl 4=cl 6=loc
under tum-bo-l refl-heart-loc
near cu-l near-loc
away ta=cakhu-l cl 1=platform-loc
in front of tɛr=cokhe-l cl 2=face-loc
behind (lit. back) ut=bɔ-l cl 4=back-loc
104 chapter four
In (a–b) the first constituent is a pronominal clitic and the second con-
stituent is either a proclitic (a) or a reflexive prefix (b). Example (c) dem-
onstrates that any pronominal clitic can be combined with case markers
to constitute one phonological word. In (d–f ) the first constituent is
bound to the word on the left and the second is bound to the word on
the right, e.g., [kona-bi] [it=phoke] [‘tendu fruit-abs’] [‘obj=cut’] ‘cut the
tendu fruit’, which, when rendered, has two bound morphemes appear-
ing as one phonological word as in kona bit phoke. Also refer to §2.5.7
and §2.5.8, the syllable attraction rule. These kinds of structures are also
discussed in §9.3.
4.4 Compounding
• N + mod
• N + N (Lexical construct (Fabri 1996))
• N + V or V + N
• N + proclitic + N
• N + proclitic + V
• N + proclitic + mod
compound. Thus, ʃiro tɛr=cɛk (sea cl 2=far) ‘centre of the sea’; bo i=ter=cɛk
(heart cl 5=cl 2=much) ‘kind person, person with a large heart’; tɛkho
tara=kamo (talk cl 6=much) ‘talkative’, are derived by this method.
4.4.1.7 Antonyms
In addition to these compounds, antonyms in PGA can be derived by add-
ing the negative verb pho ‘not’ to a noun or a modifier. Thus:
The negative pho can assume a full lexical status and can be used as a
negative verb with appropriate tense marking. This will be discussed in
Chapter 10.
Since the language uses a dependent marking system for all content
words such as nouns, verbs and modifiers, it is not surprising to see a
combination of cliticised stems forming compounds. The examples that
are found without the body division classes are semantically either non-
compositional compounds, i.e., co-bol (seed–rope) ‘knot’ or endocentric
compounds, e.g., tajiocor-bathe ‘fish-fins’ or cokbi-ʈhomu ‘turtle’s flesh.’ The
endocentric compounds are the most frequent compounds found in the
language.
As stated at the beginning of the chapter, there are no cases of ‘redu-
plication’ in the language. The only examples that were attested were lele
‘swing’ or mimi ‘mother.’ Other Great Andamanese languages such as Bo
and Khora, that are now extinct, seem to have had reduplication of a syl-
lable to form a new word. For instance, a folk song in Bo attested words
like gila gila ‘sitting with arms around knees.’ There was one case of echo-
formation thire-thiro ‘children etc.’ perhaps from the influence of Hindi. All
in all, complete reduplication of a word or of a syllable, or partial redupli-
cation as in echo formation is not a strategy that PGA allows for forming
new words.
Chapter five
Nouns
All in all, PGA marks the following nouns as dependent categories and
thus body part semantics can individuate noun references. One of the
seven class markers discussed earlier precedes these noun categories. Also
refer to Chapter 6 on possession.
Independent nouns are those that refer to the names of flora and fauna,
and generic names for celestial bodies and other words related to the
environment.
These nouns remain invariant in their shape when a case marker,
whether a suffix or an adposition, is suffixed to them.
A minimal nominal word can consist of just of a root, or a proclitic +
root, or root + root (as in compounds), or a combination of the two. Nomi-
nal compounds can be very long as they are formed of multiple roots.
These are quite frequent in the language. Refer to Chapter 4 on word
formation processes.
There are three numbers, singular, dual and plural encoded in pronomi-
nal forms (see §7.1 on pronouns). However, nouns in general are not
marked for duality and plurality. The word nu ~ ne ~ ni ~ in ‘people’ is
suffixed to a few select animate nouns to mark plurality.1 As discussed in
the last chapter this form can also appear as an enclitic =n. For instance,
1 Interestingly, this feature of forming the plural by juxtaposing the word for ‘people’ to
animate nouns is very common in Hindi spoken in the Northeast of India. Thus kutta log
‘dog people’ refers to the additive plural ‘dogs.’
nouns and noun phrases 113
bukhu-ne untele ‘call the ladies’, but mimi=n untele ‘call the mothers.’ Simi-
larly, ese ‘mother in law’, but ese=n ‘mothers-in-law.’ The forms nu, no and
ni can also be used for the word ‘people’ or in a generic sense of ‘and oth-
ers’ independently (4–5). We can safely say that the word nu ‘people’ is
grammaticalised as a plural marker enclitic =n.
The word nu for ‘people’, as can be seen in the following examples, assumes
the subject position and acts as an independent word.
Note further that many words denoting domestic animals, for example
‘dogs’, can have plural forms marked by nu, while others, such as the word
for ‘pigs’ or ‘cats’, cannot.
Another strategy of forming the plural is to use the quantifier for ‘many’
or ‘much’ cɔfe to indicate inanimate as well as non-human animate plu-
rals. Thus, constructions such as pata cɔfe ‘many mushrooms’ or ra cɔfe
‘pigs’ are common. However, quantifier and plural markers may coexist
(11) with human nouns.
5.1.1 Numerals
There are only two numbers available: ‘one’ and ‘two.’ No one in the com-
munity remembers whether they had more than two numbers. Any count
nouns and noun phrases 115
beyond two is ‘many.’ However, one consultant gave us the word incinkɔ
for ‘three.’ Manoharan (1989) notes the word for ‘four’ as maɛxuccol which
was translated by our consultant as ‘the last one.’ An interesting feature
about numerals is that cardinals can act as predicates. Consider:
5.1.2 Gender
There is no grammatical gender in PGA. It does not encode grammatical
gender in agreement, as can be seen from the following examples (17–18).
Nor is gender morphologically coded on nouns. However, the word bukhu
for ‘female’ or ‘woman’ is productively used in a compound formation to
signify female counterparts of male entities. For instance, cao ‘dog’, but
cao-bukhu ‘bitch.’
5.2 Case
So = Sa = O -bi absolutive
A -e or unmarked ergative
5.2.1 Arguments
Human subjects and objects, particularly those with proper names, are
always preceded by class 1 proclitic a= in the Jeru and Bo varieties. This
means the marker is not present in the speech of Khora, Pujjukar and
Sare. Also refer to §3.1.3.
Readers will notice that there exist, throughout the grammar of PGA,
many instances of optional overt representation of any argument mark-
ing, hence zero marking alternates with agentive -e and with absolutive
-bi. This could be due to the process involved in the genesis of koiné or
due to some pragmatic reasons.
Plural subject nouns are not marked for their argument functions. Absence
of markings are shown with a zero symbol ø. However, we could attest a
few cases of plural pronouns marked for argument structure in narration.
See ‘Text’ (sentences 81, 82 and 85) given at the end of the grammar. It
should be mentioned here that the majority of the speakers do not attach
any case marking to plural pronominal forms.
One often hears a sentence without the class 1 proclitic a- and the erga-
tive suffix -e. The Bo variety does not mark the case suffix -e after an agen-
tive nominal (31, 32). Consider:
In a ditransitive construction this case suffix occurs twice but can also be
dropped from one of the object nouns as in (38).
The choice between object marking and non-marking is not free, but is
determined by specificity. This was especially observed in discourse. If the
object is overtly marked it specifies the object under consideration, if it is
unmarked the object under consideration is interpreted as generic.
Another important fact worth noting is that there exists at times, an
optionality between presence and absence of the absolutive marking -bi
on nouns especially if there is an object proclitic or a body division ver-
bal proclitic that attaches to the left of the verb in the same sentence.
In sentence 39 the absolutive -bi is optional and can be dropped (40).
While object marking in casal suffix form has become optional (as dis-
cussed above), in the speech of some speakers, obligatory attachment of
the body class proclitic or object clitic to verbs that indicate the transitiv-
ity of the verb is not optional and is still an inherent and necessary part of
the grammar. In Chapter 3 it was mentioned in §3.1.3 that some speakers
nouns and noun phrases 121
used both markings. Without the sufficient statistical survey of such uses
one can not reach any conclusive judgment for this alternation. Refer to
Chapter 9 on verbs for details.
But
(43) thire mɔco-n-ø er=phuko-ø
child hen-pl-(abs) cl 2=hit-pst
‘The child hit the hens.’
And
(44) thire mɔco-bi er=phuko-ø
child hen-abs cl 2=hit-pst
‘The child hit the hen.’
Or
(46) thire mɔco-nu-bi er=phuko-ø
child hen-people-abs cl 2=hit-pst
‘The child hit the hens.’
This implies that (i) the absolutive -bi attaches to plain nouns which are
stripped of any inflectional endings. (ii) In the case of a noun phrase with
modification the absolutive case marker is suffixed to the whole noun
phrase.
5.2.2.4 Dative
The dative marking is -i suffixed to pronouns, as in ʈhi ‘to me’ or ŋi ‘to you’
as well as to proper nouns. Speakers have been observed dropping it in
conversation as the meaning is ascertained by the context.
5.2.2.5 Instrumental
The instrumental case marker is -ta ~ -te suffixed to nouns of instrument
and initiators in typical causative constructions. One of our speakers, Peje,
used the form te-.
There are no examples where both the instrumental -ta and the causative
ta= are used in the same sentence. It is not very clear whether the suffix
-ta or -te is instrumental or the causative proclitic to the verb ‘cut’ with the
direct object has an incorporated nominal which is not very uncommon
in the language. Refer to §9.6. Sentence (59) given below indicates the
instrumental function while (60) expresses causative function.
124 chapter five
5.2.2.6 Ablative
The ablative and instrumental are homophonous, i.e. -ta ~ -te suffixed to
the noun which indicates the source of separation.
5.2.2.7 Comitative
The comitative morpheme is an adposition, kɔc or bɔco ~ buce, and is
placed in between the two nouns that stand in a comitative relationship
with each other.
Or
(69) a=cao-no ʈh e ʈhi-bi it=ɲyo
cl 1=dog-pl 1sg.abs place(=live)-abs cl 5=home/live
‘Dogs live with us.’ (bi + ot > bit)
5.2.2.8 Purposive/Benefactive
The postposition for the benefactive is khudi which also represents
‘purposive’ or ‘cause’ functions. These are underlined.
5.2.2.9 Directional
The directional case suffixes combine with spatial deixis markers, which
will be referred to in Chapter 8. However, we give here the most common
directional suffix -ak that specifies the goal of an action. It is underlined
in the following examples.
5.2.2.10 Locational
The locative case is represented by -al ~ -il ~ -el ~ -l suffixed to specific
location words designating space. They are also suffixed to nouns which
specifically refer to the goal or source of the location. Some of these
words, as can be seen from the following table, are constituted by two
body class markers.
5.2.2.11 Genitive
PGA is a double marking language: the inalienable possession markings
are procliticised to the head noun, while alienable possession is repre-
sented by a dependent marking on the possessor noun. Possession has
been widely discussed in Chapter 3 and Chapter 6. We should recall that
there are in all, seven types of body class markers, serving the func-
tion of possession, which are attached to the head noun indicating mostly
inherent relationships between the R and the D.
As we saw in Chapter 3 PGA also has dependent marked possessor
nouns when dealing with the feature of alienable nouns. This implies that
the language has both systems: head-marked and dependent-marked; the
previous form is extensively used.
Forms that are not inalienable are marked by the genitive suffixes
-ico ~ -iʃo, which are suffixed to the possessor. These suffixes, in a real
sense, designate possessive relationships. Predictably, this is used only
with the animate possessors. Since the items listed as inalienable or alien-
able are culture-specific, the Great Andamanese have their own listing of
items as typical alienable nouns.2 These follow the possessor nouns with
the genitive -ico or -iʃo.
(104) ɖun-iʃo ko
3du.dist.vis-gen bow
‘Their bow.’
2 Hence, words designating ‘land’, ‘jungle’, ‘upper garments’, ‘lower garments’, ‘dog’,
‘friend’, ‘God’, as well as some kinship terms such as ‘son’ and ‘daughter’ are considered
alienable.
nouns and noun phrases 131
(108) ʈh-ico jo
1sg-gen song
‘My song.’
5.2.2.12 Comparative
Our database does not contain many examples of comparison. The marker
of comparison that sometimes appears is -ta, which is homophonous to
the ablative or instrumental and is suffixed to the noun.
That is, a modified noun precedes the modifier. If the noun is obligatorily
marked by a body class marker then the following adjective is stripped of
any class marker.
5.3.1.1 Descriptive
Adjectives follow the head. In a descriptive noun phrase case markings
are suffixed to the modifier, i.e. to the whole NP (114) and not to the noun
alone.
(111) ɲo nƆl
house good
‘A good house.’
5.3.1.2 Possessive
Alienable
Inalienable
(117) ŋ=ot= bo
2s=cl 4.poss= heart
‘Your heart.’
nouns and noun phrases 133
5.3.1.3 Appositional
An appositional phrase can be created when a non-finite verb followed by
a relative marker modifies the following noun.
5.3.1.4 Quantifiers
As the language is in contact with Hindi we find parallel Hindi and PGA
structures where the modifier precedes, as well as follows, the modified.
The first sentence (120) is an exact Hindi translation, hence the use of the
Hindi numeral das ‘ten’, as well as the use of Hindi word order. These
kinds of constructions are becoming very popular in day-to-day speech.
5.3.1.5 Relativisation
Relativised constructions follow two different word orders. The most
common ones are those where the modified precedes the modifier (123),
a participial phrase which is formed by adding an additional suffix, -e,
indicating the state of the nominal in question (124). The other type of
construction, where the order of modifier and modified is reversed, i.e.,
the modified noun follows the modifier, has arisen due to contact with
Hindi (125).
134 chapter five
The relative marker jukhe is also used for specific reference of a nominal
category. This type of construction parallels Hindi specifier wala (Abbi
2001). The following sentence is an exact morpheme-to-morpheme trans-
lation of the Hindi sentence əccha wala ləɽka mera beʈa hɛ ‘the boy who
is good is my son.’
The coordinated nouns can be marked for plurality by suffixing the plural
marker to the second noun in the linear order.
The optional use of the conjunction can also be seen in phrases with a
comitative reading. Thus:
5.4 Conclusion
3 For the list of languages possessing this function see Aikhenvald (2008: 595).
136 chapter five
PGA offers evidence for the gradual loss of a large number of case mark-
ers ashere are alternating forms without any markings. The context of
conversation decides the pragmatics and semantics of the sentence. The
most interesting feature of PGA is that it denotes case relations on verbs
by attaching proclitics to verb roots. This feature makes overt marking on
nouns redundant. Please refer to Chapter 9.
Chapter Six
Possession
Introduction
(1) ʈh=ot=bo
1sg=cl 4.poss=back
‘My back.’
In (1) the first singular pronominal clitic ʈh= is followed by the appropriate
class marker, viz. class 4, ot= which is selected by the body part term bo
‘back.’ The clitic is attached to the left of the possessed noun, i.e., ot=bo
and not to the possessor noun. However, as seen in Chapter 3, the pro-
nominal clitic has the tendency to attract the possessive proclitic towards
it and hence may appear as if it is attached to the possessor and forms one
word (refer to §2.5.8 and §3.8 for details).
(2) ʈh=a=tat
1sg=cl 1.poss=tongue
‘My tongue.’
(3) ʈh=a=foŋ
1sg=cl 1.poss=cavity
‘My mouth.’
(4) ʈh=a=kεr
1sg=cl 1.poss=throat
‘My throat.’
(5) ʈh=a=may
1sg=cl 1.poss=father
‘My father.’
(6) ʈh=a=mimi
1sg-cl 1.poss=mother
‘My mother.’
Table 6.2. presents some examples of the body part terms attached
to a=.
possession 141
(8) ʈh=εr=ʈɔe
1sg=cl 2.poss=bone (calf)
‘My bone.’
(9) ʈh=εr=boi
1sg=cl 2.poss=spouse
‘My spouse.’
Table 6.3. gives noun forms attached with εr= or er= class marker
proclitic.
Table 6.3. Body part terms with er=, εr= possessive class marker
Present-day Great English gloss English translation
Andamanese
ʈh=εr=co 1sg=class 2=seed/head My head
ʈh=er=kɔbɔ 1sg=class 2=skin My scalp, skin
ʈh=er=mine 1sg=class 2=brain My brain
ʈh=er=beŋ 1sg=class 2=forehead My forehead
ʈh=er=buo 1sg=class 2=ear My ear
142 chapter six
(10) ʈh=ɔŋ=korɔ
1sg=cl 3.poss=palm
‘My palm.’
(11) ʈh=uŋ=kaːra
1sg=cl 3.poss=nails
‘My nails.’
(12) ʈh=uŋ=kenap
1sg=cl 3.poss=finger
‘My finger.’
Table 6.4 gives other noun forms with uŋ= or oŋ= body division class
marker.
possession 143
Table 6.4. Body part terms with oŋ= possessive class marker
Present-day Great English gloss English translation
Andamanese
ʈh=oŋ=kenap 1sg=class 3=finger My fingers
ʈh=oŋ=kara 1sg=class 3=nails My nails
ʈh=oŋ=kɔrɔ 1sg=class 3=hand My palm, hand
ʈh=oŋ=ʈɔː 1sg=class 3=bone My wrist bone
ʈh=oŋ=kenap-cɔkhɔ 1sg=class 3=finger-face My thumb
ʈh=oŋ=kɔrɔ-tot=bɔ 1sg=class 3=hand=class 4=back My back of the hand
ʈh=oŋ=phoŋ 1sg=class 3=cavity My armpit
(13) ʈh=ut=bec
1sg=cl 4.poss=hair
‘My hair.’
(14) ŋ=ut=khirme
2sg=cl 4.poss=sweat
‘Your sweat.’
(16) ʈh=ut=ʈhi
1sg=cl 4.poss=breath
‘My breath.’
Table 6.5. Body part terms with ot=, ɔt=, ut= possessive class marker
Present Great English gloss English translation
Andamanese
ʈh=ot=bεc 1sg=class 4=hair My hair
ʈh=ot=ʈeŋ 1sg=class 4=branch My nape of neck
ʈh=ot=loŋɔ 1sg=class 4=lower part My lower part of neck
ʈh=ot=ʈɔː 1sg=class 4=bone My neck bone
ʈh=ut=khum 1sg=class 4=shoulder My shoulder
ʈh=ot=bɔ 1sg=class 4=back/heart My back (upper)
ʈh=ot=car 1sg=class 4=chest My chest
ʈh=ot=kɔrno 1sg=class 4=lungs My lungs
ʈh=ut=bo-it=dello 1sg=class 4=heart-obj=ball My heart
ʈh=ot=co-to=bat 1sg=class 4=seed=class 4=night My nipple
ʈh=ot=kɔbɔ 1sg=class 4=skin My skin
ʈh=ut=khirme 1sg=class 4=hot/heat My sweat
(18) ʈh=e=sudu
1sg=cl 5.poss=intestine
‘My intestines.’
(19) ʈh=e=teɖu
1sg-=cl 5.poss=pancreas
‘My pancreas.’
possession 145
Table 6.6. Body part terms with e=, i= possessive class marker
Present-day Great English gloss English translation
Andamanese
ʈh=e=tei 1sg=class 5=liquid My blood
ʈh=e=burɔŋo ʈɔː 1sg=class 5=fruit-bone My ribs
ʈh=e=philu 1sg=class 5=belly My stomach
ʈh=e=philu-phet 1sg=class 5=belly-big My belly
ʈh=i=ŋet 1sg=class 5=breathe My navel
ʈh=e=sudu 1sg=class 5=intestines My intestines
ʈh=e=baene 1sg=class 5=covering My covering around intestines
ʈh=e=bi-ʈɔlɔn 1sg=class 5=obj-flower My kidney
ʈh=e=meca 1sg=class 5=liver My liver
ʈh=e=teɖu 1sg=class 5=pancreas My pancreas
ʈh=e=bucɔ 1sg=class 5=lap/mangrove My lap
ʈh=e=cɔrɔkh 1sg=class 5=joint My knee
meŋe=i=ʃoŋo 1pl=class 5=body Our bodies
(21) ŋ=ara=karap
2sg=cl 6.poss=rib cage
‘Your rib cage.’
(22) ŋ=ara=phu
2sg=cl 6.poss=stool
‘Your stool.’
Table 6.7 provides some of the noun forms with ara= noun class marker 6.
146 chapter six
Table 6.7. Body part terms with ara= possessive class marker
Present-day Great English gloss English translation
Andamanese
ʈh=ara=ʈɔlɔ 1sg=class 6=flower My large intestine
ʈh=ara=karap 1sg=class 6=lower back My waist, lower back
ʈh=ara=thɔmo 1sg=class 6=fat/flesh My buttocks
ʈh=ara=karap-thomo 1sg=class 6=lower back-flesh My groin
ʈh=ara=karap-jiriŋe 1sg=class 6=lower back-?? My pelvis
ʈh=ara=ʈεt 1sg=class 6=anus My anus
ʈh=ara=ɖiletmo 1sg=class 6=ball small My bladder
ʈh=ara=ɖomo 1sg=class 6=testicles My testicles
ʈh=ara=ɖomo-tot=kɔbɔ 1sg=class 6=testicles=class My scrotum
5=skin
ʈh=ara=ili 1sg=class 6=urine My urine
aka-ara=phu 3sg-class 6=excretion His stool
(23) ʈh=o=mɔʈɔ
1sg=cl 7.poss=leg
‘My leg.’
(24) ʈh=o=roŋo
1sg=cl 7.poss=ankle
‘My ankle.’
(25) ʈh=o=mɔʈɔ-ʈo=mikhu
1sg=cl 7.poss=leg=cl 7.poss=centre
‘My sole.’
Table 6.8. Body part terms with o=, ɔ= possessive class marker
Present-day Great English gloss English translation
Andamanese
ʈh=ɔ=mɔʈɔ 1sg=class 7=leg My leg
ʈh=ɔ=mɔʈɔ-tu=jukhu 1sg=class 7=leg-class 4=extension My toe
ʈh=o=mɔʈɔ-to=mikhu 1sg=class 7=leg-class 4=centre My sole
ʈh=o=mɔʈɔ-tara=ɖole 1sg=class 7=leg-class 6=ball My heel
ʈh=ɔ=mɔʈɔ-ʈɔː 1sg=class 7=leg-bone My bone below
knee
ʈh=o=roŋo 1sg=class 7=ankle My ankle
ʈh=o=ʈɔnno 1sg=class 7=semen My semen
stand the clear-cut basis of the seven divisions. What is being presented
here is based on limited field data as language is highly endangered and
moribund. The nature of the language being a koiné has aggravated the
problem. No speaker could explain the justification for such divisions. All
we can say is that (1) there are, in general, seven primary divisions that
help the Great Andamanese to conceptually perceive the human body
and in turn, other associated relationships, and (2) it can be inferred
from the forms given in these tables that the body part terminology in
PGA represents the relationship of body parts to a ‘person’ or ‘self’ and
not as a part-whole relationship. This is reflected in the majority of sim-
plex morphemes.
In general, it can be argued that inalienable possession of anatomical
terms is the basic semantic relation of this domain. The possessor in this
context is the human being and not the body. Thus, ‘my leg’ or ‘his head’
are more readily elicitable expressions in PGA than say, ‘the leg is part
of my body.’ In fact, this is the only way body parts in this language are
expressed; the body parts are obligatorily ‘possessed.’
All of the primary body parts are possessed by the self and thereby
have the obligatory pronominal clitic or a noun as the possessor. It can be
argued in this light that there are certain body parts that are less salient
than others and hence are treated as second order body parts (see §6.5).
They derive their names either by means of descriptive terms or by jux-
taposing two primary part names, whereas the primary body parts are
primary lexemes used with a body division class marker with refer-
ence to the possessor. The choice of the class marker in the case of
the first order body parts is semantic in nature and varies according to the
perceptual division of the entire human body by the Great Andamanese.
In this sense the clustering of body parts into divisions/areas of the body
148 chapter six
Table 6.9. Frequency of occurrence of body division class markers with body
part terminology
Noun Class Body division class markers Frequency of use
class 2 er=, εr= 36%
class 5 e=, i= 17%
class 4 ot=, ɔt=, ut= 13%
class 6 ara= 12%
class 1 a= 9%
class 7 o= ɔ= 7%
class 3 oŋ=, uŋ= 6%
possession 149
Only five out of the seven class markers are used to represent kinship
terms. It is challenging to discover why only these are reserved for kin-
ship terms while others are rejected. They are: ut=, εr=, a=, o=, and ara=.
An additional factor to be considered is that ‘higher (generation)’ kin are
designated by ‘mouth cavity and related body parts.’
With the exception of the terms for ‘son’ and ‘daughter’, which are
marked alienable genitive suffixes, all other terms are attached with
inalienable markers1 symbolised by body division class markers. While
speakers were able to provide kinship terms through direct elicitation,
they were not used in natural language. Elicitation of these terms was
not easy and this explains the significant variation in the terms elicited
from different speakers. Most, if not all of the speakers, use Hindi terms
now. Table 6.10 presents a variety of class markers attached to terms for
kinship relations.
1 However, the nouns for ‘son’ and ‘daughter’ were marked by ‘possessive prefixes’ in
the extinct South Andaman languages such as Âkà-Bêa. Man (1923: 158–159) cites examples
of kinship terms including the ones used for ‘son’ and ‘daughter’ that are preceded by
possessive prefixes.
150 chapter six
The possessives function on twin levels in PGA (Som 2006), namely the
primary and the secondary. Those with a primary level of function are
used with reference to the ‘self’ and denote the major body divisions/
areas and the main kinship terms that were considered above. Those with
a secondary level of possession are used for denoting those body parts
that are extension of the major parts, e.g. ‘eyelashes’, and those kinship
terms that are descriptive, e.g. the ones used for siblings. These are added
to the basic ones. The language uses double markings to refer to words
for ‘eyelashes’, ‘tears’, etc. and those kinship terms which are perceived
as of a secondary nature. We shall now briefly discuss the formation of
these constructions. It can be argued that there are certain body parts that
are less salient than others and hence are treated as second order body
parts. They derive their names either by means of descriptive terms or by
juxtaposing two primary part names, whereas the primary body parts are
primary lexemes used with an appropriate class marker with reference to
the possessor.
In addition to attaching the various class markers to the possessed
nouns, the language offers two more strategies to derive inalienable
constructions: (1) compounding or juxtaposition of two nouns and,
(2) syntactic derivation where a combination of more than two devices
is used.
6.5.1 Juxtaposition/Compounds
The dependent and the head nouns are juxtaposed (in that order) to form
a compound. However, it should be noted that the head and the depen-
dent noun do not stand in a ‘possessive’ relationship, but a descriptive
one where the dependent noun indicates the type of entity that is being
referred to by the head noun. In the examples given below the second
noun, i.e. the head, designates a generic entity while the first noun, which
is a dependent noun, indicates the type or class to which the designated
entity belongs.
152 chapter six
It should be recalled that the ordering of the modifier and the modified
in compounds is the reverse of the order in adjective phrases where the
modifier follows the modified, i.e., the head. For example, cokbi nɔl ‘turtle
good’ and cokbi cae ‘turtle bad.’ Since the genitive precedes the possessed
noun in PGA (§3.1.2 and §6.7.1 below) and the order of the constituents in
possessive constructions is dependent followed by head (§6.2 above), one
cannot rule out the possibility that the typical structure of dependent and
head nouns with intervening class markers gave way to juxtaposition or
compounding in the order dependent-head while losing the class marker
in this journey of evolution. Compounding devices appear to be used as a
defining category rather than indicating the relationship of the possessor
and the possessed.
This implies that the head of the possessive construction is the leftmost
constituent of the compound (given in innermost brackets) because it
governs the type of the appropriate class marker. It is tap ‘chin’ and jukhu
‘the area above lip’ that dictate the presence of the class 2 εr= while the
word karap ‘waist’ governs the occurrence of the class 6 ara=. It is an
interesting finding as the semantic head in these constructions is the
rightmost nominal. Kinship terms such as ‘father-in-law’ or ‘mother-in-
law’ are also formed by this process.
It is interesting to note that two different class markers (i.e., ut= and
ara=) are chosen for designating elder and younger sibling relationships
although both of them describe temporality.
A combination of double marking complex structures and adverbial
use-constructions can be presented in a tabulated form. The lexeme thu is
grammaticalised and serves as the second possessive marker, symbolised
by small caps (born.poss), the first being represented by the specific body
division class marker. hence the construction here is:
(S 6.4) R-poss-D
{
R-poss-D
That is:
(S 6.5) [[R [R poss.1=DR] poss.2=D]]
For an explanation as to why the poss tut= is used instead of ut=, or tara=
is used instead of ara=, refer to §6.6.
6.5.2.5 Summary
To summarise the discussion so far, one can claim that there are twin
levels of possession functioning in PGA: the primary and the secondary.
The primary level is used with reference to the self, which denotes the
divisions/areas of the body and the main kinship terms. The secondary
level of possession is used for denoting those body parts that are derived
from the major seven divisions/areas and those kinship terms that are
relational in nature, e.g. the ones used for siblings. Cliticisation, juxtaposi-
tion/compounding and syntactic derivation are three processes that are
employed in relating the possessor and the possessed nominals.
These different processes in combination with the seven body division
class markers discussed above result in as many as twelve different forms
of possessives. These are selected on the basis of ethno-semantic divisions
in the language. These are given in Table 6.15. Although some languages are
known to offer multiple grammatical devices to designate body part terms
(cf. Ndjebbana or Kunibidijig, a non-Pama-Nyungan language as described
in McKay 1996), PGA appears to be unique in its own devices.
Despite the fact that there was a substantial amount of variation of
forms noted among speakers, it was found that all speakers were consis-
tent in using the body division class markers with body part terms and
kinship terms. The available choice in class markers and their associated
D (for example, I have no idea why ‘head’ and ‘arm’ are expressed by the
same class marker) do not give a very coherent semantic and cognitive
explanation as of yet. This could be because the system is very ancient
and has acquired some irregularities over time. Or, as the language is
a koiné as well as being of a ‘mixed’ nature, different language systems
merged creating some inconsistencies. As mentioned in Chapter 1, speak-
ers of PGA are drawn from mutually intelligible different speech varieties.
Their common language, which is used as a lingua franca, bears traces of
their respective pre-existing dialects.
6.6.1 Inanimate
As mentioned briefly in Chapter 3, the fundamental division of animacy
plays an important role in deciding the phonetic shape of the base form
possession 157
of the class marker. If the possessor noun is inanimate, the class marker
is prefixed by a dental consonant t-, otherwise with all animate possess-
ors, both human and non-human, class markers begin with a vowel. Thus,
possessive class markers ara=, ot=, etc., which are indicators of animate
possessors, will be rendered as tara=, tot= respectively, if the possessors
are inanimate beings. Thus, live animals and their body parts will be
marked by a class marker without the initial t- sound as is the case with
human body parts. However, when the part is cut and segregated from
the body the associated marker will be prefixed with t‑. This entails that
an intact body part belongs to one particular class and a detached one
is treated differently, but still belongs to the same class. Consider the
following in bold:
(53) moco ara=moʈho ‘hen’s leg’ but moco tara=moʈho ‘chicken leg’ [cut]
(54) kƆrƆiŋ ot=car ‘dugong’s chest’ but kƆrƆiŋ tot=car ‘chest of dugong’
[cut, or shown from a distance]
(55) kheŋe ra=uli ‘cat’s tail’ but kheŋe tara=uli ‘cat’s tail’ [cut]
(56) εrεn e=meca ‘deer’s intestines’ but εrεn te=meca ‘deer’s intestines’
[extracted]
(57) kɔrɔiɲ e=tei ‘dugong’s blood’ but kɔrɔiɲ te=tei ‘dugong’s blood’
[extracted]
But not
*cokbi-ico ʈhomu
turtle-gen flesh (alienable suffix)
Sentences such as ‘my turtle’s meat’ will use the compound form in (60) pre-
ceded by the first person possessor as in ʈh=ico cokbi ʈhomu. Although gram-
matically correct, this form does not exist in the language. The author never
observed anyone claiming ownership of food items or other consumables.
Why is it that despite being separated from the body the cut up part is
still in the realm of inalienability and is not considered on a par with the
alienated possession for which there is a separate marking -ico ~ -iʃo? The
answer to this question lies in the fact that the speakers realise the inher-
ency of these parts being conceptually dependent on each other despite
being separated. There is less conceptual distance between R and D and
thus, more intimate bonding between the two is realised. This bonding
is retained even after physical separation. Conceptual dependency is
assigned by the prime factor of non-transferability. It is the inability to
transfer the D to different Rs that makes these objects and parts inalien-
able. It is not possible to transfer body parts, affinal or non-affinal kinship,
or the inherent attributes (as seen in §3.3) of an object or a person, even
if they are transient and ephemeral. This is the reason that even after
segregating the body part of animals as in the cases illustrated above it
still retains the body division class marker. As the discussion given in
Chapter 9 on adjectives reveals, the inherent qualifications of a person
or an object are signified by attaching a body division class marker to
each of the modifiers following the possessor noun as in kaba er=buŋoi
a=kata ‘Kaba is beautiful and short’ (class 2 and class 1). The objects
that can be transferred or are expected to be transferred, such as goods in
the market, household goods of a not very intimate nature, objects of the
jungle such as trees, plants, creepers, and other objects of natural envi-
ronment do not obligatorily take ina marker. These may be considered
independent nouns.
In this context one cannot understand why the words for ‘daughter’
and ‘son’ in the language are considered ‘alienable’ and thus demands
genitive marker -ico ~ -iʃo suffixed to the possessor while the word for
‘child’ takes the body division class marker ut= which is added to its
possession 159
Thus each body division class marker can be used for objects and their
various parts on the same paradigm as body and body parts. The analogy
is very clear in examples like (63) where sound emission is considered
equivalent to products of the body. The body products are symbolised by
the ut= ~ ot= proclitic. The word for ‘sound’ takes the same clitic since
160 chapter six
sounds ‘emerge’ or ‘are produced’ from some source, and hence is marked
by tot=, or in (61), the mouth of a vessel and mouth of a human take the
same class marker and in (65) ‘behind’ of a human body and ‘behind’ a
mountain are marked by the same class marker.
As far as the analogy is concerned, there is no surprise that the hunter-
gatherer society visualizes ‘tree’ as a body and its different parts as belong-
ing to the tree as a whole. Consider Table 6.13, which provides the names
of different parts of a tree each attached to an appropriate inalienable ina
marker designated in body division class markers. It is to be noted that
while describing the partonomy of a tree the word ʈɔkho ‘wood’ is used
more often than the word ʈɔŋ ‘tree.’
Some analogical comparisons are interesting. Consider that the word
ʈɔŋ which means ‘hand’ in PGA, but the same word is also used as a word
for ‘tree’ and ‘branch.’ In a similar manner the word tei means blood as
well as ‘gum’, and the word kɔbɔ means ‘skin’ but is also used to refer to
‘bark.’ Interestingly, these analogies are not unidirectional, since words
from the semantic domains of plants and trees are also used to denote
some animal body parts. For example ɔrɔ means ‘flowers with fruits’ but
can also be used to denote ‘tail’ of some animal as in cao tara=ɔrɔ ‘dog’s
tail’ and teo tara=ɔrɔ ‘crocodile’s tail.’
1. Kin terms, e.g. ʈh=a= mimi ‘my mother’ (exceptions are ‘son’ and
‘daughter’)
2. Body parts, body products, tattoo, body paint, e.g. ut=ʈole ‘tattoo’
3. Home, village, courtyard, e.g. ŋ=ot=ɲo ‘your house’
4. Associatives such as language, words, names, clan, community, e.g.
εr=liu ‘his name’, a=jirake ‘Jirake’
5. Ailments both physical and mental, e.g. er=eʈεŋe ‘measles’
6. Boat, head gear, cap, covering for private parts made of leaves, clothes
(in modern times) and boat, e.g. er=buke ‘my cap’
7. Spatial terms, e.g. e=julue ‘in front of’
8. Parts of objects or parts of a component, e.g. ɲo tot=tara ‘roof of the
house’
9. Seascape and landscape terms, e.g. buruin ter=tekh-il ‘in the middle of
mountain’
Table 6.14. Possessive class markers defining primary possession and classes of nouns
body division class Body parts Kinship Other Types of Semantic
marker proclitics terms objects classes axioms
animate inanimate
a= ~ at= ta= ~ tat= tongue, mother language, Primary mouth and
mouth father, grand proper 1 extension
parents names of mouth
εr= ~ er= tεr= major body spouse side, near, Primary external
parts, head, name, above, 2 organs,
calf etc. cap, deictic
headgear
oŋ= toŋ= fingers, — — Primary extension
nails 3 of hand,
extremities
162 chapter six
Inalienable possession
Possessed
Possessor (inalienable)
t+V/VC/VCV V/VC/VCV
(67) n=iʃo ko
3pl=gen bow
‘Their bow.’
Syntactic Juxtaposition/
A���xation/cliticisation compounding
Adverbial
Alienable
Inalienable Partitive
Primary Secondary
the semantic nature of the possessor, the possessed, the relation between
the two, and the type of word formation processes. Out of these twelve,
only one marker is used for alienable possession: the genitive marker.
The inanimate possessor represented by initial t- is considered the twelfth
one which is demonstrated along with the forms used with animate pos-
sessor in Table 6.15. The rest are seen as variations of inalienable pos-
sessions. By taking into account a large inventory of nominal objects, it
becomes eminently clear that the terms alienable versus inalienable are
not very appropriate for the language under analysis. It should be under-
stood as the phenomenon of ‘inherency.’ The large variety of possessive
class markers as observed in PGA is unusual.
A summary of all the body division class markers serving as posses-
sive proclitics, and genitives is provided in Table 6.15. These categories
are divided into two hierarchical levels, the primary and the secondary.
The decision to categorise them as primary or secondary is based on their
morphological shape. Monomorphemic constructions are primary posses-
sions while those which use derivation or compounding are the second-
ary ones. Another reason to divide them into primary and secondary is
the semantics of body parts. The secondary body part terms define part
of a whole or a part of a component or a combination of two or three
body parts.
possession 165
As said earlier, inalienable possession does not take ‘have’ cul ~ cuo. Con-
sider the following sentence without cul ~ cuo.
The word er/εr=cuo-l (class 2 =‘near’-loc) can employ either the copula
bi ~ be or the verb of ‘exist’ jiyo. In fast speech the locative suffix -l is
often dropped.
possession 167
6.10 Conclusion
To conclude, PGA provides seven basic possession types which are derived
from the terms used for the seven divisions of the human body. These are
termed body division class markers which are grammaticalised further
to indicate possession of various inherently dependent objects, both phys-
ical and reified. The body division class markers are attached to the left
of the dependent nouns. Hence, they are proclitics. Thus, the language
has possessed entities as the head of the phrase which are marked by
the possessive morpheme. PGA has provision for further extending these
basic seven types by various word formation devices which indicate modi-
fication and part-to-whole or part-to-component dependency. NP internal
possession can serve as the head or the object of a verb or as a comple-
ment of a phrase. The language is unique in the sense that both possessor
noun as well as the possessed noun determine the choice of an appro-
priate possessive marker. PGA has only one true genitive form which is
suffixed to the possessor noun in alienable possession. This brings forth
another unique feature of the language: it has dependent-marked head
nouns in inalienable possession, as well as dependent-marked non-head
nouns in alienable possession. In all, the language has twelve different
varieties of possessions.
Chapter Seven
Demonstrative pronouns
3sg.dis.vis. di ~ ɖi
3du.dis.vis. ɖuini
3pl.dis.vis. ɖunio
3sg.dis.invis. ɖu ~ ɖuio
3pl.dis.invis. ɖune
3sg.prox1 (nearer to the speaker) khidi
3sg.prox2 (nearer to the hearer) khudi
3pl.prox2 (nearer to the hearer) khudino ~ dia ~ diya
(4) ma jero be
1pl.excl jero cop
‘We (alone) are Jero.’
(a) Demonstrative pronouns are not different from any other personal
pronouns in forming the NP by themselves.
(b) The co-referential property of demonstrative pronouns can be
explained as such: they serve an anaphoric function whereby they
substitute a full NP, the reference of which is made earlier in the
discourse. The plurality of demonstrative pronouns such as ɖunio is
co-represented in the temporal adverb ‘morning’ as a plural prefix n-
(16) or the singularity of ɖu is co-represented in the temporal adverb
for ‘morning’ as aka- (17) explicating its full NP structure.
(c) As is true for many languages, the demonstrative pronouns in PGA
can also occur with a noun in an NP (18, 19).
(d) They offer spatial contrast with proximity and distance defined along
parameters of the visibility and invisibility of the referent (16, 17) as well
as the proximity between the speaker and the referent (20, 21 and 22).
Please refer to §8.5 for details on the spatial deictic functions of these
demonstrative pronouns. Also refer to Table 8.12 on demonstrative pro-
nouns in the next chapter.
First, second and third person singular pronouns can occur as reduced
single consonant or vowel clitic forms. Different case markings can be
attached to basic pronouns for an appropriate meaning only after they
are cliticised. Thus,
(a) Pronominal clitics are not attached to the absolutive -bi. However,
case suffixes as agentive -e, accusative/dative -i, genitive -ico/-iʃo, and
ablative -a can be attached to the pronominal clitics.
(b) It is the pronominal clitic which attracts the body class marker of
the following verb or a noun and helps in generating a phonological
word. For details see §2.5.8.
(c) The second person plural form does not have a clitic form.
The following are the pronominal clitics in subject and object positions.
Some positional verbs such as ‘sit’ necessarily take the subject pronomi-
nal clitic, e.g. ʈh=aono ‘I sit.’ The first and second pronominal clitic forms
given above can be attached to such verbs. However, the third person u=
or o= is rendered as aka= (29, 31). This form also occurs if the object is of
the use of a before all human subject and object nouns while others in the
community thought it was optional to use it. It should be noted that pro-
clitics never precede pronominals. Thus, *a=ʈhu is not possible. As seen
in Chapter 3, the class 1 proclitic is grammaticalised for the semantics of
‘origin’, or ‘source.’ Interestingly, the class 1 proclitic is attached to nouns
referring to domestic animals such as ‘dog’, hence a=cao.
(37) ŋu me boʃo-b-e
2sg 1pl.obj thrash-fa-imp
‘You will thrash us.’
pronouns, pronominal and object clitics 177
inherent qualities such as ‘to be bad’ or ‘to be good.’ This implies that the
attributes of ‘good’ and ‘bad’ are internalised in a person. In addition, we
saw in the case of intransitive experiential verbs (§3.5) that all of them are
preceded by body class marker 5 symbolising the experience, emotions or
feelings which are internal to the body. The impact verbs such as ‘wash’
considered above also are accompanied by this object clitic. In the follow-
ing examples both the object noun ‘fever’ and the verb ‘to have fever’ take
the object clitic ε ~ i ~ e to signify an internalised object.
The objects of ‘sing’, ‘embrace’ and ‘eat’ also share the same clitic.
Also refer to Chapter §8.4.1. We can summarise the section on object clit-
ics by saying that the third person object clitics come in varying forms
depending on the size, shape, quality and location of the object nouns
that result from the action. These clitics attach to various verbs on their
left. In addition, they indicate the manner of an action. They attach to
temporal adverbs to designate concepts such as ‘morning’, ‘evening’, etc.
Clitics are important in order to fully comprehend the meaning of a verb
in PGA.
In PGA the forms for the interrogative and the indefinite pronouns are the
same in non-human contexts. The pronoun cay is used for both of these
functions as seen in the following examples.
PGA has two basic interrogative pronouns: one is used for human referents,
such as aʃiu and the other for non-human referents, i.e., cay. The former
has two variants (used by different speakers) given in the following table.
The latter is productively used for deriving all kinds of WH constructions.
It should be noted that the interrogative pronouns can be combined with
a demonstrative pronoun (55). Interestingly, the human interrogative pro-
noun aʃiu can be combined with the interrogative pronoun cay to give the
pronouns, pronominal and object clitics 181
(55) di aʃiu bi
3sg.dem who cop
‘Who is he?’
PGA has both nominal and verbal reflexives. The language has four dif-
ferent kinds of reflexive forms in the area of nominal reflexives. These
are suffixed to the specific pronominal clitic. None of them designates
number or person:
(a) The form -tuŋ indicates a possessive relationship with human nouns
indicating kinship and body parts (65–70);
(b) Reflexive -em ~ -εm denotes specific identification with the subject or
object (71–73) and is attached to verbal forms;
(c) In addition to being suffixed to pronominal proclitics, alternatively, it
can be suffixed to the genitive morpheme -iʃo ~ -ico to show alien-
able reflexive possession (74–76);
(d) Another form is -encuo which co-refers to the pronominal subject
of the clause (82–86) to designate emphasis. The details are as
follows.
pronouns, pronominal and object clitics 183
The reflexive is attached to the pronominal form on its right. The pres-
ence of different pronominal forms does not bring any change in its shape.
However, the pronoun is repeated once. Some examples are in order.
(b) -εm-
Reflexive forms also generate ambient verbs such as ‘become cloudy’ as in erem-
becom, ‘overflow’ embele.
(c) -iʃo-m
(76) ɲe ɲεʃε/o-m ko
2sg 2sg-gen bow
‘Your own bow.’
(d) -εncuo
Related to the forms given in (b) above, reciprocal forms are derived from
the reflexive -εm- by suffixing it to the appropriate proclitic dictated by
the host verb. Hence, it is a lexicalised form of class marker + reflexive,
such as, ara= + -εm-. The initial vowel of the reflexive form -εm- is dropped
if the preceding class marker ends in a vowel. Consider (a)–(c) given
below. This structure is not different from what we considered above in
reflexive forms. It appears that the same form can designate both a reflex-
ive and a reciprocal meaning. The structure of the reciprocal can be speci-
fied as:
The sentence given above has an alternate form with reciprocal aram if
the host verb dictates different proclitic as in ara=julo ‘slander’:
The first syllable in khudi or khidi can combine with the locative mor-
pheme -lel to give the meaning of spatial adverbs, e.g. khulol ‘there’ and
khilel ‘here.’
Introduction
We shall consider all types of modifiers in this chapter, namely, those that
modify nominal categories and those that modify verbal categories. As far
as deixis is concerned, we will only discuss here those words that occur
independently of verbal morphology, but which modify an action, event
or a state temporally or spatially. Thus we exclude from our discussion
tense marking on verb forms, which are taken care of in Chapter 9 on
verbs. This implies that currently we are concerned with the following:
• Adjectives
• Adverbs
• Temporal deixis
• Spatial deixis
8.1 Adjectives
(d) They include not only descriptive adjectives but also quantifiers
and numbers such as ‘only’, ‘some’, ‘single’, ‘many’, ‘first’, ‘third’, etc.
(8 and 11–16).
(e) They can serve as the head of an intransitive predicate with an
optional copula (18, 19).
(f) They are never marked for number.
(g) As mentioned earlier, they are marked for dependency by an appro-
priate body class marker proclitic. If two adjectives are conjoined they
retain their respective proclitics (20).
As can be seen in example (1) the case marker (if any) is suffixed to the
entire NP. Adjectival meaning is also expressed by the verb used in a par-
ticipial construction indicating a stative situation, where it modifies the
preceding noun. Consider the following sentence.
The ordinals behave the same way as the cardinals, i.e., they follow the
nouns they modify and can be used as predicates.
(15) ɲo mεxutta:wlu
house third
‘The third house.’
As expected, they all are preceded by class 5 if used for internal attributes
or class 4 if used for external attributes to describe a noun, e.g., rae=/
i=ɖirim ‘black pig’ and ut=ʈɔle ‘blossoming white, very white.’
The class markers originally refer to a specific body division. In this func-
tion of modification these are grammaticalised to denote various kinds of
attributes. However, as we saw in §3.7, the degree of grammaticalisation
varied from one class to another. Any noun modified by these adjectives
will have the following structure:
(S 8.1) N_CL=MOD
a=thie ‘heavy’
aka=mu ‘dumb’ (Bo)
ta=iʃom ‘soft, slow’
e=kokhela ‘blunt’
i:=ople ‘full of light’
e=phuŋ ‘fully ripe’
e=khui ‘hard’
i=khimil ‘hot’
i=boe ‘boiled’ (Bo)
e=liu-sʃɔŋɔ ‘brave’ (Bo)
et=to:ʈte ‘clean grass’
e=ta-julu-e ‘cool it’, ‘cold’ [resultative]
e=cay ‘dirty’, ‘bad’
e=ʈɔlɔʈ-mo ‘fair’, ‘white’
i=belo ‘flat’
i=let-amo ‘flat as in levelled’ (Bo)
ɛ=tɔk ‘frail’ (Bo)
i:=ople ‘full of light’
e=phuŋ ‘fully ripe’
e=khui ‘hard’
e=kɔʈɔbo ‘hollow’
i=khimil ‘hot’
e=cɛrʈɔk-totla ‘light brown colour’
e=co-loboŋ ‘long headed’, ‘clever’, ‘intelligent’
e=sɑre ‘salty’
ε=lɔr ‘mixed taste’ [salty-sweet-sour]
e=phae-tec ‘dried leaves’
intajionɔl ‘tasty’
lephai ‘thirsty’
phinli ‘throbbing [as in pain]’
bekha ‘useless’
eŋeʈhe ‘very dirty’
etino ‘wet’
ʈaŋʈɔ ‘lean, fatless’
mo ‘small’
tec ‘diminutive’
ilphe ‘indifferent’
totcyo ‘very’
Adverbs or modifiers of verbs can either precede or follow the verb. This is
not an option with adjectives as the position of the latter is fixed in the lan-
guage, i.e. post-nominal. The most common adverbs are those that describe
an action, namely, manner adverbs. Others fall under the paradigm of
198 chapter eight
‘space’ and ‘time’ which are considered in §8.4 and 8.5. Adverbs of manner
follow the verb and may fill the predicate slot. Alternately, they may pre-
cede the finite verb. They also have the potential to occur without a verb
as the meaning of the word includes the meaning contained in the verbal
category (24–26). Adverbs are underlined in the following examples.
(21) ʈhiyo noɳʈokha nu-bi boʃuʈ-ø nol
1sg.emph Jarawa people-abs hit.pst well
‘I bashed Jarawa well.’
(26) enliɑ-k-om
(walk) slowly-fa-npst
‘He walks slowly.’
Some of the manner adverbs are listed below. Proclitics are not attached
to all of them. Where the words have proclitics they are shown by the
symbol =. Some proclitics in manner adverbs are grammaticalised and
hence it is difficult to segregate them from the root morpheme.
ikɑɲɔrɔ-k-e ‘completely’
ɛren-cɛthe ‘angrily’
ekɑ=jirɑ-cɛrel ‘greedily’
eren-tɑ=liu ‘inadvertently’
ɛr=ɔ=pili-phu ‘incessant’
nɔle ‘properly’
e=lia ~ ɑ=leɑ ‘slowly’
ɛr=ʈɔko ~ tɑ=iʃom ‘slowly’
kɑlemo ‘soon’; ‘fast’; ‘early’
itthɑxe ‘still’
er=ɛntɑ-lie ‘unknowingly’
modification including temporal and spatial deixis 199
Another feature worth noting is that the word for morning refers to ‘yes-
terday’ as well as to ‘tomorrow.’ It is the tense marking on the verb that
clarifies the meaning. The word ambikhir appears to be the lexicalised
form of the ‘reflexive pronoun’ + ‘morning’, i.e. am + bikhir. Refer to §7.5.
but
The time before the ‘present’ or after the ‘past’ is expressed by affixing the
words for ‘before’ and ‘later’ to the word ambikhir. In forming the words
for ‘the day before yesterday’ or ‘the day after tomorrow’ the construction
is very transparent. Consider:
(29) tɑrɑ=sulu-ʈh=ambikhir
cl 6=earlier-1sg=morning
‘Morning before yesterday’s morning.’
The position of temporal adverbs is not fixed and they can move to the
beginning of the clause or a sentence. They can behave like nouns taking
case markers1 (underlined words).
(32) ko-il ʈh=ɑ=meli-b-om koic
later-loc 1sg=cl 1=return-fa-npst again
‘I will come back here again later.’
1 This pattern is very much like Contact Hindi where case marking can be suffixed to
temporal adverbial words, e.g., dopahər men ‘in the afternoon’, saal men ‘in the year’ and
əndhere men ‘in the dark/night.’
modification including temporal and spatial deixis 201
The temporal adverb may be moved so that it occurs in the middle of the
clause.
(43) ʈhu ino-bi ɖikhɔmɑ i=khue
1sg water-abs already cl 5=drink.imm.pst
‘I have already had water.’
202 chapter eight
(a) bɑt-ter=bel
night-cl 2=overflow
‘Late night.’
(b) ʈh=ut=ɖiu
1sg=cl 4=sun
‘Whole day.’
(c) it=ʈhɑxe
obj=1sg.now
‘Till now.’
(e) meŋ-er=co-ɖiu
1pl.incl-cl 2=head-sun
‘Midday’ Literally: ‘Sun on our head’
Man (1883: 144) writes about the Great Andamanese belief that the best
time to hunt turtle or fish is during the ebb between dawn and the rising
of the waning moon. This period is therefore named aks-tig-pala in the
Âkà-Bêa language. Similarly Man (1923: 182) writes about the best time to
collect shell fish known as astoya in Âkà-Bêa.
The parameters of temporal categorisation have also been discussed in
§1.9.3.1 and §1.9.3.2 and should be referred to to assess the ways the Great
Andamanese measure time historically.
Although modern Great Andamanese houses are no longer made with slop-
ing roofs touching the ground, some terms listed in Table 8.8 are still used.
The table presented below lists different locative words in PGA. The suffix
-l indicates the locative marker. Some of the forms are lexicalised in the
current form of the language. Readers will notice multiple words for the
same concept as the words are drawn from different speakers who are
from different lineages as discussed in the first chapter. Also refer to case
in sections §5.2.2.9 and §5.2.2.10.
208 chapter eight
Some examples:
The various body division class markers explicate various spatial relations
as considered in Table 8.10.
We would like to recall that third person pronouns offer the maximum
variety of forms. See §7.1. One important parameter that was discussed in
Chapter 7 was that the forms were distinguished on the basis of distance
and visibility. The distant but visible object is represented by three distinct
forms depending upon the number of the object in question (Table 8.12).
The distant but invisible objects are represented by two different forms
depending on the singular and plural number of the objects under study.
The subject or object third person is generally cliticised as u or o. Thus, u
~ o refers to any person who is not visible and is equivalent to the English
referent ‘he’, while ɖune ~ ɖuni is reserved for identifying the third person
plural referent ‘they.’ The accusative and dative form of this is unni.
The other category that plays a role in deciding the third person form,
as mentioned in the previous chapter, is the proximity to the addressor. If
the object is very close to the speaker, say, almost at arm’s length, and is
able to be touched, the form khidi is used. However, if the object is closer
to the hearer than to the speaker, the form would be khudi if singular, or
diya and its variant forms if plural.
It should be noted that the plural forms for proximate and distant visible
objects were only attested for one speaker as other speakers no longer use
them. Demonstratives are often identical to locative pro-forms and usu-
ally make a two-way distinction based on either proximity to the speaker
or the relative distance based on the visibility of the object, or the prox-
imity to the hearer. The form marked for intermediate distance, i.e. the
one used for ‘closer to the hearer’, is the least marked and often serves
as the relative clause marker or occasionally a third person pronoun. In
all, the language provides eight different forms of demonstratives given in
Table 8.12. Some of them have more than one variant form.
Locational case suffixes and location words such as tuttara:l ‘on’, ‘on
top of’ and kuttara:l ‘inside’ have already been discussed in §5.2.2.10. Also
refer to Table 5.2 given in Chapter 5. In all, deictic categories are very
complex and are derived by multiple processes. They represent the inter-
play of anthropocentrism and the perception of distance and time by the
Great Andamanese.
Introduction
(S 9.1)
Thus:
(2) ŋu ek=terol-e
2sg obj=sell-imp
‘You sell it.’
(4) oco-ta=cav-om
net-appl/caus=spread-npst
‘(He) casts the net.’
A large number of verbs are individuated by the body division class marker
proclitics, in which body part semantics shift into event type semantic
categories of various kinds. These proclitics combine with verbal roots
of any valence. These are very significant features of the language. Let us
discuss them in detail.
verbs. While the semantics for class 1 are nearly transparent, class 7 is
highly grammaticalised. Not all seven are grammaticalised to the same
degree.
Let us consider verbs of all valencies, intransitive, transitive and caus-
ative, and their association with the body division class markers.
(a) a=jeth
cl 1=vomit like
‘to feel nauseated or uneasy’
(b) Ɛ=colol
cl 5=roll
‘to roll down’
(c) e=biŋe
cl 5=think/remember
‘to think’
(6) ʈh=e=ta=biŋe
1sg=cl 5=app=think
‘I am thinking (of something).’
jome ‘scare’
(a) Ɛ=jome
cl 5=scare
‘be afraid’
(b) ut=jome
cl 4= scare
‘get startled’
The proclitics are very productive and are not very selective as they attach
to a large number of verbs. Their distribution makes sense in terms of the
basic seven body division classifications. Consider class marker 4 ut=,
ot= attached to different verbs retaining the primary meaning of ‘genesis’
the verb and verb complex 217
ʃile ‘aim’
(a) ut=ʃile
cl 4=aim
‘Aim from above’
218 chapter nine
(b) ara=ʃile
cl 6=aim
‘Aim from a distance’
(c) ek=ʃile
obj=aim
‘Aim at’ (resultative)
(d) e=ʃile
cl 5=aim
‘Aim to pierce.’ (internalised action)
pho ‘cut’
(a) ara=pho
cl 6=cut
‘cut down’, ‘fell’
(b) Ɛr=pho
cl 2=cut
‘hit with a stick in the front’
(c) ut=pho
cl 4=cut
‘separate from the source’ (betel nut from its branch)
bate ‘hit’
(a) er=bate
cl 2=slap
‘slap on the face’
(b) ek=bate
obj=slap
‘slap suddenly, unexpected’
(c) ut=bate
cl 4=slap
‘slap (hard)’
the verb and verb complex 219
(d) eren-bate
refl-slap
‘slap oneself ’
lub ‘pick’
(a) Ɛr/e=lubom
cl 2=pick up (from the ground)
(b) it=lubom
obj=pick out, (stones in ‘daal’), weed out, sort out
As said earlier, the body division class markers can change the valency of
the verb.
khir ‘hot’
(10) e=khir be
cl 5=hot cop
‘It is hot.’
(11) ut=khir be
cl 4=hot imp
‘Heat it up.’
bin ‘squeeze’
Both the object clitic and the body division class marker preceding the
verb can coexist in clitic sequencing as in the following examples (14–15).
Although case markings are dropped in fast speech as mentioned earlier
in §5.2 on case, they may also be dropped when the object noun is not
a specific one (14). The object noun ‘box’ in (15) is accompanied by an
absolutive case as it refers to a specific box.
Each body division class marker expresses different body locations when
combined with the verb for ‘tattooing’, but with class 4 which is for ‘gen-
esis’ it has the meaning of ‘uproot.’
ʈɔl ‘tattoo’
The verb ‘to pluck’, ‘break, or ‘disengage’ is ʈɔl which can be employed
with a large number of body division class markers and object clitics to
convey the nature of the action and the object concerned. In (c) above,
the basic meaning of ‘emission’ or ‘separation’ is retained by the clitic. The
combinations of various class markers and the verb root express diverse
meanings, sometimes rendering an idiomatic phrase as in (17).
The verb ‘to see’ which is marked by the internal body division class
marker 5, i.e., e= ~ Ɛ=, changes the meaning if the relational body part
class marker 2 Ɛr= is attached to it.
ole ‘see’
(a) Ɛr=ole
cl 2=see
‘Call someone by gesture.’
(b) Ɛ=ole
cl 5=see
‘see’
All these examples indicate that the body division class markers are
semantically coded with a specific meaning and thus can be attached to
a variety of verbs with the appropriate modification in each case.
As far as object clitics are concerned we observed that each one speci-
fies different readings and, at times, results in idiomatic phrases. For
instance, ek=tara=ʃile ‘to see straight ahead’, or ‘to aim straight ahead.’
However, it can be used as an idiomatic expression as in ‘to keep an eye
on someone.’ Consider:
et=mok ‘leave’
it=nol ‘obj=well’
Thus, what strikes us most from the examples given above is that:
(i) The basic division in the verbs is not between +/- transitive, but is
between +/- dependency, i.e., whether they are preceded by a class
marker proclitic or not. Verbs are either dependent or indepen-
dent. The dependent ones can take any of the seven body division
class markers and/or object clitics.
(ii) The aspect of identifying what kind of body division class marking
proclitic is attached to verb root is important, as the body division
marking changes the meaning and valency of the verb.
(iii) In a typical SOV construction, the object clitic or the body division
class marking proclitic of the verb has the tendency to attach itself
to the absolutive form -bi of the preceding object noun and thus the
object noun in the sentence has the terminal part sounding as [bit]
or [bik]. Refer to §2.5.8 for details.
the verb and verb complex 223
9.4 Reflexive/Self-directed
It has been noticed that the reflexive morpheme can also be infixed
between the root and the tense, aspect and mood categories. Thus:
If the verb has the overt causative/applicative marker ta= the reflexive
marker can be distanced from the verb root, as in:
(i) Proclitics to an indicate direct object and objects which are the result
of an action. These are:
{et= ~ it= ~ ik= ~ ek= ~ Ɛn= and ~ en=}.
(ii) Reflexive prefixes such as {Ɛm- ~ em-}.
(iii) The seven body division class marker proclitics, such as {a=, Ɛr=, oŋ=,
ut=, e=, ara=, and o=}, which are attached to the verb roots.
(iv) The causative / applicative proclitic ata= ~ ta=.
verb, its usual host, but is instead attached to the grammatical category
on the left of the verb form, which serves as the phonological host.1 This
category is often the subject of the sentence. We reiterate the discussion
for the purpose of clarity and to link it with the discussion that follows in
§9.6. Distancing is shown by the line drawing.
There is no difference in meaning between (29a) and (29b), (30) and (31),
and (34) and (35). The verbal proclitics are marked by their floating char-
acteristic feature and never create any ambiguity.
The distancing of the host from its clitic is not restricted to body division
class marker proclitics. It can also occur in the case of causative construc-
tions where the causative proclitic can be distanced from the verb root.
The causative/applicative morpheme may be considered as body division
class marker proclitic number 1, i.e., a= ~ ta= ~ ata= because of it homopho-
nous nature with class 1. However, there are some significant constraints.
The causative morpheme attaches to all kinds of verbs regardless of the
fact that they are related to the concept of ‘emission’ as expected of verbs
taking the number 1 proclitic. Secondly, it precedes independent verbs,
those that are free from any attachment of proclitics, as well as dependent
verbs. At the most, one can speculate that the causative morpheme ta=
and its variant forms were derived from the body division class marker
cl 1 a= ~ ta= and later have been grammaticalised as causation. How-
ever, we do not have any proof thereof. Let us now consider causative and
applicative morphemes in detail: the valency-increasing phenomenon.
PGA employs two strategies to derive causative or applicative forms.
One is by using the morphological causative and the other is by the peri-
phrastic causative. The valency-increasing morpheme, i.e., the causative,
applicative, or transitiviser, morpheme ta= or ata= is prefixed to the verb
root and is thus infixed between the verbal proclitic and the verb root, e.g.
e=bi:ɲe ‘to think’ but e=ta=bi:ɲe ‘make someone think’ or ‘remember.’ This
is applicable to the verbs that obligatory take proclitics and are dependent
verbs. The causative proclitic attaches directly to the verb form if it is an
independent verb without any class proclitic. Let us discuss them.
the verb and verb complex 227
In the case of the verbs with a class marker 5 proclitic signifying inter-
nalised action such as e=khole ‘laugh’, or self-directed action with the
reflexive Ɛm-beno ‘sleep’, the verbal proclitic or verbal reflexive prefix is
dropped and the causative ta= or ata= is directly added to the verb root
as in the following.
The position of the causative morpheme is not rigidly fixed and it can be
prefixed to the entire verb form, i.e., a combination of the verbal proclitic
and the verb root (39). If the indirect object is not specified overtly in a
clause and the verb has a causative marker ta= it implies its latent pres-
ence (40), e.g.:
The causative morpheme can distance itself from the verb with the direct
object or a whole noun phrase preceding the verb form. For example:
In sentence (42c), the second reading, i.e., ‘The baby drank the water with
the help of my mother’ is more appropriate than the first one ‘my mother
fed the baby the water’ as {ta=} is homophonous with the instrumental
suffix {-ta}.2 Also refer to §5.2 on case.
In the following example (43), the proclitic i= is distanced from its
host by the intervening object noun kitab ‘book’ as well as the caus-
ative marker ta=. In the next example (44), the causative marker ta=
is moved further from the verb incorporating the object noun, with the
verbal proclitic still being distanced from its host. In short, the causative
proclitic ta= or ata= is moveable and can be distanced from the verb form
2 This appears to be the ‘case on verbs.’ That is, “the meaning of a morpheme used
in different morphosyntactic contexts changes because of the context itself. . . . Such ‘ver-
satile’ cases can express (i) temporal, causal and other relationships between clauses”
Aikhenvald (2008: 565).
the verb and verb complex 229
and the object noun can be incorporated between the two bound mor-
phemes, the verbal and the causative proclitics and the verb root.
i=ole ‘see’
The applicative/causative ta= can also be distanced from the verb root
and can move to the beginning of the verb phrase as in the following
sentences:
3 The word boundary is measured by the presence of a pause. Thus, speakers give a
pause between ʈhota and the following verb ŋol ‘cry.’
230 chapter nine
Many speakers were observed dropping the causative morpheme ta= and
instead used the verb for ‘ask’ in the first clause for causative reading.
Since the second clause lacks the overt subject in most of the construc-
tions the sentences appear to be sequential in nature.
As these constructions were not shared by the other speakers, one can
speculate that this was a feature of the Bo language, now extinct as we
lost Boa, Sr., the last speaker of the language, in 2010.
To summarise the discussion so far, a very significant fact about PGA
is: dependency in PGA does not necessarily license adjacency. Bound root
forms can be bound long-distance with clitics. The distancing of these
bound forms does not bring any change in meaning.
All verb forms that end in an open syllable are followed by a single con-
sonant -b, or -k or -l as a formative affix before the TAM morphemes are
attached to them.4 This consonant can never terminate the verbal form.
4 In my earlier works on the language (Abbi 2006a, 2006b, and 2009) I had considered
these formative affixes as verb class markers with some specific meanings. A closer look
into the language and subsequent visits to the field revealed that these are phonetic ele-
ments which serve as the linkers between the verb root and the TAM markings.
232 chapter nine
Hence, its status in the grammar is that of an infix. The variations between
these forms are not conditioned by any phonological or semantic environ-
ment. They appear to be remnants of various dialectal variations which
came together to form PGA. The author has observed that these are occa-
sionally dropped in current speech. We will use the non-past form or the
imperative mood marker to explain this phenomenon. Thus, the form for
‘goes’ is constituted of [a proclitic] [verb root] [the formative affix] and
the [non-past tense morpheme -om]. Consider:
(56) ut=cone-b-om
cl 4=go-fa-npst
‘(He) goes’, ‘leaves.’
(57) iji-b/k-om
eat-fa-npst
‘(He) eats.’
(58) ie-k-om
give-fa-npst
‘(He) gives’
(59) et=mo-k-e
obj=leave-fa-imp
‘leave (it)’ (imp)
The verb root ending in a closed syllable, i.e. ending in a consonant, does
not take the formative affix and the TAM suffixes are directly attached to
the verb root.
(60) buʈh-o
fall-dst.pst
‘(it) fell.’
(61) it=ʃir-e
obj=wash-imp
‘Wash it.’
(66) nu aka-uno-ø
Nu 3sg-sit-pst
‘Nu sat down.’
Verbs can be compounded with the final verb taking a zero past tense
marker, e.g.:
5 It is a combination of /du/ ‘deictic for invisible distance’ and /bo/ ‘and’, the
conjunction.
the verb and verb complex 235
Another feature that caught our attention was that some speakers, espe-
cially the Khora speaker, used the -e suffix indiscriminately for the imme-
diate past tense (sentence 74 and 75 below) as well as for the imperative
mood (see §9.8.4). Consider:
As stated above, the fourth type of the past marker -il ~ l is used in a nar-
rative style. Refer to the narrative text given at the end of the grammar.
Its usage as an adverbial subordination with the meaning of ‘after’ has
been termed converb in subordinate clauses (79, 80). For further discus-
sion refer to §10.5.1. Consider the following sentences extracted from a
folk tale:
(77) u ne boi-(e)m-il
3sg pl marry-rec-nrr.pst
‘They got married among themselves.’
9.8.3 Aspect
The aspectual functions of ‘perfective’, ‘iterative’, and ‘prospective’ in PGA
are expressed by adverbial constructions and not by an aspect marker added
to the verb root. Let us first consider the use of adverbial constructions.
Aspect can mark the stage of an action. The prospective aspect is indi-
cated by the combination of the verb ut=jukho ‘about to’ which specifies
anticipatory action, and the verb in a nominalised form (refer to §10.5.4
on action nominals).
The other aspect marker -me which indicates a habitual or generic mean-
ing is attested in the Bo language. Other speakers were not seen using this
suffix. In our database there are very few examples of -me. It has been
noticed that most of the current speakers use -om for indicating the itera-
tive and generic as well as habitual meaning.6 As stated earlier in §9.8.2
the non-past tense marker indicates several meanings and one of them
could be ‘generic.’ The following are examples from Bo.
6 It is not uncommon to use present tense forms for generic or habitual meaning in
languages, e.g., Hindi mƐn məndir jati hun ‘I go to temple (everyday).’
238 chapter nine
9.8.4 Mood
Only one marker of mood was attested by all speakers, viz., imperative.
The imperative mood is symbolised by {-e ~ -be}.7 It is attached to the
formative affix or to the verb root as the case may be. Consider:
(98) aʈ iku-b/k-e
Wood burn-fa-imp
‘Burn the firewood.’
Table 9.2. Verbs with Formative affixes, tense and mood suffixes
Verb Non-past Various past Imperative English gloss
markers
1 cone cone-b-om cone-ø, cone-b-o cone-b-e go, leave
2 ɲa aɲa-b-om aɲa-ø, ɲa-b-o aɲa-b-e bark, eat
3 ŋol ŋol-om ŋol-o ŋol-e cry
4 caʈ caʈ-om caʈ-o caʈ-b-e do
5 ɖe ɖe-b-om ɖe-b-o ɖe-b-e shut up
6 boʃo boʃo-b-om boʃo-b-o boʃo-b-e beat, hit
7 ʈɔlo ʈɔlo-k-om ʈɔlo-k-o ʈɔlo-ke break
8 iye iye-k-om iye-k-o iye-k-e catch
9 iji iji-k-om iji-k-o iji-k-e eat
10 juvu juvu-k-om juvu-k-o juvu-ke fly
11 ʃolo ʃolo-k-om ʃolo-k-o ʃolo-k-e hang
12 tɔle tɔle-k-om tɔle-k-o tɔle-k-e mix
13 uno uno-b/k-om uno-k/b-o uno-k/b-e sit
14 beno beno-b/k-om beno-b/k-o beno-b/k-e sleep
15 eole eole-b-om eol-a, eole-b-o eole-b-e see
16 lele lele-b/k-om lele-b/k-o lele-k/b-e swing
17 Ɛmphil Ɛmphil-om Ɛmphil-o Ɛmphil-e die
18 araʈhul araʈhul-om araʈhul-o araʈhul-e kick
19 ʈhitbol ʈhitbol-om ʈhitbol-o ʈhitbol-e search the
ground/jungle
20 kac kac-om kac-il kac-e make
By prefixing various adverbial words to the verb with the imperative {-be
~ -e} suffix, one can get a reading of the hortative and optative mood.8
Thus:
8 We found only one example of the subjunctive mood koliŋe in our database as shown
in example (106) in the next chapter. Several attempts to obtain similar sentences failed as
speakers preferred to use paratactic constructions. Hence, we presume this mood marker
is no longer used.
240 chapter nine
The copula can also exist with negative verb pho as in:
jiyo ‘exist’
PGA has a related word jiyo signifying ‘existence’ of an entity with refer-
ence to a ‘location’ at a particular point in time.
The word jiyo ~ jiyu can optionally take the copula as in the following.
As should be clear by now, the PGA verb does not agree with any of the
arguments in the sentence. Hence, the verb form remains the same in the
following sentence.
To summarise the discussion, one can present the following table. All ver-
bal affixes and their variants are given in the table. Object clitics and body
division class marker clitics which precede the verbs are not included for
obvious reasons as each of them will vary according to the nature of the
verb. Causative proclitics are also not shown here as the table lists all the
affixes.
The moveable affixes have the potentiality to occur as prefixes as well
as infixes.
While collecting data for PGA, it was noticed that the two verbs ‘come’
and ‘go’ had several forms, each with a distinct meaning. They occur in
both forms, dependent and independent. The latter, the ones without the
body division class marker proclitics, are more common. Readers may find
this list interesting from the point of view of ethnolinguistic information.
Some of the structures have become obsolete as speakers who used them
are no more in this world.
242 chapter nine
The exact meaning of every form is not very clear. Sometimes the mixed
nature of the language gives us more forms than necessary. Consider some
of the examples.
As can be observed, very few of these take proclitics and thus, are free
forms. Readers may refer to the dictionary of the language (Abbi 2012) for
the semantics of verb forms and their variations.
To summarise, a verb root in PGA is free to take any of the seven body
division class markers or any object clitic. It is the combination of the pro-
clitic and the verb root which gives the complete meaning to a sentence.
Chapter Ten
Syntactic organisation
Introduction
PGA is a verb-final language and the order of the constituents is SOV (1).
However, there is some flexibility in the ordering of the word, not only
because of focus, but also because the speakers were observed varying the
order freely. As the language is not used anymore it was difficult to reach
any statistical analysis of the preference of aberration from the prototypi-
cal SOV pattern.
The direct object is next to the verb on its left. Hence the ordering is S IO
DO V.
The order among elements at the same level differs from one group of
languages to another (Shopen 2007: 111–113). Our database of PGA does
not contain demonstratives, adjectives, quantifiers and numerals in the
same phrase. Since adjectives and numerals follow nouns but demonstra-
tives precede nouns we have a mixed ordering system with elements at
the same level.
10.1.2 Genitive
The genitive -ico ~ -iʃo is attached to the possessor noun or pronoun. How-
ever as discussed in Chapter 6, the inalienable possessions are preceded
by possessive body division proclitics. Thus (11) has genitive suffix
but (12) has possessive proclitic.
However, Jeru speakers, such as Surmei, always used the adverb in post-
sentential position.
However, one Jero speaker, Surmei, did use the relative marker jukhe ~
ukhe preceding the noun it modifies as in the following.
10.1.10 Interrogatives
As far as the ordering of the interrogative particle/word and its proposi-
tion are concerned, PGA allows two strategies. Languages generally make
a distinction between question particles, words that signal polar questions
and those which are known as WH-words, which occur in content ques-
tions. In PGA the latter occurs in situ, i.e., in the same position in which a
corresponding non interrogative expression would occur (35). The former,
i.e., the interrogative clause of yes-no questions has no specific question
particle or word. Normally a declarative sentence is spoken with a rising
tone to ask the yes-no question. Thus:
Focus can also be indicated by moving the locative object before the
subject.
seven proclitics on its left. There are two interesting aspects: one, verbal
proclitics are moveable and can be long distanced from their hosts as was
discussed in §9.5; second, if the preceding word to the proclitic ends in an
open syllable the proclitic combines with it like an enclitic in fast speech
§2.5.7 and §2.5.8 or may be rendered as clitic sequencing as discussed
in §3.8.3. Thus, the first line in the example below represents the pho-
netic realisation which is derived from the underlying form given in the
second line:
10.2 Coordination
These constructions are different from the serial verb constructions dis-
cussed below in §10.6.
It has been noticed that speakers drop the comitative marker more often
than not and it is, instead, the context that delivers the appropriate
meaning.
10.3 Negation
Just like any other verb form the negative lexeme can define a state and
thus can be followed by a copula.
One of the Jeru speakers used the negative word khum instead of pho for
negating the psychological verbs such as bɔ ‘understand’ or PGA bɔ ‘ask’
Consider:
(70) ŋu beno-bim
2sg sleep-prohb
‘Do not sleep.’
To show respect to the addressee the Jeru speakers use the negative verb
phu instead of the prohibitive negative marker.
10.4 Interrogatives
Another interesting fact is that forms for indefinite pronouns and ques-
tion words are identical. See §7.4 for details. The question word cay can be
followed by any case marker as the situation demands; cay-bi ‘what-abs’,
cay-ta ‘what-abl/inst’, cay-khudi ‘what-dat’, etc.
(80) di aʃiu be
3sg.dem who cop
‘Who is he?’
The question word combined with the relative marker can be the final
word in the sentence. This means it can take the predicate position as
this is a verbless clause.
There are the modal interrogative words ʃitani and khitani that convey the
meaning of ‘how.’ The order of this word is not rigidly fixed as it can occupy
the initial position as well as the second position in the sentence.
As stated earlier, the question word cay is suffixed with the locative
marker -l to represent a locative interrogative such as the equivalent of
English ‘where’. Thus, PGA has cay-l but since the metathesis rule applies
(see §2.5.9), the word is rendered as cyal ~ cya:l ‘where.’
(b) C
ause and effect. If the relationship between the two events is one
of cause and effect then the first verb is unmarked and the final verb
takes any of the tense markers as the situation demands (94).
(d) V
erb serialisation can also be used as the first verb in the sequence
performs the manner in which the final verb is undertaken (98). In
such cases the first verb is suffixed by the homophonous past marker
reserved for narration, i.e., ‑il or -l and performs the function of the
converb.
(e) The subject identity is maintained in most of the examples unless the
sequence of events are undertaken by two distinct subjects (99–100),
in which case the construction represents chaining of two clauses.
Refer to §10.6.
It can be generalised that if the two actions are undertaken by the same
subject the first event in a sequence of two events serves an adverbial
function followed by the second verb in a finite form with full TAM
markings.
It is observed that PGA does not have the prototypical specific word for
the complement conjunction ‘that.’ Hence, all direct speech involving the
syntactic organisation 261
PGA is losing the complement markers -amo and -khude as these construc-
tions are being replaced by clause chaining. The context always serves as
the clue for the appropriate interpretation.
While discussing verb sequences one cannot ignore the action nominals
in the language as they are derived from non-finite verbs and indicate ‘pur-
pose’ of the action signified by the finite verb. Let us consider them now.
1 This is the only example of the subjunctive mood in our database. Enquiries regarding
getting more constructions of this type did not bring any results.
syntactic organisation 263
The structure of juxtaposing the main clause, i.e., reporting speech and
the reported speech exists for all kinds of subordinate clauses involving
interrogatives, imperatives, and negatives in the reported speech.
Consider the following sentence where the subject pronoun (ʈhu) ‘1sg’ is
gapped.
(c) Thirdly, there are constructions which match exactly the pattern of
Hindi syntax. Consider the following.
Constructions for indicating kinship relations younger and elder than the
speaker are either derived by primary possessive (127) or by secondary
possessive (128) devices as seen in Chapter 6 on possession.
(127) ʈh=ara=ʃulu
1sg=cl 6.poss=later
‘Younger than me.’
(128) ʈh=ut=toa-thu
1sg=cl 4–=before-born
‘My elder brother.’
2 This means PGA has both non-verbal predicates as discussed in Chapter 3 and exem-
plified elsewhere in the grammar as well as non verbal clauses.
syntactic organisation 267
10.8 Relativisation
We briefly mentioned in §10.1.9 that the relative marker is not used much
except in Jeru. Other Great Andamanese languages use paratactic strat-
egies and participial constructions. In all, PGA uses the following three
structures for relativisation described below in (S 10.2–4):
(S 10.3) S_S
(S 10.4) NP (Vstate)
The following example does not have the verb ‘cut’ in the participial but in
the root form. It acts as a verbal modifier modifying the ‘pig’ ra. This looks
remarkably similar to the Dravidian relative participle strategy which has
no accessibility hierarchy restrictions.
To summarise the discussion for this section and the others considered
above, it seems that the impending death of the language is forcing the
language to adopt more paratactic structures and to do away with embed-
ding and complex structures. The simplification could also have been the
result of the ‘mixed’ nature of the language as variation is very evident
and to mitigate the variation speakers accommodate to simple structures.
One could also hypothesise that the simple structures are the remnant of
the old Great Andamanese language. The anthropocentrism that is repre-
sented in the body division classes pervading all content words appears
to be the clue for the initial stages of language evolution. The antiquity of
the language, genetic study of the community and the long isolation of the
community with the rest of the world add to this hypothesis.
Appendix A
Appendix A
Lico’s genealogical a���liation and her language pro��le
Lico’s genealogical affiliation and her language profile
Figure A1
Note: Names in parentheses indicate names of the tribe and the language
of the person. The symbol = means ‘married to.’
Appendix B
Methodology
The following words were tested to analyse initial, medial and final later-
als across speakers.
Analysis
The laterals in Peje’s speech are the result of a combination of two dif-
ferent articulations, lateral [l] and an approximant [w]: the former is an
alveolar lateral and the latter is a bilabial approximant. In other words, the
tongue is positioned as if to produce a lateral, but the lips are configured
276 appendix b
lɛ:c arrow_mono
0.0987723133 0.23835005
5000
F4
4000
3000 F3
2000
F2
1000
F1
0
0.09877 0.2384
Time(s)
lɛ:c_arrow_Nao_mono
0.412049691 0.495232858
5000
F4
4000
3000 F3
2000
F2
1000
F1
0
0.412 0.4952
Time(s)
Figure B4. Formant Structure of [l] (Nao Jr.)
278 appendix b
Formant Structure
This is a creation myth and was told to me by Nao Jr.1 on the night of 21st
January 2006.2 Nao was so fascinated by this particular story that he never
tired of telling it again and again as he thought this was one of the greatest
love stories that he ever heard in his life. He told me that creating one’s
own partner according to one’s own liking was the best part of the story.
In many respects it is a complete story as it mentions five basic elements
of life, i.e., fire, water, earth, sky/space and air.
5. kacole-me
shave the arrow [make]-hab
‘He used to make bows and arrows.’
19. u ik=khuni-ø
3sg obj=return-pst
‘He (took some of it and) returned.’
21. o i=ta=phai-ø
3sg obj=tr=dry-pst
‘He dried it.’
50. u ne boi-(e)m-il
3 pl marry-rec-nrr.pst
‘They married each other.’
7 Pharako is a kind of rope which is made out of a creeper by the same name, i.e.,
‘pharako.’ The rope is especially known for its strength.
the great narrative of phertajido 285
60. u ɛr=ʈeʈer-el
3sg cl 2=got caught/entangled-nrr.pst
‘It (the rope) was stuck.’
63. (e)k.ɔ=cop-ø
3 obj.cl 7=tie-pst
‘He tied it (the rope).’
bamboo. He roamed here and there, searched for food and lived alone. He
spent his time making bows and arrows.
One day, he shot the arrows here and there in all directions. Next day,
he went to search for the shot arrows. When he searched for the arrows,
he found a spring and drank the water from it and thus discovered drink-
ing water.
He went to look for more of the arrows and found one hidden in the
roots of a potato plant. He thus found the potato and took some with
him.
He looked for more of the shot arrows. This time he found the third
arrow in heap of incense (dhoop). He took a bit of the dhoop with him.
He went to look for more of the shot arrows. This time he found a very
fine soil of Kaut. He took some of this also with him. He made pots out of
the soil. He dried them to harden. When the pots dried and became hard,
he put the potatoes in the pot and boiled them. He ate those potatoes and
thus enjoyed the meal of boiled potatoes.
While eating potatoes, an idea came to him to carve a sculpture from
the remaining Kaut.
He wasted no time and in a few days made a human look-alike dummy
out of the Kaut soil.
He put this dummy on a raised platform and burnt some fire under it
so as to dry it well. Thereafter, he resumed his bow-making.
He would look at the dummy on the platform again and again while he
was busy making his bows and arrows. He ensured that the Kaut was lying
on the platform and did not fall down. He would occasionally get up and
go to the platform to put some more wood into the fire and then would
come back to his bow-shaving job. After a while he looked back again.
He was surprised. The platform shook as the female figure of Kaut
turned over. Phertajido was overwhelmed. He was immensely satisfied
with his work. He stood up again to kindle the fire and complete the job
of drying the figure.
When he got tired of making bows, he went into the jungle to hunt,
leaving the Kaut on the platform for it to be dried completely. He found
game and proceeded home with it.
As he approached home, he glanced from a distance at the platform. He
found nothing on the platform. The platform was empty. He was shocked.
His felt dejected and lost.
He put down the game and sighed, “Where did Kaut disappear?” He
was feeling sad. He sat down on the ground with a heavy heart. He did
the great narrative of phertajido 289
not know what to do next. He was oblivious of the fact that the lady Kaut
was inside the house.
Kaut saw Pertajido from inside the house and started laughing. She
laughed, and laughed until she got tired of it. Surprised, Phertajido looked
back. He saw Kaut sitting inside the house laughing merrily.
Phertajido ran to her. He embraced Kaut and burst in tears out of
the sheer joy of discovering Kaut. After that, both of them started living
together as husband and wife. They had many children. Their children
married among themselves and thus their clan increased by leaps and
bounds.
Phertajido once asked his wife Kaut to make ropes.
He went to bring a creeper (pharako), found in the jungle which is good
for making rope, and came back with it.
He asked his wife to peel the creeper and make rope of it.
His wife followed his advice and made a very long rope. It was so long
that it coiled in the shape of a heap.
Phertajido tied a stone at the head of the rope. He swirled and swirled
the rope several times and finally threw it up in the sky. He pulled back
the rope and found that the rope was entangled somewhere and would
not come down. He twisted the rope to make it harder. The rope tight-
ened and stiffened. He tugged at it, but the rope would not move. He
knew that the rope was stuck somewhere.
He went to call Kaut. He said to her, “I will go up above the clouds. I
will go and see the place above us. I will find out what the place looks like.
I will go there tomorrow.”
Next day he climbed up above the clouds.
He saw the place and was surprised to find many people like himself.
He came back to the earth and told his wife about this. He told her that
the place above them was nice and there were many people like the peo-
ple of Andaman. He suggested to her that both of them should go there.
Kaut did not like his suggestion. She said, “How can we leave our chil-
dren’s place?”
Phertajido said, “We will convince our children and then we will go.”
He gathered his children at one place. He tried to convince them. Pher-
tajido said, “My dear children, please keep silent for a while. Your father
and your mother are speaking to you. We will no longer stay here on this
earth. We will go up above the clouds. You should live your life well here.
Our time here is completed. Now we will go.”
Thus saying, they went up above the clouds through the rope. Once
they had reached the top, they cut the rope off from above.
Bibliography
Abbi, Anvita. 1991. Reduplication in South Asian Languages. An Areal, Typological and His-
torical Study. New Delhi. Allied Publishers.
——. 2001. A Manual of Linguistic Field Work and Structures of Indian Languages. Munich:
Lincom Europa.
——. 2003. Vanishing Voices of the Languages of the Andaman Islands. Paper presented at
the Max Planck Institute of Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, June 13.
——. 2005. “Is Great Andamanese Typologically Divergent from the Standard Aver-
age Andamanese?” Paper presented at ALT VI, Max Planck Institute, Padang,
Indonesia.
——. 2006a. Endangered Languages of the Andaman Islands. München: Lincom GmbH.
——. 2006b. “Vanishing Voices: A Typological Sketch of Great Andamanese.” In: Anju
Saxena & Lars Borin (eds.). Lesser-Known Languages of South Asia. Status and Policies,
Case Studies and Applications of Information Technology. Berlin / New York: Mouton de
Gruyter. 107–123.
——. 2008. Folk Songs of the Great Andamans CD-Rom. New Delhi: Jawaharlal Nehru Uni-
versity.
——. 2009. “Is Great Andamanese Genealogically and Typologically Distinct from Onge
and Jarawa?” Language Sciences 31: 791–812.
——. 2011. “Body Divisions in Great Andamanese: Possessive Classification, the Semantics
of Inherency and Grammaticalization.” Studies in Language 35/4: 735–788.
——. 2012. A Dictionary of the Great Andamanese Language. English-Great
Andamanese-Hindi (with CD Rom) Delhi: Ratna Sagar P. Ltd.
——. 2012. An Ancient Tale from Andaman. Narrated by Nao Jr. Delhi: National Book Trust,
India.
Abbi, Anvita, Bidisha Som & Alok Das. 2007. “Where Have All the Speakers Gone? A Socio-
linguistic Study of the Great Andamanese.” Indian Linguistics 68/3–4: 325–342.
Abbi, Anvita, Nao Jr. & Abhishek Avatans. 2008. Varnamala the Book of Letters of Great
Andamanese. New Delhi: Jawaharlal Nehru University.
Abbi, Anvita & Pramod Kumar. 2010. “In Search of Language Contact Between Jarawa and
Aka-Bea: The Languages of South Andaman.” Paper read at the First Conference on ASJP
and Language Prehistory (ALP-I), Max Planck Institute of Evolutionary Anthropology,
Leipzig, Germany on 18 September 2010.
Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y. 2000. Classifiers: A Typology of Noun Categorization Devices.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
——. 2002. “Typological Parameters for the Study of Clitics, with Special Reference to
Tariana.” In: R. M. W. Dixon & Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald (eds.). Word: A Cross-Linguistic
Typology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 42–78.
——. 2007. “Typological Distinctions in Word-Formation.” In: Timothy Shopen (ed.), Vol.
3. 1–65.
——. 2008. “Versatile Cases.” Journal of Linguistics 44: 565–603.
Allan, Edward J. 1975–1976. “Inalienable Possession in Four Ethiopian Languages.” Afrika
und Übersee 59: 300–307.
Ameka, Felix. 1996. “Body Parts in Ewe Grammar.” In: Hilary Chappell & William McGregor
(eds.). 783–840.
Anderson, R. 2005. Aspects of the Theory of Clitics. Oxford: Oxford University Press (Oxford
Studies in Theoretical Linguistics).
Avtans, Abhishek. 2006. Diectic Categories in Great Andamanese. M.Phil. dissertation, Jawa-
harlal Nehru University, New Delhi.
292 bibliography
Cooper, Zarine. 2002. Archeology and History. Early Settlements in the Andaman Islands.
New Delhi: Oxford University Press.
Crowley, Terry. 1995. “Inalienable Possession in Paamese Grammar.” In: Hilary Chappell &
William McGregor (eds.). 383–432.
Dahl, A. 1985. “The Non-Past Tense.” In: Timothy Shopen (ed.), Vol. 2. 23–26.
Das, Alok. 2006. “Directionality of the Pre-Neolithic Linguistic Spread: Gathering Evidence
Through the Andamanese Languages.” Journal of Philology 1(22): 128–134.
Dasgupta, D. & S. R. Sharma. 1982. A Handbook of Onge Language. Calcutta: Anthropologi-
cal Survey of India.
Diem, Werner. 1986. “Alienable und Inalienable Possession im Semitischen.” Zeitschrift der
deutschen Morgenländischen Gesellschaft 136: 227–291.
Dixon, Robert M. W. 2004. The Jarawara Language of Southern Amazonia. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
——. 2010. Basic Linguistic Theory. Vol. 2. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dryer, Matthew S. 2007. “Clause Types.” In: Timothy Shopen (ed.), Vol. 1. 224–275.
Dutta, P. C. 2004. The Great Andamanese. Past and Present. Kolkata: Anthropological Sur-
vey of India.
Endicott, Phillip, M. Thomas P. Gilbert, Chris Stringer, Carles Lalueza-Fox, Eske Willerslev,
Anders J. Hansen & Alan Cooper. 2003. The Genetic Origins of the Andaman Islanders.
American Journal of Human Genetics 72, Report no. 178.
Evans, N. 1996. “The Syntax and Semantics of Body Part Incorporation in Mayali.” In: Hilary
Chappell & William McGregor (eds.). 65–109.
Fabri, Ray. 1996. “The Construct State and the Pseudo-Construct State in Maltese.” Rivista
di linguistica 8: 229–244.
Fant, Gunnar. 1960. Acoustic Theory of Speech Production. The Hague: Mouton.
Fleck, David. W. 2006. “Body Part Prefixes in Matses: Derivation or Noun Incorporation?”
International Journal of American Linguistics 72/1: 59–96.
Forth, Gregory. 2004. Nage Birds. London: Routledge.
Frederike, Caesar. 1625. Purchas: His Pilgrimes. Eighteen Years of Indian Observations.
Vol. II: 1710.
Ganguli, P. 1966. “Vocabulary of the Negritos of Little Andaman with Grammatical Notes
and Materials.” Bulletin of the Department of Anthropology XV: 1–30.
Gerlach, Birgit & Janet Grijzenhout. 2000. Clitics in Phonology, Morphology and Syntax.
Amsterdam / Philadelphia: Benjamins (Linguistik aktuell / Linguistics today, 36).
Givon, T. 2002. Bio-Linguistics: The Santa Barbara Lectures. Amsterdam / Philadelphia:
Benjamins.
Haspelmath, Martin. 2007. “Coordination.” In: Tomothy Shopen (ed.), Vol. 2. 1–51.
Haspelmath, Martin, Matthew S. Dryer, David Gil & Bernard Comrie (eds.). 2005. The
World Atlas of Language Structures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Hawkins, Roger. 1981. “Towards an Account of the Possessive Constructions: NP’s N and the
N of Np.” Journal of Linguistics 17: 247–269.
Heggie, Lorei & Francisco Ordóñez (Eds.). 2005. Clitics and Affix Combination: Theoreti-
cal Perspectives. Amsterdam / Philadelphia: Benjamins (Linguistik Aktuell / Linguistics
Today, 74).
Heine, Bernd. 1997 [2006 Reprint]. Possession. Cognitive Sources, Forces and Grammatical-
ization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (Cambridge Studies in Linguistics, 83).
Heine, Bernd & Tania Kuteva. 2007. The Genesis of Grammar: A Reconstruction. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Hinnebusch, Thomas J. & Robert S. Kirsner. 1981. “On the Reference of ‘Inalienable Posses-
sion’ in Swahili.” Journal of African Language and Linguistics 2: 1–16.
Hyman, Larry M., Danny Keith Alford & Elizabeth Akpati. 1970. “Inalienable Possession in
Igbo.” Journal of West African Languages 7: 85–101.
Justin, Anstice. 2000. “Who are the Jarawa?” Reproduced by www.andaman.org.
294 bibliography
Kashyap, V. K., T. Sitalaximi, B. N. Sarkar & T. Trivedi. 2003. “Molecular Relatedness of the
Aboriginal Groups of Andaman and Nicobar Islands with Similar Ethnic Populations.”
International Journal of Human Genetics 3/1: 5–11.
Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Maria. 1993. Nominalizations. London: Routledge.
Kumar, Pramod. 2005. Field Notes on Jarawa. Unpublished. Jawaharlal Nehru University,
New Delhi.
——. 2012. Grammar of Jarawa. Ph.D. Dissertation, Max Planck Institute of Evolutionary
Anthropology, Leipzig and Jawaharlal Nehru University, New Delhi.
Ladefoged, Peter & Ian Maddieson 1996. The Sounds of the World’s Languages. UK / USA /
Australia: Blackwell Publishing.
Langacker, Ronald W. 1991. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Descriptive Application. Vol
II. Stanford University Press.
——. 1995. “Possession and Possessive Constructions.” In: John R. Taylor & Robert E.
Maclaury (Eds.). Language and the Cognitive Construal of the World. Berlin / New York:
Mouton De Gruyter (Trends in Linguistics. Studies and Monographs, 82). 51–79.
Levy, Paulette. 1992. “Body Part Prefixes in Papantla Totonac.” Zeitschrift Für Phonetik,
Sprachwissenschaft Und Kommunikationsforschung 45/6: 530–542.
——. 1999. “From ‘Part’ to ‘Shape’: Incorporation in Totonac and the Issue of Classification
by Verbs.” International Journal of American Linguistics 65: 127–175.
Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien. 1914. “L’expression de la Possession dans les Langues Mélanésiennes.”
Memoires de la Société de Linguistique de Paris 19: 96–104.
Lindblom, B. & Ian Maddieson. 1988. “Phonetic Universals in Consonant Systems.” In:
H. Hyman & C. Li (Eds.). Language, Speech and Mind. London: Routledge. 62–79.
Maddieson, Ian. 2005. “Consonant Inventories.” In: Martin Haspelmath et al. (Eds.). 10–13.
Majid, Asifa. 2010. “Words for Parts of the Body.” In: Barbara C. Malt & Phillip Wolff (Eds.).
Words and the Mind. New York / Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Man, E. Horace. 1875–1879. Andaman Vocabulary. Part I (English-Andamanese); Part II
(Andamanese-English). Handwritten Manuscript. London: Royal Anthropological Institute.
——. 1883. “On the Aboriginal Inhabitants of the Andaman Islands.” Journal of the Anthro-
pological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 12: 69–116 (Part I); 117–175 (Part II); 327–
434 (Part III).
——. 1885. On the Aboriginal Inhabitants of the Andaman Islands with Reports of Researches
Into the Language of the South Andaman Islands by A. J. Ellis. London: Anthropological
Institute of Great Britain and Ireland.
——. 1923. A Dictionary of the South Andaman (Âkà-Bêa) Language. Mazgaon, Bombay:
British India Press.
Man, E. Horace & R. C. Temple. 1875-1878. A Grammar of the Bojingyida or South Anda-
man Language. Handwritten Manuscript, Loose Sheets. London: Royal Anthropological
Institute.
Manoharan, S. 1980. “Language of the Present Great Andamanese.” Journal of the Indian
Anthropological Society 15: 43–55.
——. 1983. “Subgrouping Andamanese Group of Languages.” International Journal of Dra-
vidian Linguistics 12/1: 82–95.
——. 1989. A Descriptive and Comparative Study of the Andamanese Language. Calcutta:
Anthropological Survey of India.
Masefield, John (Ed.). 1908. The Travels of Marco Polo the Venetian. London: J. M. Dent
(Everyman’s Library).
Mayank. 2009. ComparatIve Lexicon of Great Andamanese Languages. M.Phil. Dissertation.
Jawaharlal Nehru University, New Delhi.
Mckay, Graham R. 1996. “Body Parts, Possession Marking and Nominal Classes in Ndjéb-
bana.” In: Hilary Chappell & William Mcgregor (Eds.). 293–326.
Muysken, Pieter. 2008. Functional Categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Nichols, Johanna. 1988. “On Alienable and Inalienable Possession.” William Shipley (Ed.).
In Honor of Mary Haas: From the Haas Festival Conference in Native American Linguistic
S. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter. 557–609.
bibliography 295
——. 1992. Linguistic Diversity in Space and Time. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Nichols, Johanna & David A. Peterson. 2008. “N-M Pronouns.” In: Martin Haspelmath, Mat-
thew S. Dryer, David Gil & Bernard Comrie (Eds.). The World Atlas of Language Struc-
tures Online. Munich: Max Planck Digital Library. Chapter 136 and 137. Available Online
At Http://Wals.Info/Feature/137.
Pande, Satish & Anvita Abbi. 2011. Ethno-Ornithology. Birds of Great Andamanese. Names,
Classification and Culture. Pune: Ela Foundation, Bombay Natural History Society and
Oxford University Press.
Portman, M. V. 1887 [1992 Reprint]. A Manual of the Andamanese Languages. Delhi: Manas
Publications.
——. 1898. Notes On the Languages of the South Andaman Group of Tribes. Calcutta: office
of the Superintendent of Government Printing, India.
——. 1899 [1990 Reprint]. A History of Our Relations With the Andamanese. Vols. I–II. New
Delhi: Asian Educational Services.
Radcliffe-Brown, A. R. 1922 [3rd Reprint 1948]. The Andaman Islanders. Illinois: Free Press,
Glencoe.
Ross, Malcom. 2004. “Demonstratives, Local Nouns and Directionals in Oceanic Languages:
A Diachronic Perspective.” In: Gunter Senft (Ed). 15–36.
Sarkar, B. N. & R. Sahani. 2002. “A Demographic and Health Profile of the Jarawas of
Andaman Islands.” In: K. Mukhopadhyay, R. K. Bhattacharya & B. N. Sarkar (Eds.).
Jarawa Contact: Ours with them/theirs with us. Calcutta: Anthropological Survey of India.
132–147.
Schmid, Monika S. 2011. Language Attrition. Key Topics in Sociolinguistics. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Seiler, Hansjakob. 1983. Possession as an Operational Dimension of Language. Tübingen:
Gunter Narr Verlag (Language Universals Series, 2).
——. 2001. “The Operational Basis of Possession. A Dimensional Approach Revisited.” In:
Irène Baron, Michael Herslund & Finn Sørensen (Eds.). 27–40.
Seliger, Herbert W. and Robert Michael Vago Eds. 1991. First Language Attrition. Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Senft, Gunter. 2004. “Aspects of Spatial Deixis in Kilivila.” In: Gunter Senft (Ed.). 59–80.
—— (Ed.). 2004. Deixis and Demonstratives in Oceanic Languages. Canberra: Pacific Lin-
guistics, Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, the Australian National University.
Shibatani, Masayoshi & Sandra Thompson (Eds.). 1996. Grammatical Constructions. Their
Form and Meaning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Shopen, Timothy (Ed.). 2007. Language Typology and Syntactic Description. Vols. 1–3, 2nd
Ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Siegel, J. 1985. “Koinés and Koinéization.” Language and Society 14: 357–378.
Som, Bidisha. 2006. A Lexico-Semantic Study of Great Andamanese: A Thematic Approach.
Ph.D. Dissertation, Jawaharlal Nehru University.
Spanoghe, Annie-Marie. 2001. “(In)alienability and (In)determination in Portuguese.” In:
Irène Baron, Michael Herslund & Finn Sørensen (Eds.). 227–242.
Spencer, Andrew. 2000. “Verbal Clitics in Bulgarian.” In: Birgit Gerlach & Janet Grijzenhout
(Eds.). 355–386.
——. 2011. Towards a Typology of ‘Mixed Categories’. Preprint Copy.
Temple, R. C. 1903 (1994 Reprint, New Delhi). “A Grammar of the Andamanese Language.”
Chapter IV of Part I, Volume 13 of The Census Report on the Andaman and Nicobar
Islands 1901. Port Blair: Superintendent’s Printing Press.
Thangaraj, Kumarasamy, Gyaneshwer Chaubey, Vijay Kumar Singh, Ayyasamy Vanniarajan,
Ismail Thanseem, Alla G. Reddy and Lalji Singh. 2006. “In Situ Origin of Deep Rooting
Lineages of Mitochondrial Macrohaplogroup ‘M’ In India.” Bmc Genomics 7: 151.
Thangaraj, Kumarasamy, Lalji Singh, Alla G. Reddy, V. Raghavendra Rao, Subhash C.
Sehgal, Peter A. Underhill, Melanie Pierson, Ian G. Frame & Erika Hagelberg. 2003.
“Genetic Affinities of the Andaman Islanders. A Vanishing Human Population.” Current
Biology 13: 86–93.
296 bibliography
——. Underhill, Melanie Pierson, Ian G. Frame & Erika Hagelberg. 2005. “Reconstructing
the Origin of Andaman Islanders.” Science 308: 996.
Tsunoda, Tasaku. 1996. “The Possession Cline in Japanese and Other Languages.” In: Hilary
Chappell & William Mcgregor (Eds.). 565–630.
Velázquez-Castillo, Maura. 1996. The Grammar of Possession. Inalienability, Incorporation
and Possessor Ascension in Guarani. Amsterdam / Philadelphia: Benjamins (Studies in
Language Companion Series, 33).
Voeltz, Erhard F. K. 1976. “Inalienable Possession in Sotho.” In: Larry M. Hyman, Leon C.
Jacobson & Russell G. Schuh (Eds.). Papers in African Linguistics in Honor of Wm. E.
Welmers. Los Angeles: University of California (Studies in African Linguistics. Supple-
ment 6). 255–266.
Waelchli, Bernard. 2005. Co-Compounds and Natural Coordination. Oxford: Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Wierzbicka, Anna. 1976. “Mind and Body from a Semantic Point of View.” In: James Mcca-
wley (Ed.). Notes from the Linguistic Underground. New York: Academic Press (Syntax
and Semantics, 7). 129–158.
Wierzbicka, Anna. 1985. Lexicography and Conceptual Analysis. Ann Arbor: Karoma.
Wohlgemuth, Jan & Michael Cysouw (Eds.). 2010. Rethinking Universals: How Rarities Affect
Linguistic Theory. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.
Wunderlich, Dieter & Ray Fabri. 1995. “Minimalist Morphology: An Approach to Inflec-
tion.” Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 14: 236–294.
Zide, Norman & Vishvajit Pandya. 1989. “A Bibliographical Introduction to Andamanese
Linguistics.” Journal of the American Oriental Society 109/ 4: 639–651.
Zwicky, Arnold M. & Geoffrey K. Pullum. 1983. “Cliticization vs. Inflection: English N’t.”
Language 59/3: 502–513.
Websites with various kinds of information and database available on languages of the
Andaman Islands:
• www.andaman.org
• www.andamanese.net
Index
Dependency 66, 78–79, 81–83, 87, 94, 158, Homophonous 70, 117, 124, 131, 135, 226,
167, 190, 193–197, 205, 209, 222, 231 228, 233, 238 n. 7, 258–260, 262, 265
Descriptive adjectives 132, 190 Honey calendar 30–31, 203
Diem, W. 76 Human propensity 77, 190
Dimension 190 Hyman, L. M. 76
Disjunct 253
Dixon, R. M. W. 92 Imperative 238–239
Double marking 65–66, 130, 150–151, Inalienability 16, 66, 76–87, 94, 137–138,
153–155 156–158, 161, 175, 194, 202, 205, 215
Dryer, M. 240, 266 Inalienable 65–66, 77–78, 80–84, 86, 111,
Dual semantic system 65, 76, 90, 214 130–131, 137–139, 143, 147–151, 157–158,
160–167, 175, 178, 216, 243, 245
Enclitic 92, 102–103, 112–113, 121, 135, 251 Inchoative 98
Endangered language xvii, 35, 169 Inclusive 169–170
Endocentric compound 110 Incorporation 66, 82, 88, 123–124, 228
English 7, 12, 18, 25, 68 n. 1, 97, 166, 170, Independent form 113, 117, 241
178, 183, 210, 219, 258, 261, 264, 287 Inherency 66, 76–79, 81, 86, 158, 161, 164,
Epenthetic semi-vowel 41, 44 177
Ergative 67–68, 116, 119–120, 135 Intransitive predicate 190
Ethno-biological 34–36 Intransitive verbs 16, 67, 83, 89, 94, 116,
Ethno-taxonomy 35 134, 215–217, 225
Evans, N. 82
Evidential marker 177 Jangil 5, 11
Exclusive 169–170 Jarawa xvii, 3, 5–6, 11, 13–16, 18, 42, 169,
249
Fleck, D. W. 82 Jeru (Jero) xvii, 8–10, 13–14, 20–21, 23,
Focus 89, 170, 243, 250 25–27, 30, 47, 116, 175, 247, 255–256, 267,
Folk generics 35 273
Folk specifics 35 Justin, A. 14 n. 8
Formant structure 278
Formant transition 278 Kashyap, V. K. 36
Forth, Gregory 35 Key to competency levels 22
Frederike, Caesar 12 Kharia 3
Khora xvii–xviii, 8, 10, 14, 21, 25–27,
Generic 35, 101, 112–113, 116, 120, 151, 200, 47–48, 52–53, 58, 62, 110, 116, 163, 235,
236–237 273
Genitive 67, 99, 106, 130–131, 149, 152, Kirsner, R. S. 76
158–159, 163–164, 167, 174, 176, 182, 184, Koiné xvii, 3, 8–9, 14, 18, 25–26, 28, 37,
245 117, 147, 156
Gerlach, B. 92 Koptjevskaja-Tamm 70
Grammaticalised morpheme 65, 77, 82, Kumar, P. 18
90, 101 Kunibidijig 156
Grizenhout, J. 92 Kurux 3
Boa Senior
Boro, Anvita Abbi, Boa Senior and Peje at Strait