Hancock - Sustained Attention Under Thermal Stress - 1986
Hancock - Sustained Attention Under Thermal Stress - 1986
This article reviews the effects of the thermal environment on vigilance.A reinterpretation of early
and contemporary studies contradicts the existingnotion that vigilanceis facilitatedin ambient tem-
peratures that exceed a comfortable level. Rather, performance is degraded as thermal homeostasis
of the observer is disturbed. Significantbreakdown in capability becomes manifest with measurable
perturbation to deep body temperature. This assertion requires that conditions induce a dynamic
change, as performance is unaffectedwith no variation in deep body temperature and is facilitated
when the observer is establishedin a static hyperthermic state. An attentional account of this phe-
nomenon is offered, which contrasts with previous arousal explanations.
A common strategy in experimental psychology is to study a (1950). Although he acknowledged a debt to Head (1926) in
particular phenomenon by examining response under the effects relation to the origin of the term, Mackworth's empirical ob-
of some perturbation or adverse condition. This tactic has the servations provided the fundamental foundation on which cur-
added benefit of also providing information about the action of rent research is built (see Davies & Parasuraman, 1982; Davies
some specific stressor or group of stressors on human capabilities. & Tune, 1970; Mackie, 1977; Warm, 1984). Among Mackworth's
Through this approach, a unitary theoretical account of stress initial experiments was a practical examination of the effect of
and performance has come to prominence, in that the single environmental temperature on watchkeeping ability. His findings
mechanism of arousal has been postulated to subsume the action suggested that performance was optimized at an ambient tem-
of many different stressors on a wide variety of abilities. Recent perature exceeding that of typical thermal comfort. This obser-
theoretical and empirical insights appear to belie such a simple vation was replicated by Pepler (1958), who affirmed an inverted
overall interpretation (e.g., Hancock, 1984b; Hockey & Hamilton, U-shaped relation between performance capability and the level
1983), although it should be noted that the disparate action of of thermal stress. Despite subsequent experimental work, current
differing stressors was first recognized over two decades ago assertions concerning sustenance of attention in heat and cold
(Broadbent, 1963). are essentially derived from these seminal investigations. Thus,
More thorough understanding of the action o f stress may be Davies and Tune (1970) indicated that capability in general was
derived from the examination of evidence concerning one par- vulnerable to thermal effects, whereas latterly, Davies and Para-
ticular type of performance and how it varies under the impact suraman (1982) concluded that mild heat facilitates performance,
of one specific stressor (e.g., Hancock, 1981, 1982). Using this and cold impairs monitoring capability.
tactic, I examine both early and contemporary evidence con- Grether (1973) was more specific in stating that optimal vig-
cerning one order of performance, that of sustained attention or ilance occurs at 80 ~ (26.7 ~ on the effective temperature
vigilance, and its variation under the impact of differing thermal (ET) scale, l which is an index of the thermal environment cal-
environments. Previous views concerning the change in vigilance culated from measures of dry-bulb temperature, relative hu-
capability under hot and cold conditions are not affirmed, and midity, and air velocity (Houghten & Yagloglou, 1923). Grether's
the preponderance of experimental evidence suggests a different value was derived from a mean of the two conditions noted by
perspective from which to view the action of thermal stress and, Mackworth (1950) and Pepler (1958) as those in which superior
potentially, a wide variety of other stressors. performance could be observed. The resultant inverted O-shaped
The genesis of coherent study of the phenomena of vigilance function has been used as support for an arousal account of
is attributed appropriately to the now classic work of Mackworth vigilance performance under thermal stress that remains the only
comprehensive explanatory construct of performance variations
noted (cf. Hancock, 1984b; Poulton, 1977). One requirement
The author gratefully acknowledgesthe support of H. H. Hurt, Jr., for a unitary account of vigilance under thermal stress is the
through the auspices of the Safety Helmet Council of America, and the ability to predict the change in performance as reported by var-
Head Protection Research Laboratory, Department of Safety Science, ious existing studies. The arousal position accomplishes this by
University of Southern California. The comments of K. M. NeweU,
identifying differing arousal states. These states are dependent
M. H. Chignell,B. M. House, and two anonymous reviewerson an earlier
version of this manuscript proved most relevant and their contribution on reaction to the level of the particular stressor at hand. Increase
is also gratefullyacknowledged. in performance level is induced by elevation of arousal, whereas
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to P. A.
Hancock, Department of Safety Science,Institute of Safety and Systems t Alltemperature valuesare expressedin both Farenheitand Centigrade
Management,Universityof Southern California,Los Angeles,California with the first figure being as in the original report referenced with its
90089-0021. scalar equivalentfollowingin parentheses.
263
264 P. A. HANCOCK
depression of arousal reduces performance efficiency (Poulton, individuals at the termination of the exposure. These observations
1977). suggest that the dynamic perturbation to the deep body tem-
The present approach distinguishes between the thermalstates perature of the subject may be an important element in perfor-
of the observer involved. In the following sections, three basic mance variation.
thermal states are presented. The first is a dynamic state in which, The second tendency that Mackworth noted was that the dec-
because of the environmental thermal load, the participant ex- rement in vigilance efficiency due to the stress was palliated by
periences a constant change in deep body temperature away from the experience level of the subject on watch (see also Hancock,
both a normative level and a steady state. The second is an el- in press). This trend was interactive with both increase in ambient
evated level of body temperature, or hyperthermic state, with temperature and time on watch, and indicated that the more
the distinction that the subject has been stabilized in such a experienced the subject at the task at hand, the less they were
condition. Parenthetically, there are currently no corresponding disturbed by the introduction of the thermal stressor. It is salutary
studies that report on performance under equivalent stabilized to note that this seminal work is, in many ways, still the most
hypothermic states. In the third condition the deep body tem- comprehensive empirical examination of sustained attention
perature of the observer does not vary despite the change in under thermal stress to date.
ambient environment. These three states are distinguished for A subsequent study by Pepler (1953) is usually projected as a
both hot and cold conditions. The synthesis of evidence that replication that provides direct support for the position advanced
follows is based on this tripartite differentiation and is followed by Mackworth. This is an oversimplification. Pepler (1953) con-
by a discussion that examines previous and current theoretical ducted two experiments on the identical clock test but with par-
proposals that might account for the results presented. ticipants who were naturally acclimatized to heat through hab-
itation in a tropical locality. Three ambient conditions, 67 ~
E x p e r i m e n t a l Evidence: Sustained A t t e n t i o n U n d e r (19.4 ~ 82 ~ (27.8 ~ and 92 ~ (33.3 ~ ET were used.
T h e r m a l Stress In an initial experiment, after eliminating the results from six
subjects whose data were confounded by artifactual contami-
Heat: Dynamic Change in Deep Body Temperature nation, Pepler found signal omission was greatest and perfor-
Degrades Performance mance poorest at the median 82 ~ (27.8 ~ ET condition.
In Mackworth's (1950) original experiment, subjects were re- These data were in direct disagreement with the initial findings
quired to monitor a hand on a clock face that made sequential of Mackworth some 7 years earlier. Pepler claimed that the ir-
clockwise jumps at 1-s intervals. A full revolution of the hand regularity of experimental exposures and the equivalency of the
consisted of 100 such jumps and the critical signal for response median condition to the ambient tropical climate in which the
was a temporally irregular double j u m p of the clock hand. Al- work was conducted were mainly responsible for the apparently
though immediate response was requested, subjects were also contradictory results.
instructed to respond if they recalled critical signals to which There is an alternate account of these data that is founded on
they had not responded one or more jumps before. A between- two assumptions. The first assumption is the existence of a prac-
subjects design was used to investigate the effect of four different tice effect on the vigilance task used. This appears reasonable,
heat conditions. Results for response latency, signal omission, given Pepler's observation of a significant Performance • Days
and a combined median response time performance metric in- effect in a second, more thorough, experiment although a null
dicated optimum efficiency at 79 ~ (26 ~ ET compared with effect for practice on the clock task has been reported by Car-
a lower, 70 ~ (21 ~ ET, and two higher, 87.5 ~ (31 ~ and penter (1946). The second assumption is that performance mo-
97 ~ (36 ~ ET, conditions. Performance capability declined tivation is reduced by testing over a period of days with irregular
during the second hour on watch and this propensity, commonly exposures. Again, Pepler provided subjects' evaluative responses,
termed the vigilancedecrementfunction, was exacerbated by in- which indicate that such is the case. These assumptions suggest
creasing the heat above the optimum 79 ~ ET level. that performance is at a maximum toward the middle of any
There are two important elements of this foundational work individual's testing regimen, when initial practice has improved
that have generally escaped notice. First, the optimal performance capability while motivation to perform has not been extinguished.
condition was the only one of the four investigated that did not Empirical data from Pepler's subjects indicate that more than
cause a dynamic change in the deep body temperatures of the 50% of the 18 subjects exhibited this particular trend. In this
participants. Mackworth indicated that his acclimatized subjects experiment, Pepler took care to counterbalance for this order
performed the sedentary task wearing only gym shorts. They effect by using four 6 X 6 Latin square designs for the 24 subjects
were exposed to an air movement of 100 ft/min and experienced tested. However, in eliminating the results for six subjects for
an average 1.0 ~ (0.6 ~ fall in deep body temperature during legitimate artifacts, that is, two subjects for sleeping on watch
the 2-hr exposure at 70 ~ (21 ~ ET. It has been indicated and four for guessing the algorithm for critical signal appearance,
that exposures above 85 ~ (29.4 ~ ET result in a noncom- Pepler inadvertently eliminated a disproportionate number of
pensable rise in deep body temperature, where absolute rate of occasions in which the intermediate 82 ~ (27.8 ~ ET condition
rise is dependent on severity of the heat and length of exposure occurred in the central testing position. In the 18 subjects ana-
(e.g., Grether, 1973; Hancock, 1982; Houghten & Yagloglou, lyzed subsequently, only 3, instead of 6, performed this condition
1923, Figure 5; Lind, 1963). The two highest heat stress condi- while in the middle phase of their testing regimen. This might
tions in Mackworth's experiment, that is, 31 ~ and 36 ~ ET, be responsible for the result of elevated signal omission at the
exceeded this threshold value. Mackworth affirmed body tem- median temperature. In consequence, Pepler's initial contrasting
perature increases by reporting elevated rectal temperatures for result may be due to a problem with subject elimination, rather
SUSTAINED ATTENTION, TEMPERATURE 265
ii"
jective reactions of the drivers were analysed. Measures of self-
alertness and fatigue indicated progressive decrement with time,
although some conflict appeared between what is reported in
1.o1 the text and the illustration for the problem of fatigue (cf. Mackie
& O'Hanlon, 1977, Figure 14).
0.:] .1o ", L ,.,.,
0
2 The Wet-Bulb, Globe Temperature Index, first proposed by Yaglou
O" 7.'5 13 22:5 30 and Minard (1957) synthesizes air temperature, relative humidity, air
movement, and radiant heat. It is a superior index for assessingheat load
MinutesFromStsrt with respect to performance capability, as noted in this article. However,
Figure6. Falsedetections (A, A), responsetime (O, O), and signalomission typically, radiant heat is not recorded in experiments examining such
(lq, O) for 19 ~ effective temperature (ET; solid line) and 33 ~ ET performance. Consequently, the more common but less appropriate ef-
(broken line) conditions by sequential 7.5-rain periods on watch. (Data fective temperature scale has been used, particularly where such values
from Poulton, Edwards, & Colquhoun, 1974.) appear in the original report.
SUSTAINED ATTENTION, TEMPERATURE 271
temperature that follows the characteristic transient increase on The most recent investigation of the effects of cold on vigilance
immediate exposure to cold (Figure 12). These data suggest, was conducted by Angus, Pearce, Buguet, and Olsen (1979). They
therefore, that sustained attention decrement is related to dy- used a Wilkinson visual vigilance task and compared baseline
namic deep body temperature change, although in the above data with that obtained during 16 days at an ambient temperature
case it is a reduction in core temperature level. This finding is ranging from 0 ~ to 5 ~ (32 ~ OF) in Arctic conditions.
consistent with information presented previously on performance The vigilance task was performed in 40-min sessions on altemate
under dynamic body temperature decrease as given in the reports days, with one 2-day break around Day 8. Results indicated an
of Mackworth (1950) and Pepler (1953, 1958). immediate effect on signal omission. This increased by 27% from
Following the work of Kissen and his colleagues, Poulton, the final day of baseline measurement to the first experimental
Hitchings, and Brooke (1965) conducted a practical evaluation day. The authors speculated that the deprivation of rapid eye
of lookout efficiency in Arctic 28 ~ (-2.2 ~ and a slightly movement (REM) sleep that accompanied initial cold exposure
more temperate 35 ~ (1.7 ~ climatic conditions. There were was related to the decrement observed. Although body temper-
considerable practical and methodological problems because of atures were measured during sleeping hours, there was no in-
the action of wind and rain during one watch period. These dication of their level during actual performance; therefore it is
problems were accompanied by uncontrolled performance difficult to assess the role of body temperature in this case, as
transfer between conditions for the two groups examined. Despite an indication of time-related variation in performance with ther-
these contaminants, the authors recorded a 1.2 ~ (2.16 ~ fall mal state was not reported. The data are interesting however, in
in oral temperature during the exposure to the lower air tem- that recovery of performance during sequential days was indi-
perature. They concluded that the reduction in performance ef- cated, which may be due to a task-practice effect. This progressive
ficiency of watchkeeping in the cold may be correlated with the recovery might also have been related to the sequential decrease
dynamic decrease in body temperature. Although this is not in the impact of the environment on thermal state, although this
strong evidence, it is in clear agreement with previous observa- remains a somewhat speculative suggestion. The authors' obser-
tions (e.g., Kissen et al., 1964; Mackworth, 1950; Pepler, 1958). vations on REM sleep loss also deserve consideration. Results
indicated total recovery of vigilance to baseline levels after 16
days, whereas REM recovery stood at 75% of preexposure levels.
The simple correlation between measures of REM and perfor-
mance was not significant. This indicated that REM loss cannot
be the sole factor involved. However, using a multiple correlation
technique, the authors calculated that over 90% of the variation
in the vigilance performance could be accounted for by three
factors: percentage of REM recovery, task days, and temperature.
Decline in vigilance efficiency with dynamic perturbation to deep
body temperature is suggested by the overall conclusions of the
study.
efficiency (Dean & McGlothen, 1965; Edholm, 1963; Fine et al., variation in vigilance under the impact of thermal stress, there
1960; Loeb & Jeantheau, 1958). have been three basic proposals. These are physiological adequacy,
behavioral arousal, and attentional-resource capacity. The utility
Cold." Absence of Experimental Results Under of these three approaches is evaluated in light of the insights
Stabilized Hypothermia revealed by the preeeeding differentiation of performance based
on the thermophysiological state of the observer involved.
There are apparently no studies in which vigilance perfor-
mance has been specifically monitored while the subject was Physiological Adequacy
stabilized in a hypothermic state. Of the three options concerning
performance variation--no change, decrement, and improve- Perhaps the earliest notion concerning human capability in
ment--the least likely would seem to be no effect for such a heat is that of physiological adequacy, which emanates from initial
manipulation. However, judging the likelihood of the latter two descriptive and experimental observations (Blagden, 1775a,
possibilities appears to be a matter of speculation. Evidence from 1775b; Ellis, 1758; Sutton, 1909). According to this proposition,
other performance areas, such as time perception, appears to measurements of body temperature, sweat rate, and other ther-
favor the notion of a slowing of response and therefore decrement moregulatory actions are taken as indices of physiological state.
(Baddeley, 1966). However, even within this latter realm, indi- Tolerance is set in terms of those conditions in which the thermal
vidual differences exert an important effect (e.g., C. R. Bell, 1966; equilibrium of the body is first disturbed. Physiological adequacy
Hancock, 1983). In the absence of more substantial indications, implies that performance remains unaffected while regulatory
no firm assertion concerning vigilance performance under this mechanisms are effective and homeostasis prevails.
hypothermic condition is possible at this time. Although early experimental studies appeared to support the
In summary, the consensus of data available concerning the adequacy notion (see Connell, 1948), subsequent research failed
thermal environment, body temperature, and capability on sus- to confirm the generality of the construct (e.g., Bartlett & Gronow,
tained attention tasks does not support the notions advanced 1953; Weiner & Hutchinson, 1945). However, these early studies
by Grether (1973) and by Davies and his colleagues (e.g.,Davies failed to distinguish between the type of cognitive or psychomotor
& Parasuraman, 1982; Davies & Tune, 1970). These authors performance task undertaken. With a careful differentiation be-
have indicated a general decrement due to cold and facilitation tween such tasks, specific increases in deep body temperature
in thermal conditions exceeding subjective comfort, with capa- can be used to describe the limits of efficient performance in
bility not failing until imminent physiological collapse. The mental, psychomotor, and dual-task performance categories
present review, in contrast, indicates variation in sustained at- (Hancock, 1982). Although the concept of physiological adequacy
tention efficiency with a measurable change in the deep body was one of the first accounts of performance under thermal stress,
temperature of the observer (see Colquhoun & Goldman, 1972). its early rejection appears to have been based on insufficient
As indicated in the data of Benor and Shvartz (1971), this dec- recognition of the variety of tasks undertaken by the stress par-
remental function is continuous and appears to follow a positive ticipants. Consequently, it has languished, disused for a consid-
exponential-type function. This function, typical of a positive erable time. This may have been an inappropriate treatment of
feedback system, is mirrored in the morphology of the breakdown a useful construct.
in body temperature itself under the driving force of some non- From the foregoing evaluation of vigilance in heat and cold,
compensable thermal surround, such as desert conditions. Sig- where significant performance decrement is apparently induced
nificant decrement in vigilance appears to coincide with the point by dynamic change in deep body temperature, the construct of
at which deep body temperature change may first be observed. physiological adequacy appears a reasonable account of the data
This review has been restricted to those conditions in which reported. However, despite this recognition, it is important to
the effect of an external thermal stress has been imposed on the note that it is largely a descriptive rather than explanatory account
individual. This does not include many studies (e.g., Bonnet & of the phenomenon at hand. Although the appeal to physiological
Alter, 1982; Moses, Lubin, Naitoh, & Johnson, 1978; Poock, mechanisms implies some empirical predictions, no distinct
Tuck, & Tinsley, 1969; Taub & Berger, 1976) that have examined causal mechanism is identified by this proposal.
the effect on performance of naturally occurring fluctuations in
body temperature due to circadian rhythm. However, in accord Behavioral A rousal
with the observations on static hyperthermia, it is clear that the
natural increase in deep body temperature associated with cir- Perhaps the most influential theoretical account that has been
cadian rhythm is also accompanied by an increase in capability proposed concerning the overall effects of stress on human per-
formance is behavioral arousal. With respect to the effect of en-
on a variety of tasks (Kleitman, 1963), including vigilance.
vironmental temperature on sustained attention, this position
has been presented most cogently by Poulton (Poulton, 1976,
Discussion
1977). To account for the variations in performance, as outlined
There have been a number of accounts of the impact of en- in the previous section, Poulton proposed five different effects
vironmental stressors on vigilance (e.g., Hancock, 1984a; Poul- founded on changes in arousal state. Initially, and after the ex-
ton, 1977) and, similarly, several proposals concerning the effect perimental findings of Mackworth (1950) and Pepler (1958), he
of thermal stress on a wide variety of cognitive and neuromus- postulated that mild heat, sufficient to induce discomfort, facil-
cular abilities (see P. A. Bell, 1981; Kobrick & Fine, 1983). These itates performance through an increase in arousal. Such an in-
range from simple descriptive approaches to those postulating crease is also associated with the initial entry into a heated en-
some form of underlying causal mechanism. With respect to vironment (Poulton & Kerslake, 1965). Continuation in a mildly
SUSTAINED ATTENTION, TEMPERATURE 277
uncomfortable ambient temperature is accompanied by both cessing, feature extraction, response choice, and motor adjust-
the sensation of lassitude and a slow increase in body temperature, ment. It is suggested that stress arises when the effort mechanism
which reduces arousal and the ability to sustain attention. Poulton is overloaded over a period of time, or fails completely in adjusting
(1977) suggested that, should conditions be sufficient to cause a necessary energetic requirements. This model provides some in-
more abrupt rise in deep body temperature, then such arousing teresting insights into the mechanism of stress and is superior
circumstances promptly improve efficiency (e.g., Colquhoun & to the simple arousal constructs that precede it.
Goldman, 1972; Wilkinson et al., 1964). As with all capabilities,
the sustenance of attention fails rapidly as the individual ap- Attentional Resource Capacity
proaches thermally intolerable conditions (e.g., Benor & Shvartz,
1971). From this post hoc perspective, the arousal construct ap- In contrast to a linear-stage model of information processing,
pears to account for the diversity of experimental results reported. the human may be characterized as possessing a limited pool of
However, the very flexibility of the construct that allows for such attentional resources (Knowles, 1963). These resources may be
encapsulation and lends supposed explanatory power is, in ac- distributed among tasks, or elements of tasks, in order to accom-
tuality, a liability. Thus, the arousal construct is unable to dif- plish successful performance. Kahneman (1973) viewed these
ferentiate a priori which conditions will prove arousing and which resources as residing in a single global pool to be allocated as
depressing. Consequently, arousal remains a post hoe construc- appropriate to demand. As resources themselves have a finite
tion based on the various data sets reported. limit, so the human has limitations on both single- and multiple-
In the arousal account, when performance is described solely task abilities. Wickens (1980) has elaborated on this position, to
in terms of arousal change, the argument is a simple tautology include structure-specific resource pools, thus integrating a re-
with no practical or predictive utility. However, the arousal po- source model with the linear-stage approach. Specific research
sition gains greater credence if some mediational construct is pools are differentiated by modality of stimulus assimilation,
interpolated between arousal state and performance level. Such decision making, and mode of response. Considerable support
a construct was introduced by Easterbrook (1959), who postu- for this position is gained from observations of difficulty insen-
lated the restriction of cue utilization with increasing arousal sitivity, which are occasions on which two tasks may be performed
level. As arousal increases from a minimal level, non-task-related simultaneously, or time-shared, with no appreciable reduction
stimuli are rejected, and in consequence, performance improves. in the performance efficiency of either task. There have been
With increasing arousal, task-relevant cues are filtered and per- some recent objections to the methodology used in experiments
formance is diminished. This form of construct results in the that support the multiple-resource model (Damos & Lyall, 1984),
classic inverted U-shaped function matching arousal with effi- and a general critique of the notion of attentional resources by
ciency. One important element of this account, not commonly Navon (1984), who was instrumental in introducing the idea of
recognized, is that the correct rejection of non-task-related cues, attentional resources originally. However, the full elaboration of
which gives rise to the ascending arm of the inverted U, must be these arguments is beyond the scope of the present work. (For
a somewhat different process from the subsequent elimination more complete discussions, see Wickens, 1984, and Parasuraman
of task-relevant cues. This is because the former requires some & Davies, 1984). Rather, my central concern in this article is
form of selection, whereas the latter process is some general form with the impact of stress on attentional resources, and particularly
of degradation. Consequently, a further construct is essentially with how stress might influence the performance of a task re-
needed to account for the two different processes illustrated. In quiring the long-term sustenance of attention.
previous work (Hancock & Dirkin, 1983; Hockey, 1970), it has The foregoing arguments suggest that stress acts to drain at-
been suggested that attention may be able to account for such tentional resources. Further, from evidence on vigilance it appears
actions. This proposal is explored in more detail in the next that stress sufficient to perturb efficient homeostasis, that is, to
section specifically devoted to attention. cause an uncontrolled rise or fall in deep body temperature,
In sum, although arousal has been a useful construct in ac- results in significant performance decrement. This position is
counting for performance variation under stress, and has a sub- somewhat reminiscent of that adopted by Cohen (1978, 1980),
stantive physiological basis, its ubiquitous application has been who reasoned that the threatening nature of stressors demand
questioned (Broadbent, 1963). Also, its composition as a unitary attentional resource capacity and that prolonged demand results
entity has been recently criticized (Hancock, 1984b). For a more in shrinkage or cognitive fatigue. However, Cohen suggested that
detailed examination of the current status of the arousal notion, stress both competes for and drains resources, without specifying
see Hockey and Hamilton (1983). In conclusion, the simple the level of stress at which such a draining process becomes crit-
arousal construct is failing as a descriptive account, whereas its ical. In the present work, such a critical point is identified as 85
use as a causal explanation has always been somewhat suspect ~ ET for heat stress, where an uncontrolled rise in deep body
in that on most occasions the appeal has been to some vague temperature may be observed.
physiological mechanism that often remains undefined. For vigilance, the notion of physiological adequacy is partic-
An interesting exception to the above statement is the recent ularly descriptive of the summary of data reported. However, it
work of Sanders in which he attempts to relate stress and arousal does not describe the increases in performance capability, and,
to a linear-stage model of human information processing (Sand- further, it requires the mediational attentional construct to pro-
ers, 1983). Briefly, he outlines a model that relates three energetic vide some element of causality. The use of attention is also par-
supply systems, as envisaged by Pribram and McGuiness (1975), ticularly appealing for two separate reasons. First, it has been
to specific cognitive processing mechanisms. Arousal, effort, and indicated that those tasks that demand greater attention for their
activation are linked to processing stages of stimulus prepro- successful completion are most vulnerable to the effects of per-
278 P. A. HANCOCK
turbation to deep body temperature (Hancock, 1982). Vigilance, In the review sections concerning dynamic change to deep
as reviewed in this work, is particularly vulnerable, such that body temperature and its functional relation with performance
any perturbation to deep body temperature is enough to affect efficiency, it was noted that above the threshold at which deep
efficiency (see also Hancock & Pierce, 1984). A second reason body temperature was perturbed, performance was degraded,
is that if stress acts to drain attention, then those individuals and this function took the form of an increasing exponential. It
who through some strategy need to devote less attention to suc- was also recognized that this same function describes physiolog-
cessfully perform the task should prove less vulnerable to stress ical breakdown under the driving force of an extreme thermal
effects. Hancock (in press) has indicated that individuals who condition. In other work (Hancock & Chignell, 1985) we have
are skilled on the task they are performing, and are able presum- attempted to explore the implications of this potential iso-
ably to use relatively attention-free automatic-type processes, morphism and the possibility that is may represent some more
clearly suffer less adverse stress effects than their unskilled peers ubiquitous stress effect.
(see also Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977). In summary, stress drains attention. With less attention the
The linkage of stressor effects and the resultant diminution in perceiver is constrained to adopt a tactic of narrowing onto cues
attentional resource capacity suggests the use of psychological of perceived task salience in order to sustain successful perfor-
measures (cognitive performance) as predictive of imminent mance for as long as is feasible. Individuals who are skilled on
physiological breakdown under extreme thermal stress. Hancock a task have the opportunity to use relatively attention-free au-
and Chignell (1985) proposed that, outside of a comfort zone, tomatic processes that leave them less vulnerable to the impact
there are regions of adaptability to stressors, with the zone of of the stressor. Parenthetically, those individuals who have fa-
psychological adaptability being enclosed within the zone of miliarity with the stressor and are acclimatized to the stressful
physiological adaptability (Hancock & Chignell, 1985, Figure conditions also suffer less (Wilkinson, 1969b). However, this may
1). In this view, breakdown of performance on psychological represent amelioration of the physiological impact of the stress
measures would be directly reflected in change of physiological rather than the influence of attentional capacity per se. Finally,
state. Thus performance on attention-demanding cognitive tasks unsupportable conditions drain all attention resources and this
may be used as a form of early warning signal for the loss of leads to the termination of purposive behavioral activity, or un-
regulatory homeostasis or physiological adaptability. The con- consciousness. The continued impact of the stress eventually su-
nection between thermal stress and attentional resource capacity persedes physiological capability and this represents the termi-
is a natural one given that the level of thermal stress induces nation of life-sustaining functions, or death.
instability in core temperature. Cognitive processes are liable to After exploring the detrimental effects of temperature, it is
be susceptible to fluctuations in core temperature and associated important to redress the balance somewhat by indicating those
change in central nervous system temperature (Goodman et al., occasions on which hyperthermia improves performance. This
1984; Hancock, 1983). This rationale has been used to justify occurs when the individual is established in a static hyperthermic
deep auditory canal temperature as a reflection of mental work- state (Colquhoun & Goldman, 1972; Wilkinson et al., 1964).
load (Hancock, Meshkati, & Robertson, 1985). These conditions are similar to those observed during the se-
The notion of narrowing, as seen in the work of Easterbrook quential increase in temperature associated with the circadian
on cue utilization, is also important in understanding the effects rhythm. Under these circumstances, in which the relative rate
of stress on attention. Clearly, if stress acts to drain attentional of change of body temperature is small, performance also im-
resources, then less resources are available to perform any par- proves with ascending temperature level. These indications sug-
ticular task. Under such conditions, the optimal performance gest that both absolute level and rate of change of deep body
strategy is to use what resources are left available to process task- temperature mediates the impact on attention.
relevant cues. This appears to be the tactic adopted by the per- In conclusion, it should be noted that not only the level and
ceiver, but with the caveat that cues that are processed are those rate of change affect performance, but also the concomitant
of perceived highest salience, which may or may not coincide change in peripheral or skin temperature. Although core tem-
with those of greatest task relevance (Cornsweet, 1969; Easter- perature has been implicated in the speed of performance (Allnutt
brook, 1959). This concept has been explored most thoroughly & Allan, 1973; Hancock, 1983; Hoagiand, 1933), the rate of
in experiments by Hockey using noise as a stressor (e.g., Hockey, error has been associated with fluctuation in skin temperature
1970). Recently, we have demonstrated that the narrowing effect (Allan & Gibson, 1979). With respect to vigilance, the relation
is an attentional rather than a visual process. This was accom- between these two measures and performance largely remains
plished by showing superior response to task-relevant and per- to be explored. A fuller picture based on these more compre-
ceived salient cues in the visual periphery, compared with less hensive physiological measures awaits further experimentation.
relevant but more centrally placed comparison signals (Dirkin
& Hancock, 1984). There are a variety of empirical studies that Summary
demonstrate the narrowing phenomenon. Among those using
heat stress, those by BursiU (1958) and Provins and Bell (1970) In this article I have proposed that the key factor in determining
are most notable. Results from their work using dual-task per- change in vigilance under thermal stress is the thermophysiologi-
formance imply that stress drains rather than competes for at- cal state of the observer involved. When conditions are insufficient
tentional resources. However, a clear and unequivocal demon- to change deep body temperature, performance remains essen-
stration of this phemomenon under the impact of both extremes tially unaffected. However, thermal stress exposures that perturb
of heat and cold awaits a more thorough experimental investi- deep body temperature away from both normal and steady-state
gation. conditions impair vigilance, while static hyperthermic states are
SUSTAINED ATTENTION, TEMPERATURE 279
beneficial with respect to sustained attention. The insights derived to hot and humid conditions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy-
from sustained attention performance under thermal stress are chology, 10, 113-129.
potentially useful for the construction of an overall theory of Burton, A. C., & Edholm, O. G. (1955). Man in a cold environment.
London: Arnold.
stress and attention. Whereas initial efforts have confirmed such
Carlson, L. D. (1961). Human performance under different thermal loads
utility (see Hancock & Chignell, 1985), the elaboration into a
(Tech. Rep. No. 61-43). Brooks AFB, TX: U.S.A.E Aerospace Medical
full theoretical perspective demands long-term efforts and an in- Center, School of Aviation Medicine.
tegration of understanding of the action of a variety of stressors Carpenter, A. (1946). The effect of room temperature on the performance
on human behavior that has yet to be achieved. of the resistance box test; A performance test of intelligence (R.N.P.
Rep. No. 46/318). Unpublished report to Royal Naval Personnel
References Committee of the Medical Research Council, Cambridge, England.
Chiles, W, D., Iampietro, P. E, & Higgins, E. A. (1972). Combined effects
Allan, J. R., & Gibson, T. M. (1979). Separation of the effects of raised of altitude and high temperature on complex performance. Human
skin and core temperature on performance of a pursuit rotor task. Factors, 14, 161-172.
Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine, 50, 678-682. Cohen, S. (1978). Environmental load and the allocation of attention. In
Allnutt, M. E, & Allan, J. R. (1973). The effects of core temperature A. Baum, J. E. Singer, & S. Valins (Eds.), Advances in environmental
elevation and thermal sensation on performance. Ergonomics, 16, 189- psychology (pp. 1-29). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
196. Cohen, S. (1980). Aftereffects of stress on human performance and social
Angus, R. G., Pearce, D. G., Buguet, A. G. C, & Olsen, L. (1979). Vig- behavior: A review of research and theory. PsychologicalBulletin, 88,
ilance performance of men sleeping under Artic conditions. Aviation, 82-108.
Space and Environmental Medicine, 50, 692-696. Colquhoun, W. P. (1960). Temperament, inspection efficiency and time
Arees, E. A. (1963). The effect of environmental temperature and alerting of day. Ergonomics, 3, 377-378.
stimuli on prolonged search. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Uni- Colquhoun, W. P. (1969). Effects of raised ambient temperature and
versity of Massachusetts, Amherst. event rate on vigilance performance. Aerospace Medicine, 40, 413-
Baddeley, A. D. (1966). Time estimation at reduced body temperature. 417.
American Journal of Psychology, 79, 475-479. Colquhoun, W. P., & Goldman, R. E (1972). Vigilance under induced
Bartlett, D. J., & Gronow, D. G. C. (1953). The effects of heat stress on hyperthermia. Ergonomics, 15, 621-632.
mental performance (Rep. No. 846). England: Flying Personnel Re- Connell, L. (1948). The effect of heat upon the performance of men in
search Committee. high speed aircraft:A critical review (Pep. NO. 151-1-17). New York:
Bazett, H. C. (1949). The regulation of body temperatures. In L. M. Psychological Corporation.
Newburgh (Ed.), Physiologyof heat regulation and the scienceof clothing Cornsweet, D. M. (1969). Use of cues in the visual periphery under con-
(pp. 109-192). Philadelphia: Saunders. ditions of arousal. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80, 14-18.
Bell, C. R. (1964). Climate, body temperature and vigilance performance. Craig, A., Wilkinson, R. T., & Colquhoun, W. P. (1981). Diurnal variation
Proceedings of the Second International Congress on Ergonomics, 2, in vigilance efficiency. Ergonomics, 24, 641-651.
169-172. Damos, D. L., & Lyall, E. A. (1984). The effect of asymmetric transfer
Bell, C. R. (1966). Control of time estimation by a chemical clock. Nature, on dual-task assessment of voice technology. Proceedings of the Human
210, 1189-1190. Factors Society, 28, 21-25.
Bell, C. R., & Provins, K. A. (1962). Effects of high temperature envi- Davies, D. R., & Parasuraman, R. (1982). The psychology of vigilance.
ronmental conditions on human performance. Journal of Occupational London: Academic Press.
Medicine, 4, 202-221. Davies, D. R., & Tune, G. S. (1970). Human vigilanceperformance. Lon-
Bell, C. R., Provins, K. A., & Hiorns, R. W. (1964). Visual and auditory don: Staples Press.
vigilance during exposure to hot and humid conditions. Ergonomics, Dean, R. D., & McGlothen, C. L. (1965). Effects of combined heat and
7, 279-282. noise on human performance, physiology and subjective estimates of
Bell, P. A. (1981). Physiological, comfort, performance, and social effects comfort and performance. Institute of Environmental Sciences Annual
of heat stress. Journal of Social Issues, 37, 71-94. Technical Meeting Proceedings, 55-64.
Benor, D., & Shvartz, E. (197i). Effect of body cooling on vigilance in Dirkin, G. R., & Hancock, P. A. (1984). Attentional narrowing to the
hot environments. Aerospace Medicine, 42, 727-730. visual periphery under temporal and acoustic stress. Aviation, Space
Blagden, C. (1775a). Experiments and observations in an heated room. and Environmental Medicine, 55, 454. (Abstract)
Philosophical Transactions, 65, 111-123. Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and
Blagden, C. (1775b). Further experiments and observations in an heated the organization of behavior. PsychologicalReview, 56, 183-201.
room. Philosophical Transactions, 65, 484-494. Edholm, O. G. (1963). Heat acclimatization studied in the laboratory
Bonnet, M. H., & Alter, J. (1982). Effects of irregular versus regular sleep and the field. A multi-disciplinary approach. Ergonomics, 6, 304-305.
schedules on performance, mood and body temperature. Biological Ellis, H. (1758). An account of the heat of the weather in Georgia. Phil-
Psychology, 14, 287-296. osophical Transactions, 50, 755.
Broadbent, D. E. (1953). Noise, paced performance and vigilance tasks. Epstein, Y., Keren, G., Moisseiev, J., Gasko, O., & Yachin, S. (1980).
British Journal of Psychology, 44, 295-303. Psychomotor deterioration during exposure to heat. Aviation, Space
Broadbent, D. E. (1954). Some effects of noise on visual performance. and Environmental Medicine, 51, 607-610.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6, 1-5. Fine, B. J., Cohen, A., & Crist, B. (1960). Effect of exposure to high
Broadbent, D. E. (1963). Differences and interactions between stresses. humidity at high and moderate ambient temperature on anagram so-
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15, 205-211. lution and auditory discrimination. PsychologicalReports, 7, 171-181.
Broadbent, D. E. (1971). Decision and stress. London: Academic Press. Fine, B. J., & Kobrick, J. L. (1978). Effects of altitude and heat on complex
Brown, I. D. (1967). Measurement of control skills, vigilance and per- cognitive tasks. Human Factors, 20, 115-122.
formance of a subsidiary task during 12 hours of car driving. Ergo- Fraser, D. C. (1957). Some effects of heat stress on performance of a
nomics, 10, 665-673. vigilance task under speed stress. National Coal Board Medical Research
Bursill, A. E. (1958). The restriction of peripheral vision during exposure Memorandum, 1, 50-63. (London)
280 P. A. HANCOCK
Goodman, D., Hancock, P. A., Runnings, D. W., & Brown, S. L. (1984). Iampietro, P. E, Melton, C. E., Higgins, E. A., Vaughan, J. A., Hoffman,
Temperature-induced changes in neuromuscular function: Central and S. M., Funkhouser, G. E., & Saldivar, J. T. (1972). High temperature
peripheral mechanisms. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59, 647-656. and performance in a flight task simulator. Aerospace Medicine, 43,
Grether, W. E (1973). Human performance at elevated environmental 1215-1218.
temperatures. Aerospace Medicine, 44, 747-755. Jones, R. D. (1970). Effects of thermal stress on human performance: A
Hancock, P. A. (1981). Heat stress impairment of mental performance: review and critique of existing methodology (Tech. Rep. No. 11-70).
A revision of tolerance limits. Aviation, Space and Environmental Aberdeen, MD: Human Engineering Laboratories, Aberdeen Research
Medicine, 52, 177-180. and Development Center.
Hancock, P. A. (1982). Task categorization and the limits of human Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort, Englewood Cliffs, N J: Pren-
performance in extreme heat. Aviation, Space and Environmental tice-Hall.
Medicine, 53, 778-784. Kissen, A. T., Reifler, C. B., & Thaler, V. H. (1964). Modification of
Hancock, P. A. (1983). An endogenous metric for the perception of short thermoregulatory responses to cold by hypnosis. Journal of Applied
temporal intervals. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Physiology, 19, 1043-1050.
Illinois, Urbana-Champaign. Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and wakefulness. Chicago: University of Chi-
Hancock, P. A. (1984a). Effect of environmental temperature on display cago Press.
monitoring performance: An overview with practical implications. Knowles, W. B. (1963). Operator loading tasks. Human Factors, 5, 155-
American Industrial Hygiene Association Journal, 45, 122-126. 162.
Hancock, P. A. (1984b). Environmental stressors. In J. S. Warm (Ed.), Kobrick, J. L., & Fine, B. J. (1983). Climate and human performance.
Sustained attention in human performance. London: Wiley. In D. J. Oborne & M. M. Gruneberg (Eds.), The physical environment
Hancock, P. A. (in press). The effect of skill on performance under an at work (pp. 69-107). London: Wiley.
environmental stressor. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine. Lind, A. R. (1963). A physiological criterion for setting thermal envi-
Hancock, P. A., & Chignell, M. H. (1985). The principle of maximal ronmental limits for everyday work. Journal of Applied Physiology, 18,
adaptability in setting stress tolerance standards. In R. Eberts & C. E. 51-56.
Eberts (Eds.), Trends in ergonomics/human factors H (pp. 117-125). Loeb, M., & Jeantheau, G. (1958). The influence of noxious environ-
Amsterdam: North-Holland. mental stimuli on vigilance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 42, 47-49.
Hancock, P. A., & Dirkin, G. R. (1983). Stressor induced attentional Mackie, R. R. (Ed.). (1977). Vigilance: Theory, operational performance
narrowing: Implications for design and operation of person-machine and physiological correlates. New York: Plenum Press.
systems. Proceedings of the Human Factors Association of Canada, Mackie, R. R., & O'Hanlon, J. E (1977). A study of the combined effects
16, 19-21. of extended driving and heat stress on driver arousal and performance.
Hancock, P. A., Meshkati, N., & Robertson, M. M. (1985). Physiological In R. R. Mackie (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operationalperformance and
reflections of mental workload. Aviation, Space and Environmental physiological correlates (pp. 537-558). New York: Plenum Press.
Medicine, 56, 1110-1114. Mackworth, N. H. (1950). Researches on the measurement of human
Hancock, P. A., & Pierce, J. O. (1984). Toward an attentional theory of performance. Medical Research Council Special Report Series 268.
performance under stress: Evidence from studies of vigilance in heat London: Her Majesty's Stationary Office.
and cold. In A. Mital (Ed.), Trends in ergonomics/human factors I Mortagy, A. K. (1971). Effects of work~restschedules on monitoring per-
(pp. 1-7). Amsterdam: North-Holland. formance in the heat. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas Tech
Hancock, P. A., & Pierce, J. O. (1985). Combined effects of heat and University, Lubbock.
noise on human performance: A review. American Industrial Hygiene Mortagy, A. K., & Ramsey, J. D. (1973). Monitoring performance as a
Association Journal 46, 555-566. function of work/rest ratio schedule and thermal stress. American In-
Harminc, M. (1976). Einige psychische und physiologische Funktionen dustrial Hygiene Association Journal, 34, 474-480.
in der Beiziehung zum Mikroklima [Some psychological and physio- Moses, J., Lubin, A., Naitoh, P., & Johnson, L. C. (1978). Circadian
logical functions due to the state of the microclimate]. Studia Psy- variation in performance, subjective sleepiness, sleep and oral tem-
chologica, 18, 41-50. perature during an altered sleep--wake schedule. Biological Psychology,
Head, H. (1926). Aphasia. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University 6, 301-308.
Press. Navon, D. (1984). Resources: A theoretic soupstone. PsychologicalReview,
Hoagiand, H. (1933). The physiological control ofjudgements of duration: 91, 214-224.
Evidence for a chemical clock. Journal of General Psychology, 9, 267- Parasuraman, R., & Davies, D. R. (1984). Varieties of attention. New
287. York: Academic Press.
Hockey, G. R. J. (1970). Signal probability and spatial location as possible Pepler, R. D. (1953). The effect of climatic factors on the performance of
bases for increased selectivity in noise. Quarterly Journal of Experi- skilled tasks by young European men living in the tropics. 4: A task of
mental Psychology, 22, 37-42. prolonged visual vigilance. (Rep. No. 156/53). Cambridge, England:
Hockey, G. R. J., & Hamilton, P. (1983). The cognitive patterning of Medical Research Council, Royal Naval Research Committee, Applied
stress states. In G. R. J. Hockey (Ed.), Stress and fatigue in human Psychology Unit.
performance (pp. 331-362). London: Wiley. Pcpler,R. D. (1958). Warmth and performance: An investigationin the
Hohnsbein, J., Piekarski, C., & Kampmann, B. (1983). Influence of high tropics. Ergonomics, 2, 63-88.
ambient temperature and humidity on visual sensitivity. Ergonomics, Poock, G. K., Tuck, G. A., & Tinsley, J. H. (1969). Physiological correlates
26, 905-911. of visual monitoring. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 29, 334.
Hohnsbein, J., Piekarski, C., Kampmann, B., & Noack, T. (1984). Effects Poulton, E. C. (1973). Unwanted range effects from using within-subject
of heat on visual acuity. Ergonomics, 27, 1239-1246. experimental designs. PsychologicalBulletin, 80, 113-121.
Houghten, E C., & Yagloglou, C. P. (1923). Determining lines of equal Poulton, E. C. (1976). Arousing environmental stresses can improve per-
comfort. Transactions of the American Society of Heating and Venti- formance: Whatever people say. Aviation, Space and Environmental
lating Engineers, 29, 163-176. Medicine, 47, 1193-1204.
Iampietro, P. E, Chiles, W. D., Higgins, E. A., & Gibbons, H. L. (1969). Poulton, E. C. (1977). Arousing stresses increase vigilance. In R. R. Mackie
Complex performance during exposures to high temperatures. Aero- (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance and physiological
space Medicine, 40, 1331- 1335. correlates (pp. 423-459). New York: Plenum Press.
SUSTAINED ATTENTION, TEMPERATURE 281
Poulton, E. C., & Edwards, R. S. (1974a). Interactions and range effects Teichner, W. H. (1966). Individual thermal and behavioral factors in cold-
in experiments on pairs of stresses: Mild heat and low-frequency noise. induced vasodilation. Psychophysiology, 2, 295-304.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 621-628. Viteles, M. S., & Smith, K. R. (1946). An experimental investigation of
Poulton, E. C., & Edwards, R. S. (1974b). Interactions, range effects, and the effect of change in atmospheric conditions and noise upon perfor-
comparisons between tasks in experiments measuring performance mance. Heating, Piping and Air Conditioning, 18, 107-112.
with pairs of stresses: Mild heat and 1 nag of L-hyoscine hydrobromide. Warm, J. S. (Ed.). (1984). Sustained attention in human performance.
Aerospace Medicine,45, 735-741. London: Wiley.
Poulton, E. C., Edwards, R. S., & Colquhoun, W. E ( 1974). The interaction Weiner, J. S., & Hutchinson, J. C. D. (1945). Hot humid environment:
of a loss of a night's sleep with mild heat: Task variables. Ergonomics, Its effect on the performance of a motor coordination test. British
17, 59-73. Journal of lndustrial Medicine, 2, 154-157.
Poulton, E. C., Hitchings, N. B., & Brooke, R. B. (1965). Effect of cold Wickens, C. D. (1980). The structure of attentional resources. In R. S.
and rain upon the vigilance of lookouts. Ergonomics, 8, 163-168. Nickerson & R. Pew (Eds.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 239-
Poulton, E. C., & Kerslake, D. (1965). Initial stimulating effect of warmth 257). Hillsdale, NJ: Edbaum.
upon perceptual efficiency. AerospaceMedicine, 36, 29-32. Wickens, C. D. (1984). Engineeringpsychology and human performance.
Pribram, K. H., & McGuinness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation and effort Columbus, OH: Merrill.
in the control of attention. PsychologicalReview, 82, 116-149. Wilkinson, R. T. (1969a). Sleep deprivation: Performance tests for partial
Provins, K. A., & Bell, C. R. (1970). Effects of heat stress on the perfor- and selective sleep deprivation. In L. A. Abt & B. E Reiss (Eds.), Progress
mance of two tasks running concurrently. Journal of Experimental in clinical psychology (pp. 28-43). New York: Grune & Stratton.
Psychology, 85, 40-44. Wilkinson, R. T. (1969b). Some factors influencing the effect of environ-
Sanders, A. E (1983). Toward a model of stress and human performance. mental stressors upon performance. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 260-
Acta Psychologica, 53, 61-97. 272.
Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human Wilkinson, R. T., Fox, R. H., Goldsmith, R., Hampton, I. F. G., & Lewis,
information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psycho- H. E. (1964). Psychological and physiological responses to raised body
logical Review, 84, 1-66. temperature. Journal of Applied Physiology, 19, 287-291.
Sutton, H. (1909). The influence of high temperatures on the human Yaglou, C. P., & Minard, D. (1957). Control of heat casualties at military
body, especially with regard to heat stroke. Journal of Pathology and training centers. Archives of lndustrial Heatlh, 16, 302-316.
Bacteriology, 13, 62-73.
Taub, J. M., & Berger, R. J. (1976). Effects of acute sleep pattern alteration Received July 1, 1985
depend upon sleep duration. Physiological Psychology, 4, 412-420. Revision received August 19, 1985 9
Hancock, P.A. (1986). Sustained attention under thermal stress. Psychological Bulletin, 99 (2), 263-281.