0% found this document useful (0 votes)
31 views26 pages

Alba - The Twilight of Ethnicity Among Americans of European Ancestry The Case of Italians

This document discusses the concept of assimilation and ethnic boundaries in relation to Italian Americans. It argues that assimilation should be viewed not just as individual decisions but also as changes in group boundaries. Examining interethnic relations can reveal changes in ethnic boundaries over time. The case of Italian Americans demonstrates the importance of considering structural factors and boundary shifts, rather than just individual assimilation, when understanding ethnicity.

Uploaded by

martins0
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
31 views26 pages

Alba - The Twilight of Ethnicity Among Americans of European Ancestry The Case of Italians

This document discusses the concept of assimilation and ethnic boundaries in relation to Italian Americans. It argues that assimilation should be viewed not just as individual decisions but also as changes in group boundaries. Examining interethnic relations can reveal changes in ethnic boundaries over time. The case of Italian Americans demonstrates the importance of considering structural factors and boundary shifts, rather than just individual assimilation, when understanding ethnicity.

Uploaded by

martins0
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 26

Ethnic and Racial Studies

ISSN: 0141-9870 (Print) 1466-4356 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rers20

The twilight of ethnicity among Americans of


European ancestry: The case of Italians

Richard D. Alba

To cite this article: Richard D. Alba (1985) The twilight of ethnicity among Americans of
European ancestry: The case of Italians , Ethnic and Racial Studies, 8:1, 134-158, DOI:
10.1080/01419870.1985.9993478

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01419870.1985.9993478

Published online: 13 Sep 2010.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 39

View related articles

Citing articles: 15 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rers20

Download by: [University North Carolina - Chapel Hill] Date: 30 October 2016, At: 14:44
The twilight of ethnicity among Americans
of European ancestry: the case of Italians*
Richard D. Alba
State University of New Yorkat Albany

The course of ethnicity in advanced industrial societies continues to be


debated without satisfactory resolution. Earlier social theorists, inspired by
a vision of the erosion of traditional structures under the impact of a tide of
modernization, tended to see ethnicity as receding. More recently, sociologists
and others have proclaimed the resilience of ethnicity ; for some, this is because
ethnicity is an affiliation apart, primordial and only superficially modified by
currents of modernization, while for others, it is due to ethnicity's moorings
in durable structures of inequality.
Proponents of the view that ethnicity is resilient are the dominant voice
in contemporary discussions, but their dominance is by no means assured,
since the conceptual groundwork for interpreting ethnicity remains unsettled.
There is in fact no consensus on the proper vantage point from which to view
ethnicity, 'assimilation' having been dethroned as the crowning concept of
the field over the last two decades (Blauner, 1972; Greeley, 1977).
This paper examines some of the interpretative difficulties surrounding
ethnicity through the experiences of one group, Italian Americans. In par-
ticular, Italians are taken to constitute a strategic test case for some reigning
assumptions in the study of ethnicity of European-ancestry groups in the
United States. I will argue that the Italian experience demonstrates the impor-
tance of boundary-shifting processes, as opposed to assimilation at the
individual level only, and that these shifts require for their explanation the
invocation of historical contingencies, rooted in structural changes external
to the group.

Assimilation and ethnic boundaries

For a long time, assimilation appeared as one of the most successful and
important concepts for the study of ethnicity; this status is reflected in its
classic treatment at the hands of Milton Gordon (1964). But much recent
writing on ethnicity rejects or avoids assimilation as a focus of major concern.
At least part of the reason appears to lie in an implicit model of assimilation,
which is ahistorical, individualistic and incrementalist — which, in other
words, does not connect assimilatory processes to macrostructural dynamics,

Ethnic and Racial Studies Volume 8 Number 1 January 1985


© R.K.P. 1985 0141-9870/85/0801-0134 gl.50/1
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 135
but instead conceives of them as individua decisions played out against a
static background. Such a conception naturally places the emphasis on
social psychological constructs, including the acceptability of a group's
members to the majority or core and, perhaps more importantly, their
motivation to merge with the majority. At the same time, it is implicitly
one-directional: assimilating individuals are affiliating with a new group,
thereby dropping the cultural and other garb of their original one.
This individualistic conception makes it easier to understand why assimi-
lation has slipped out of the inner circle of concern. Since it assumes that
assimilation hinges on the willingness of individuals to surrender to the
majority, then the importance of assimilation would appear to decrease as
this willingness does. And this is precisely what seemed to happen during the
1960s, in what appeared to many as a revival of ethnicity among American
groups, both the racial minorities and, somewhat surprisingly, those of
European ancestry. The revival meant, to use a characterization that, with
minor variations, rings throughout the literature on ethnicity in America,
that the ethnics were refusing to assimilate (e.g., Novak, 1972).
One difficulty with this diminishing of the importance of assimilation
as a concept is that statistical indicators, such as intermarriage rates (Alba
and Chamlin, 1983), suggest the cresting of assimilatory processes in recent
decades. The apparent contradiction with the presumed ethnic revival
indicates the limitations inherent in the individualistic conception of assimi-
lation and the need to reconceptualize it in a way that allows it to be linked to
structural processes of group formation and dissolution. One way to achieve
this is to explicitly include the notion of group boundaries within the focus
of assimilation. Group boundaries in this context refer to the recognition of
ethnic distinctions in interaction, and thus are premised upon 'criteria for
determining membership and ways of signalling membership and exclusion'
(Barth, 1969:15). Ethnic distinctions are socially maintained by such boundary
markers as language, speech mannerisms, food, culture more broadly, and
physical appearance, all of which can serve to identify group members to each
other and to outsiders.
Reexamining the concept of assimilation with the notion of group boun-
daries in mind forces the recognition of two ideal types of assimilation.
One is the type envisioned by the individualistic conception described above:
namely, an individual moves across an ethnic boundary, transferring allegiance
to another group, but without any change to the boundary itself. Assimilation
of this kind can be viewed as a sort of population trade between different
ethnic blocs (e.g., Newman, 1973; Greeley, 1971). Research advancing such
an interpretation has emphasized such consequences of intermarriage as the
conversion of one spouse to the religion of the other (Newman, 1973: 162-4).
The consequences of this kind of assimilation for ethnic change are problem-
atic; it can be plausibly argued that it does not weaken ethnicity.
The second kind is a group form: it is assimilation accomplished through
a change in ethnic boundaries, either through a weakening to reduce their
salience or through a shift that removes a previously recognized distinction.
136 Richard D. Alba
By definition, such boundary changes mean changes to ethnicity as well.
That they may occur is made plausible by the much noted observation that
the coincidence of ethnic and other boundaries, such as those of occupation
and residence, tends to enhance ethnic solidarity (Glazer and Moynihan,
1970; Hechter, 1978; Yancey et al., 1976); consequently, a dilution through
mobility of ethnic considerations in particular occupational strata or neighbor-
hoods might be expected to weaken ethnic boundaries.
Empirically, of course, there is not necessarily a sharp distinction between
the two types. Nonetheless, a separate recognition for the second type is
valuable because it forces attention to the structural factors that may enhance
or detract from ethnic solidarity, such as those stemming from the cultural
division of labor (Hechter, 1978), group size (Blau, 1977), and the institutional
completeness of ethnic communities (Breton, 1964).
The type of assimilation at the group level also underlines the cardinal
importance of studying interethnic relations, since they provide a means of
detecting ethnic boundaries and the changes that occur to them. The same
does not hold true for the 'content' of boundaries, i.e., the cultural and other
signs of group membership, which may change without change to the boun-
daries themselves (Barth, 1969); for this reason, the study of culture by
itself is not decisive for resolving questions of ethnicity. The occurrence, even
the frequent occurrence, of interethnic relations also need not contradict the
existence of an ethnic boundary, but the maintenance of such a boundary
requires that interethnic contacts be asymmetric in some fundamental way,
as would be true of relations between members of groups of unequal status
(Barth, 1969). Interactions structured by ethnicity help to maintain ethnic
distinctions. This is generally not the case for symmetric, nonsegmental
relations, such as those of friendship and marriage. A change in the pattern
of such relations is a signal of a change in ethnic boundaries.1

A case in point: the Italian Americans


Italian Americans provide an intriguing example of the significance of boundary-
shift processes as well as a litmus test for the most frequently advanced
interpretations of ethnicity. Thus, those who argue for the persistence or
revival of ethnicity generally point to white ethnic groups such as the Italians
to support their arguments. In this view, Italians remain entrenched in ethnicity
partly because of their recency of arrival and partly because their core values —
in particular, the values embodied in the family — have enabled them to
maintain solidary ethnic communities, manifest for example in vital urban
neighborhoods.
These contemporary arguments find an echo in older ones. Few argued on
behalf of the assimilability of the Italians at the time of their arrival, for they
entered as one of the most despised of European immigrant groups (Higham,
1970). The bulk of the Italian immigration before the close of mass immigration
in the 1920s came from the rural villages of the south, or Mezzogiorno,
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 137
although because of the imprecision of both American and Italian statistics,
it is not possible to estimate precisely the proportion from southern provinces
(Sori, 1979).2 The available statistics, however, do clearly support the well-
known overall picture of an immigration swollen with a dislocated peasantry.
For example, tabulations published by the Immigration Commission of
1911 reveal that in the crucial period 1899-1910, when 2.22 million Italian
immigrants arrived on American shores (44 percent of the total from 1820 to
1970), 32 percent of those with European work experience described them-
selves as farm laborers and an additional 43 percent as laborers (Kessner,
1977: 33-4). The general category of 'laborer,' or bracciante in Italian,
included many who had only recently been forced out of agricultural work
(Sori, 1979).
The experiences of southern Italians hardly constituted preparation for
integration into an urban, industrial society. The Mezzogiorno presents a
classical picture of an underdeveloped society where the penetration of
capitalist markets of land and labor created severe dislocations, uprooting
peasants from the land and transforming them into a rural proletariat. By the
latter part of the nineteenth century, many rural dwellers were forced to
work the land of others, frequently under share-cropping or other tenancy
arrangements that gave them little return for their efforts. Patterns of land
holding and land use, combined with unfavorable climate and topography,
produced an agriculture of scarcity, characterized by chronic shortages of
work and food (Covello, 1972; Schneider and Schneider, 1976; Sori, 1979).
And the nature of the work bore little relation either to farm or industrial
work in the United States. The tools were primitive, so that, according to
the immigrant writer, Constantine Panunzio, 'When they come to America,
the work which comes nearest to that which they did in Italy is not farming
or even farm labor, but excavation work' (Panunzio, 1928: 78; quoted by
Kessner, 1977: 39).
The cultural values engendered by the social and material contours of the
Mezzogiorno also did not mesh well with the exigencies in the United States.
In such a landscape of scarcity, a supreme value was placed on the family.
It has been observed many times that the family, not the individual, was the
basic social atom of Mezzogiorno society; in the well-known words of Robert
Foerster,
Life in the South exalts the family. It has been said of Sicily that the
family sentiment is perhaps the only deeply rooted moral sentiment that
prevails. (Foerster, 1924: 95)
This was not, however, the 'amoral familism' of Banfield (1958), which
portrays Mezzogiorno life as a Hobbesian war pitting each nuclear family
against all others. Southern Italian social structure was constituted in good
part from filaments of family-like relations extending beyond the nuclear
family, such as extended kinship, fictive kinship created through the institution
of godparentship (compareggio), and friendship (amicizia) (Chapman, 1971;
Schneider and Schneider, 1976).
138 Richard D. Alba
An aspect of the southern Italian ethos with repercussions for Italians in
America lay in the presumption that family interests should take precedence
over individual ones. A well-known instance of this occurred in relation to
marriage. Since the position of a family was affected by the marriages of its
members, families attempted to exert considerable control over the choice
of a spouse, to the point that many marriages were arranged. Family control
was enhanced by the sexual provisions of the Mezzogiorno's code of honor,
which drastically restricted contact between eligible men and women
(Chapman, 1971). A second instance lay in the economic value attached to
children. In peasant families, children were generally expected to make an
economic contribution as soon as they were able to work, beginning usually
during adolescence. The early initiation to work brought an abrupt transition
to adulthood. It also generally spelled the end of formal schooling. This was
in any event in accordance with the family-centered culture, in which education
was regarded with suspicion, as a potential danger to family solidarity (Covello,
1972: 257; Gambino, 1974).
This occupational and cultural background powerfully shaped the niche
the immigrants were able to establish for themselves. The majority of Italian
immigrants sought work in urban labor markets, in part because they
frequently intended to repatriate after earning enough money to improve
their position and this limited them to places where employment was readily
available. But immigrants fresh from the peasantry discovered upon their
arrival that only 'peek and shuvil' work, as Panunzio described it (quoted by
Kessner, 1977: 58), was open to them. In 1905 in New York City, i.e., at the
height of immigration in the city with the largest concentration of Italians in
the United States, nearly 60 percent of Italian household heads did unskilled
or semiskilled manual labor, working on construction gangs or as rag pickers
and longshoremen (Kessner, 1977: 52—9). The reasons were not limited to
a shortage of skills that could be applied in the industrial sector. Culturally
engendered expectations about the nature of work, carried from the Mezzo-
giorno, also constrained occupational possibilities. That many immigrant
men took jobs in construction or on the docks was partly a result of a prefer-
ence for outdoor work, an attempt to reproduce familiar work cycles and
conditions. This preference tended to consign Italians to seasonal work outside
the regular channels of blue-collar mobility, which were found in factories
(Yans-McLaughlin, 1977: 35-44).
Culture also limited the work horizons of women. One of Mezzogiorno's
strongest prohibitions was directed against contact between women and
male strangers, and this powerful norm went far toward defining what was an
acceptable work situation for women. Work in the home was strongly
preferred. Some took in boarders (generally relatives or paesani in order not
to compromise the family honor), and others homework such as laundering
or the manufacture of artificial flowers. One instance where Italian women
did work outside the home occurred in the New York City garment industry,
where women could work among other women (Yans-McLaughlin, 1977:
50-4).
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 139
Immigrant adjustment was complicated by the intention to repatriate. The
number who ultimately returned to the Mezzogiorno is uncertain, but clearly
it was large; one estimate is that 1.5 million Italians returned from the United
States in the years between 1900 and 1914 (Caroli, 1973: 41). The sojourner's
orientation toward the homeland, felt undoubtedly also by many who stayed,
delayed such important adaptations as the acquisition of citizenship and the
learning of English.3 Lieberson's study of ten cities, for example, shows that
m 1930, at a point when new immigration had all but ceased, Italians had the
highest percentage of foreign-born who did not speak English in nine of the
cities (they ranked second highest in the other); and they had the first or
second highest percentages of immigrants who were not citizens in eight
(Lieberson, 1963: 206-18). Obviously, this retarded adaptation had a large
impact on the group, disadvantaging it relative to other immigrant groups
who arrived around the same time (particularly in relation to Jews, who did
not wish to return to the European societies from which they fled (Kessner,
1977: 167)).
The prospects for Italians seemed bleak also on the basis of American
reactions to them. The Italian group arrived in a period when racial ideologies
were widespread in the United States; and its arrival served to stimulate their
further development, as Italians became a focus for explicitly racist thinking
and stereotypes. The Italians were perceived as prone to crime, both organized
and that spurred by passion and vengeance, the latter symbolized for Americans
by the stiletto (Higham, 1970: 66—7). The Italian distinctiveness was perceived
in physical terms as well: the immigrants were 'swarthy' and seemed to bear
other signs of physical degradation, such as low foreheads. In the racially
conscious climate, at a time when race theoreticians were attempting to
draw biological distinctions among European peoples to the disfavor of those
from the south and east, the question of color may have been unavoidable.
It would go much too far to say that Italians were viewed as non-whites, but
their color position was problematic. This is evident in the common epithet
for them, 'guinea,' which was derived from a term referring originally to
slaves from the western coast of Africa (Mencken, 1963: 373; Craigie and
Hulbert, 1940: 1192-3).

The situation in the 1930s


The assimilability of the Italians continued to seem unlikely in the 1930s,
after the close of the period of mass immigration. This is not to deny that
significant cultural changes had taken place by then; these were especially
evident in the transition to the second generation. Important aspects of the
family-centered culture of the Mezzogiorno were so attuned to southern
Italian situations that they could not be reestablished successfully in the
United States. For example, strict control over unmarried daughters was
only workable in southern Italian villages, where parents were in a position
to evaluate the suitability of all potential suitors. Parental superiority broke
down in American ghettos, since more acculturated children were better able
140 Richard D. Alba
to make appropriate matches for themselves. The extent of change in family
norms is suggested by Ware's study of Greenwich Village in the early 1930s
(Ware, 1935: 180-202). In a survey of its Italian residents, she found clear-cut
differences between older and younger respondents, a division that no doubt
corresponded well with generational status (i.e., foreign versus native-born).
Older Italians were less likely than younger ones to reject such Mezzogiorno
family norms as 'girls should not associate with men unless engaged' and
parental arrangement of marriages.
But in other ways, the same survey indicates second-generation fidelity
to the southern Italian cultural heritage. Only half the younger group rejected
the proposition that 'a child should sacrifice his personal ambition to the
welfare of the family group'; and only 15 percent denied that 'children owe
absolute obedience to parents' (Ware, 1935: 193).
One area in which the remaining power of the family ethos was undeniably
manifest was that of education. The conflict between the school system and
the family that had existed in the Mezzogiorno was renewed in America.
Immigrant families perceived many points of friction in the contact between
these culturally alien worlds. These occurred even in seemingly innocuous
matters such as school recreation, which immigrant parents saw as creating
moral and physical risk for teen-agers, who in their eyes were already adults
(Covello, 1972: 325-6). Undoubtedly, the most important conflict centered
on the economic contribution expected of children, which was jeopardized
by compulsory attendance laws, greatly resented by Italian parents.
As a result of the clash between school and family, Italian children had
high rates of truancy and frequently left school as early as the law allowed
(Covello, 1972). In fact, during the height of mass immigration, it is estimated
that as many as 10 percent of the immigrant children in New York City
managed to avoid school altogether (Kessner, 1977: 96). But even as late as
1930, only 11 percent of Italian Americans who entered New York City high
schools graduated from them, at a time when over 40 percent of all the city's
high school students stayed through to receive their diplomas (Covello, 1972:
285). The obvious consequence was low ultimate educational attainment for
second-generation Italians and a channeling of them towards jobs where
educational credentials were not important, mostly in the blue-collar ranks.
The ultimate assimilation of the Italians was also put in question by
attitudes of the Italians themselves. Two studies of Italian-American ghettos,
in Boston and New Haven, offer relevant testimony. Whyte's (1955) classic
study indicates a split among Italians in their attitudes towards assimilation.
He portrays the division in terms of 'college boys,' oriented toward mobility
into the larger society, and 'corner boys,' loyal to their peer groups and held
on ghetto corners by that loyalty. Whyte did not provide direct evidence on
the relative popularity of these two orientations, but Child's (1943) New
Haven study did. Child depicted the attitudes of Italians as defined against
a background of virulent prejudice directed at their group, which hedged in
the possible choices with the risk of potential losses. Identification with the
Italian group meant risking complete exclusion by other Americans and
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 141
the loss of any prospects for mobility. On the other hand, identification
with Americans, and hence a positive valuation of assimilation, risked a
double rejection: by non-Italians as a result of prejudice and by other Italians
on the grounds of disloyalty to the group. According to Child, the most
common response to this double-bind situation was one he labeled 'apathetic':
a denial of the meaningfulness of nationality distinctions and of the existence
of prejudice against Italians. Individuals displaying the apathetic response
remained through inertia within the orbit of Italian-American social and
cultural life, for it required deliberate action to break this social gravity
and move into non-Italian spheres. Because of the risks involved, few main-
tained such intentions.

The 1940s and 50s: the watershed


By the end of the 1930s, an analysis based solely on the group's experiences
and its cultural and occupational background would seem to have doomed
Italian Americans to a perpetual position of inferiority and separateness in
American society. But such an analysis would have been misleading because
other developments were taking place in the larger society that affected the
context within which Italian-American preferences would be played out.
These factors came to a head during and shortly after World War II.
Some had been in the background all along, but the war sharpened their
effects. One such was the transformation of the occupational structure and
the attendant structural mobility. Between 1930 and 1970, for example, the
white-collar proportion of the national labor force expanded rapidly from
29.4 to 44.8 percent (all figures are from U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1975:
139); about half this change, moreover, was concentrated in the upper
part of the white-collar spectrum, the category of professional and technical
workers, whose share of the labor force increased in this period from 6.8
to 13.7 percent. Although the proportion in the combined blue-collar and
service occupations hardly changed, within them a significant realignment
was taking shape. In particular, unskilled laborers, a category which included
many Italian Americans in the earlier part of the century, declined sharply
from 11.0 to 4.4 percent. The structural mobility engendered by such shifts
in the occupational distribution holds a special significance for disadvantaged
ethnic groups because it does not have a 'zero sum' character. Thus, the
upward mobility of an individual or group can occur without the comple-
mentary downward mobility of another; and as a result, it is not likely to
produce a heightened salience of group boundaries among more advantaged
groups, intended to keep the disadvantaged in their place.
The effects wrought by structural mobility were most sharply felt in those
places where Italian Americans were concentrated: the metropolitan areas of
the north. This is made clearest by examining the kinds of jobs that were
opening up and closing out in different places in the post-war interval, since
it is the changes at the margins that chiefly dictate the occupational options
for young people entering the labor force and thus shape intergenerational
142 Richard D. Alba
occupational mobility. Over the period 1940—60, metropolitan areas in
general were the places of greatest job growth (Stanback and Knight, 1970).
In the older metropolises of the Northeast and Midwest, growth was primarily
concentrated in white-collar rather than blue-collar jobs (Berry and Kasarda,
1977: ch. 12).
A corollary of structural occupational shifts during the 1940s and 1950s
was another kind of structural mobility: the rapid expansion of higher
education and its transformation from a selective system to a mass one. In
1940, only 15 percent of the college-age group actually attended college, but
by 1954 the rate of college attendance had climbed to 30 percent; by 1960,
it was almost 38 percent (Trow, 1961). This expansion played an important
role in reducing status differences because, in addition to propelling occu-
pational mobility, higher education extends a sense of equality among its
students through an experience that is viewed as a sharp alteration in status
and is sanctified by the selectivity of colleges and universities.
World War II acted as a catalyst for both kinds of mobility. The War helped
to drag the United States out of the Depression and open up an era of pros-
perity and economic growth, signaled by a steady growth in real income
beginning in the early 1950s (Miller, 1971); and it specifically fueled the
expansion of higher education through the G.I. Bill. But the impact of the
war was much wider than the socioeconomic changes it helped to stimulate,
for the war had a powerful effect on American perceptions of nationality
and national origins.
The crux of the wartime situations during this century is that they have
turned ethnic identity into a matter of national loyalty, thereby giving
ethnicity a subversive appearance and ultimately hastening a deemphasis
on nationality differences. The diversity of the origins of Americans and the
substantial proportion of those of recent origins, particularly from combatant
nations, have made Americans sensitive to the potential frailty of national
solidarity. During World War I, the presence of millions of recently arrived
European immigrants provoked intense anxieties about the immediate loyalties
of aliens and the potential for subversion from within, leading to overt xeno-
phobia and demands for the 'pressure-cooker assimilation' and '100 percent
Americanization' of the immigrants (Higham, 1970). By the 1940s, the flood
tide of immigration had receded; the groups with the potential for loyalty
to enemy nations were increasingly composed of the native-born, and the
responses of Americans were accordingly different.
This is not to say that the war did not stimulate anxieties over national
loyalty. The internment of Japanese Americans demonstrates indisputably
that it did. In the case of European ethnics, clouds of suspicion gathered early
during the war over Germans and Italians, but then largely gave way to a
cultivated national unity that was also a response to the wartime strains.
The melding of Americans of different nationalities was almost ritualistically
promoted by festivals to celebrate the contributions of immigrant groups to
America (Polenberg, 1980: 54). More significant, wartime reporting and films
about the war made for domestic consumption self-consciously highlighted
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 143
the spirit of unity among American fighting men from different backgrounds,
portraying the armed forces as a melting pot in miniature (Blum, 1976: 63).
The war no doubt served to drive home the perils of too strong an ethnic
identification for many ethnics. One of Child's New Haven respondents
sharply formulated a general problem:

Then a lot of times in the show you see Mussolini on the screen and they
all start to razz him. Then I feel, 'How the hell do I stand?' (Child, 1943:
88)

A frequent response on the part of the ethnics was a push toward further
assimilation. Ethnics had high rates of enlistment in the military, and there
was massive adoption of American citizenship by the foreign-born — more
than 1,750,000 became citizens in the period 1940-45 (Polenberg, 1980:
57). Movement toward acculturation is evident in the waning of the foreign
language press that occurred during the war. The number of radio stations
broadcasting in immigrant languages dropped by 40 percent between 1942
and 1948 (Polenberg, 1980: 55).
An ultimate impact of the war was to render the perceptions of the
ethnics more fluid and thus open to the possibility of change. One realm
in which this influence is visible is in the novels about the war, published
during it and afterwards. Norman Mailer's The Naked and the Dead, James
Jones's From Here to Eternity, Harry Brown's A Walk in the Sun, and John
Hersey''s A Bell for Adano, which were all popular novels made into successful
films, presented a very different version of American society from that
which prevailed before the war. like many wartime films, these novels
depicted military groups that contained American ethnic diversity, or more
precisely the part of European ancestry, in microcosm, and showed ethnics as
the moral equals of those of 'old stock' origins (Blum, 1976). The novels,
which served to interpret the war experience for many Americans, signaled
a shift in attitudes towards ethnics.
Thus, World War II stands as a watershed for European ethnics, partly
because it lies at a fortuitous conjunction of forces — structural transformation
of the labor force, demographic transition from the immigrant to the second
generation among the ethnics of recent European origins, and a cultural
relaxation of the attitudes towards ethnics — that served to fluidify the
boundaries separating ethnics from old stock groups. It remains still to
confirm that these massive forces actually had an effect on the life chances
of ethnics. Relevant evidence is supplied by Lieberson's recent study (1980),
which reveals a prodigious socioeconomic leap for the 1925—35 cohort of
second-generation South-Central-Eastern European ethnics, which came to
maturity during and shortly after the war (Lieberson, 1980: 200-6, 328-32).
The boundary fluidity associated with the large-scale mobility in the after-
math of the war was further advanced by the enormous residential movements
of the 1950s and 1960s. In the single decade from 1950 to 1960, the
population in the suburbs increased by nearly 50 percent, from 41 to 60
144 Richard D. Alba
million (Polenberg, 1980: 128). For ethnics and others, the suburban exodus
was often directly connected with occupational chances — and not merely
the result of increasing affluence — since the bulk of newly created jobs were
to be found in the suburban fringes, not central-city areas (Berry and Kasarda,
1977: ch. 12). But the exodus was full of portent for ethnic groups because
it disrupted urban ethnic communities and brought many mobile families into
an ethnically heterogeneous milieu, a shift with obvious ramifications for the
next generation. The residential changes of Italians are exemplified by the
group's distribution in the metropolitan region centered around New York
City and Newark, which contained nearly a third of the Italian Americans the
Census counted in 1970. By then, the second generation had significantly
dispersed to the suburbs. According to Census figures, 47 percent were living
in the area's smaller places, those with fewer than 100,000 residents; and 41
percent were living in places with fewer than 50,000. These figures are
only slightly lower than those for whites generally (50 percent and 45 percent,
respectively). However, first-generation Italians remained distinctly more
concentrated in the region's larger cities. Only 35 percent were in places
smaller than 100,000 in population, and 29 percent in places smaller than
50.000.4
Obviously, the changes of the post-war period did not mean a complete
dissolution of ethnic communities and subcultures. Gans's (1962) study of
Boston's West End in the late 1950s establishes that many, particularly in
the urban working class, remained firmly in the grip of ethnic worlds. But a
process had been initiated, one that spelled a gradual lowering of ethnic
boundaries among European ancestry groups and an upward shift in the life
chances of their younger members.

The contemporary situation of Italian Americans


This process of boundary shift has had a profound impact on Italian Americans,
and recent evidence points to a convergence with other European ancestry
groups, including those of older stock. As one demonstration, consider the
educational trajectory across different cohorts of second- and later-generation
Italians, compiled in Table 1 from the November, 1979, Current Population
Survey.s To provide a rigorous yardstick against which to measure change,
comparable figures are also provided for third- and later-generation Americans
of exclusively British ancestry (defined as those who report ancestry only
from England, Scotland, and Wales). Such a comparison group avoids the
confusion that might be introduced by including other recent ethnics in the
reference group and also compares the Italians to an ethnic category that is
indisputably part of the American core, thus underlining the sharpness of the
changes. For similar reasons, the focus in the table is exclusively on rates of
college attendance and graduation.
What stands out in the table is a pattern of convergence across cohorts.
Although the pattern is complicated somewhat by an unsustained peak in
college education among British Americans in the 1946—50 cohort (which
British Amercians."
Men
Third-generation
Second generation Third generation British Americans
% attended % finished % attended % finished % attended % finished
Cohort college 4 or more yrs. college 4 or more yrs. college 4 or more yrs.
1951- 56.6 28.9 54.4 25.8 53.2 27.1
1946-50 42.1 32.8 55.9 29.1 66.4 38.1
1941-45 45.4 26.2 51.8 35.7 55.7 38.5
1936-40 42.9 30.3 42.3 22.1 51.5 35.1
1931-35 33.0 18.7 39.0 18.4 50.3 31.3 S
1926-30 24.9 11.7 31.5 15.2 42.1 27.8
1921-25 22.1 16.0 20.1 7.7 43.9 23.8 I
1916-20- 17.5 3.9 13.4 11.3 35.7 20.1 S
-15 16.3 7.9 15.2 6.2 30.1 17.4
!
Women
1951 — 50.4 20.2 46.6 26.3 48.8 24.0
1946-50 35.0 17.1 40.5 20.0 53.5 31.8
1941-45 27.3 13.1 32.1 13.5 44.7 22.5 i
1936-40 28.3 13.2 18.2 3.9 39.0 21.7
1931-35 9.1 4.4 17.9 10.5 33.2 16.1
1926-30 14.6 5.7 27.6 9.2 41.8 23.0 3
1921-25 8.2 4.5 22.6 5.1 29.7 14.5
1916-20 7.0 4.2 30.1 20.2 37.6 17.8
-15 5.1 1.8 2.7 0.0 26.5 12.3
Source: My tabluations from November, 1979, Current Population Survey
Table restricted to individuals older than 22. The 'third generation' contains all native-born group members with native-born parents and thus encompasses
the third and later generations.
146 Richard D. Alba
may be part of a Vietnam era phenomenon revealed by a recent Census
report) and by some wandering of the numbers from a simple trajectory of
linear change, its basic nature is clear: a gradual narrowing of Italian differences
from British Americans and the achievement of parity in the youngest cohort
(who were in their mid-twenties in 1979). This convergence holds for both
men and women, and indeed what the table also reveals is the relatively
greater disadvantage of Italian-American women in the past, especially in
the second generation. For this last group, the rise in college attendance (from
9.1 percent to 50.4 percent) across a twenty-year time span, from the 1930-35
cohort to that of 1951—56, is very strong. The convergence also holds for
both generations, and underlining the historical nature of the convergence
is the fact that the generations do not seem much different, although the
third generation shows some tendency to take the lead in rising rates of
college attendance.
Evidence of cultural convergence is provided by survey items that tap
attitudes and values connected with the stereotypical family-centered ethos
presumed to color Italian-American life (e.g., Greeley and McCready, 1975).6
One widely cited expression of this is greater loyalty to kin groups, purportedly
evident for instance in a^eluctance to move away from the family (Gambino,
1974; Vecoli, 1978). Another is conservatism on family-related matters,
ranging from hostility toward changes in sexual mores and the position
of women to a low frequency of divorce (Greeley, 1974; Femminella and
Quadagno, 1976).
The thinness of any residual cultural patina among individuals of Italian
heritage is evident from Table 2, which reports the analysis of items from the
General Social Surveys for the years 1975 through 1980 (the table is a
selection from a larger set discussed in Alba, 1985).7 The comparison is
again to those of British ancestry (more precisely: since the General Social
Surveys ask for the religion in which a respondent was raised, the comparison
group contains those with Protestant ancestry from the British Isles). The
table presents the comparison without any controls and also with controls
for: current region and size of place as well as those where the respondent
was raised; education and occupation of respondent and parents; and sex
and age.8
On items relating to traditional family roles, Italians are generally quite
similar to WASPs. They do not significantly differ from WASPs in terms of
acceptance of abortion, for example, although they appear slightly more
conservative after controls are applied because of their greater concentration
in areas where liberal attitudes prevail.9 Italians do not differ from WASPs
in their acceptance of women outside the home.10 Similarity between the
groups is also found, albeit with an important exception, in attitudes toward
the raising of children. Italians have been depicted as emphasizing traditional
values, rather than those of self-direction (Rosen, 1959; Schooler, 1976).
But in terms of their rating of the desirability of various traits in children,
they are not meaningfully different from British Protestants.11 Worthy of
mention for its echo of the Mezzogiorno is one trait on the list, 'that he
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 147
Table 2. Cultural comparison between WASPs and Italian Americans.

WASP Italian diff. after


mean mean difference adjustment3
Anti-abortion scale 1.33 1.42 .09 .20*
Anti-feminism scale 1.26 1.25 -.01 .08
Premarital sex is 'always wrong' 34.5% 22.6% -11.9* 1.3
Adultery is 'always wrong' 69.8% 58.6% -11.2* -3.5
Homosexual sex is 'always wrong' 69.3% 60.4% -8.9* -4.7
Ever divorced or legally separated 25.7% 21.9% -3.8 -4.4
Divorce should be 'more difficult' 50.1% 41.3% -8.8* -3.1
Scale of value put on self-direction
for children 1.24 1.17 -.07 -.12
Young people 'should be taught by
their elders' 37.9% 53.0% 15.1* 19.8*
Reside in same place where grew up 39.5% 53.2% 13.6* 6.6*
Socialize with relatives weekly 33.8% 46.8% 13.0* 10.4*

•Indicates statistical significance.


Source: Tabulations from the NORC General Social Surveys, 1975-80.
a
Variables for which adjustment has been made include: current region and
size of place and those where respondent grew up, education and occupation
of respondent and parents, age and sex.

[the child] obeys his parents well.' Just a quarter of the Italian-American
respondents prize obedience as one of the most desirable traits in children,
a figure not statistically different from that for WASPs (28 percent). The
exception to this general similarity concerns whether young people should
be taught 'by their elders to do what is right' or 'to think for themselves
even though they may do something their elders disapprove of.' About half
of Italians agree with the position consistent with the family-centered ethos —
namely, that young people should be taught by their elders — compared
to 38 percent of WASPs. Nonetheless, the Italian percentage is not far from
the one for all Americans, 45 percent of whom favor the traditional option.
Despite their conservative image, Italians are more liberal than WASPs in
certain respects, apparently because of their location in the metropolitan
northeast, where cosmopolitan outlooks are frequent. They are less likely to
condemn adultery, premarital sex, and homosexuality as 'always wrong.'
They are also less likely to feel that divorce laws should be tightened to make
divorce more difficult to obtain. (The proportion who have ever been divorced
or separated is also, incidentally, not statistically different from that found
among British Americans.) But in all these cases, the differences disappear
after statistical controls are introduced, and an inspection of the regressions
indicates that the reduction is chiefly brought about by the controls for
place.
148 Richard D. Alba
Broadly speaking, then, there is little support for the image of a distinctive
Italian conservatism on family matters. Where there does appear to be greater
evidence for an Italian-American ethos is in terms of loyalty to the family
group, but at best its remaining strength seems no more than moderate. This
loyalty can be examined through two items in Table 2.
One tests the idea that Italians remain rooted in one place because of their
reluctance to move away from family. Indeed, an impressive 53 percent
reside in the same place where they grew up; however, the percentage of
WASPs who do so also is high, 40 percent. Moreoever, the Italian percentage
could be expected to be higher on the grounds that Italians have more
frequently grown up in the cosmopolitan magnets that attract others from
their hometowns (New York City is the prototype) and also have lower
overall educational and occupational attainment, factors associated with
less residential mobility. When controls are applied, the difference between
the two groups is only modest, 7 percentage points.12
Finally, the Italian pattern of socializing with relatives, emphasized by
Gans (1962) in his depiction of the 'peer group society,' still persists to some
degree. Nearly half of Italians socialize with family members weekly or more
frequently, compared to only a third of WASPs. This difference is not explained
very much by the background variables, as the tendency to socialize within
the family is not much affected by socioeconomic variables, and this is
counterbalanced for WASPs by the fact that it is somewhat higher among
those who live in smaller places. After controls, Italians are still 10 percent
more likely to socialize on a weekly basis with relatives.
Thus, what remains of the family-centered ethos is a slightly greater
tendency to remain in the same place, greatly diluted from ancestral peasant
rootedness, and a moderately greater willingness to keep company with
relatives. The evidence of cultural convergence seems substantial,13 but there
is still more imposing evidence of convergence and assimilation: in inter-
marriage rates. Intermarriage stands as the cardinal indicator of boundary
shift for several reasons (cf. Merton, 1941). To begin with the obvious, because
marriage is an enduring and intimate relation, intermarriage provides a stringent
test of group perceptions, of the social distance between Italians and others.
Moreover, an intermarriage is not simply an isolated crossing of ethnic boun-
daries but carries far wider ramifications, including most importantly those
for the next generation, which will be raised in an ethnically heterogeneous
milieu. Finally, the occurrence of intermarriage implies the occurrence of
other relations that penetrate ethnic boundaries.
The intermarriage rates of Italians, calculated from the 1979 Current
Population Survey, are presented in Table 3. In the case of marriage, it makes
little sense to combine individuals of part Italian ancestry with those of
wholly Italian parentage, because the social contexts in which the two types
are raised are so different that their intermarriage rates are likely to be as
well; and consequently, they are shown separately in the table. The marriage
rates are also decomposed by generations and birth cohort, and presented
separately for men and women.
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 149
The table indicates a rapid rise in the intermarriage rate, which has reached
the point that, of Italians marrying recently, generally two-thirds to three-
quarters, depending on the category of the group, have intermarried. Revealing
of the changes is the trend by birth cohort for persons with unmixed Italian
ancestry, especially in the second generation. Among those born before 1920,
i.e., during the era of mass immigration, about 60 percent of this second
generation chose spouses of wholly Italian percentage. But this strict endogamy
falls off with each new cohort. Among men, a sharp drop occurs with the
cohort born during the 1930s; for women, such a drop occurs with the cohort
born in the next decade. This rapid change has, among men, closed the gap
between the second and third generations. For both, only about 20 percent
of men born since 1950 have chosen wives with all Italian parentage, while
another 10 to 15 percent have chosen wives with part Italian ancestry. The
gap between the generations is not quite closed among women; second-
generation women have the highest rate of endogamy in the youngest cohort,
although this may be a statistical aberration, since a small number of cases
is involved. In any event, the great majority of Italian Americans in this

Table 3. Marriage patterns of Italian Americans, by sex, generation, and cohort.

Men
Second generation Third generation
Cohort Ancestry of spouse i s . . . Ancestry of spouse i s . . .
wholly wholly wholly wholly
Ancestry type Italian % non-Italian % Italian % non-Italian %

1950-
wholly Ital. 20.3 64.1 20.0 70.5
partly Ital. —a —a 5.4 78.9
1940-49
wholly Ital. 30.0 60.0 24.4 69.2
partly Ital. 0.0 82.7 10.7 76.8
1930-39
wholly Ital. 29.8 62.9 24.1 63.3
partly Ital. 17.8 81.5 6.9 80.1
1920-29
wholly Ital. 44.6 51.7 38.8 60.9
partly Ital. 15.7 83.7 4.8 90.0
Before 1920
wholly Ital. 56.7 41.7 42.7 57.3
partly Ital. —a —a 15.8 78.6
150 Richard D. Alba
Table 3. Continued
Women
Second generation Third generation
Cohort Ancestry of spouse i s . . . Ancestry of spouse i s . . .
wholly wholly wholly wholly
Ancestry type Italian % non-Italian % Italian % non-Italian %

1950-
wholly Ital. 38.7 53.2 23.8 72.7
partly Ital. —a —a 10.3 79.1
1940-49
wholly Ital. 25.7 71.3 31.7 58.4
partly Ital. 20.7 72.1 11.7 77.8
1930-39
wholly Ital. 38.8 61.0 49.4 46.6
partly Ital. 17.0 83.0 17.2 75.6
1920-29
wholly Ital. 54.9 44.6 34.6 61.7
partly Ital. 10.3 89.7 18.6 69.3
Before 1920
wholly Ital. 59.5 37.6 40.8 60.2
partly Ital. —a —a 11.5 78.5

Source: My tabulations from November, 1979, Current Population Survey.


a
Percent not reported because it is based on 10 or fewer cases (weighted).

cohort belong to the third or later generations, where high intermarriage


rates prevail.
Individuals of wholly Italian ancestry provide a conservative estimate of
intermarriage rates. Individuals of mixed background have higher inter-
marriage rates and, moreover, the overall Italian rates will increasingly resemble
theirs, since the group is more and more composed of them (this will be
made clear shortly). For example, if the two ancestry groups are combined
among men, then nearly three-quarters of third-generation men born since
1950 have chosen wives with no Italian ancestry. The comparable third-
generation figure among women is nearly identical to that for the men.
It might be argued that these high intermarriage rates do not establish
by themselves a relaxing of boundaries between Italians and other groups
because they do not show whom Italians marry when they marry outside.
Thus, it remains possible that other boundaries, enclosing clusters of culturally
and socially similar groups, constrain their choices. It is true that, like other
European ancestry groups, Italians are very unlikely to marry Hispanics and
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 151
non-whites (Alba and Golden, 1984). But this important exception aside,
two pieces of evidence damage the thesis of selective intermarriage. One is
Alba and Kessler's (1979) analysis of marriage patterns among Catholics,
demonstrating that very little selectivity is visible among those who marry
across nationality lines. The second emerges from data that reveal fairly high
rates of marriage across religious lines. For example, Alba (1985) shows from
General Social Survey data that about half of Italian Catholics born since
World War II have married Protestants. So, in other words, it appears that
the elective affinities of intermarrying Italians are not narrowly channeled
to a few groups, but range widely across the spectrum of European ancestries.
The rising rate of intermarriage is bringing about a profound transformation
of the Italian ancestry group. The character of this transformation is quite
evident when the proportion of individuals with mixed Italian ancestry is
displayed by birth cohort, as is done in Table 4. 14 The figures reveal a striking
relationship of mixed parentage to cohort, with a percentage change between
the oldest and youngest cohorts of over 75 percent These dramatic figures
indicate that a tremendous swing in the nature of the Italian ancestry group is.
destined to take place by the end of this century, as members of older cohorts,
for the most part of unmixed ancestry, die and are replaced by younger
persons of mixed parentage. Thus, in the Current Population Survey, persons
with only Italian ancestry make up two-thirds of the adult ancestry group, a
comfortable majority. But counting individuals of all ages, including children,
they were a scant majority; 52 percent. Taking into account the expected
mortality in the older group, these figures suggest that individuals with one
non-Italian parent will compose a majority of the ancestry group by the end
of the next decade.

Table 4. Type of Italian ancestry by age (1979).

% with mixed
Italian ancestry
all ages 48.0
65 and over 5.9
55 to 64 11.4
45 to 54 18.5
35 to 44 36.1
25 to 34 48.1
18 to 24 60.5
14 to 17 71.3
5 to 13 . 77.8
under 5 81.5

Source: November, 1979, Current Population Survey, report in U.S. Bureau


of the Census (1982): Table 2.
152 Richard D. Alba
It is doubtful that even the mild distinctiveness of Italians on matters of
family solidarity can withstand such higher intermarriage rates. Intermarriage
not only tests the extent of existing cultural differences among groups, but it
ultimately alters the cultural boundary. Johnson's (1982) study of kinship
contact among Italians in Syracuse, New York, illustrates the general process.
She compared in-married and out-married Italians to each other and to
Protestants of non-Italian background in terms of the frequency of their
contact with parents, siblings, and other relatives. Although contact with the
relatives on the Italian side appeared dominant among the intermarried Italians,
in the sense that both spouses saw more of them than of the non-Italian
relatives, the frequency of contact was diminished; the intermarried group
stood intermediate between the in-married Italians, the majority of whom
had daily contact with parents and with siblings, and the Protestants, the
majority of whom had comparatively infrequent contact with their relatives.
Johnson's research implies that high rates of intermarriage are associated with
further erosion of what Herbert Gans labeled as the 'peer group society' in
the 1950s.

Conclusion
Italian Americans are on the verge of the twilight of their ethnicity. 'Twilight'
appears an accurate metaphor for a stage when ethnic differences will remain
visible, but only faintly so. The metaphor acknowledges the claims of many
(e.g., Glazer and Moynihan, 1970; Greeley, 1977) that indeed ethnicity has
not speedily disappeared and, therefore, the optimism of the melting-pot
portrayal of American society seems to have been ill-founded. At the same
time, it also captures the reality that ethnicity, at least among whites, seems
to be steadily receding.
The approach of this twilight may seem deceiving, for when Italians and
some other white ethnic groups are observed in the aggregate, their ethnic
features still appear prominent. But in the case of the Italians, this happens
because earlier generations and older cohorts are quite different from old-
stock Americans on such factors as educational and occupational attainment.
Hence, it is only when the group is analytically decomposed by generation or
birth cohort that the leading edge of change can be discerned.
Properly analyzed, the evidence on behalf of the looming ethnic twilight
among Italians appears overwhelming. Despite the widely accepted image of
an intense, family-centered Italian-American culture, the group's cultural
distinctiveness has paled to a feeble version of its former self. Paralleling this
change, the social boundary between Italians and other Americans has become
easily permeable; intermarriage, an irrevocable indicator of boundary shifts,
takes place quite freely between Italians and those of other European ancestries.
Acculturation and social assimilation have been fed by a surge in the edu-
cational attainment of Italians, which has brought cohorts born since World
War II to the brink of parity with British Americans, the quintessential
American group. Moreover, this profound transformation of the Italian
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 153
group has taken place at a time when the fourth generation, the first genera-
tion without direct contact with the immigrant experience, is small (Steinberg,
1982; Alba, 1985). But this generation will grow substantially in size during
the rest of this century, and simultaneously, the first and second generations,
which presently constitute the majority of the group, will shrink.
In a number of respects, events among the Italians seem to parallel those
among other groups descended from European immigrants, although because
of differences in their times of arrivals, the specific situations that greeted
them, and their occupational and cultural heritages, no two groups are
following exactly the same pathways to the twilight stage. Yet among virtually
all white ethnic groups, one can observe a progressive, if gradual, dampening
of cultural distinctiveness. Core values have been overwhelmed by a common
American culture so that even though cultural uniformity has not been the
end result, the remaining differences among groups are so mild as to constitute
neither a basis for group solidarity nor a barrier to intergroup contact.
Additionally, among almost all groups, one can see a spreading pattern of
intermarriage, testimony to the minor nature of remaining group differences
and guarantee of additional assimilation (e.g., Alba, 1976). The strength of
this pattern is confirmed by events among Jewish Americans, who provide the
acid test of pervasive intermarriage. Historically, the rate of Jewish-Gentile
intermarriage rate has been quite low, but recent studies have confirmed a
sharp rise in this rate, starting in the 1960s (Cohen, 1983).
Such pervasive intermarriage suggests the emergence of a new ethnic
group, one defined by ancestry from anywhere on the European continent.
This need not mean that ethnic differences within this group will disappear
altogether, but rather that their character is being fundamentally altered.
This appears to be increasingly the case with ethnic identity. As Herbert
Gans (1979) has observed, many mobile ethnics attempt to maintain some
psychological connection with their origins, but in such a way that this
attachment does not prevent them from mixing freely with others of diverse
backgrounds. This contemporary form of ethnicity is private and voluntary,
intermittent and undemanding; it focuses on symbols of ethnic cultures,
rather than the cultures themselves, and tends to be confined to leisure-
time activities. There is a wide latitude available for this 'symbolic ethnicity' —
for Italians, it can range from a liking for pasta to a repudiation of criminal
stereotypes — but the crucial point is that it is the individual who decides
on the appropriate form. Such an ethnic identity is, in other words, a personal
style, and not the manifestation of membership in an ethnic group.
The impending twilight of ethnicity among those of European ancestry is
not matched by equal changes among most of America's non-European
minorities. Black Americans stand as the extreme case. Though their socio-
economic progress in recent years has been debated, no informed observer
claims that they are even close to parity with whites (Farley, 1985). It hardly
needs saying, then, that racial boundaries remain salient. Residentially, blacks
are still extremely segregated from whites, and the incidence of black-white
intermarriage is very small (Heer, 1980).
154 Richard D. Alba

The position of some other minorities is more ambiguous. Some older


non-European groups that were voluntary immigrants to the United States
evidence developments like those among the white ethnics, though these are
not as far along. For example, Japanese Americans, despite the bitter legacy
of World War II internment, have been quite successful in socioeconomic
terms, with high rates of college attendance and occupational mobility. In
tandem with this upward movement have come increases in intermarriage,
frequently with whites (Montero, 1981; Woodrum, 1981). Although in the
future it may become appropriate to speak of an ethnic twilight among
Japanese Americans, the picture for non-European groups is complicated by
the large-scale imfhigration from Asia, the Caribbean, and Latin America
since immigration laws were revised in 1965. Immigrants frohi Colombia,
Cuba, Haiti, Korea, Mexico, the Philippines, Taiwan, Hong Kong, Vietnam,
and still other places are adding new parts to the American ethnic tapestry.
Thus, although twilight may be descending on those ethnic groups whose
forebears came from Europe, ethnicity itself is not subsiding as an issue for
American society. In the future, the salient ethnic outlines may stem from
non-European origins, just as those of European origins have been prominent
in the recent past.

Notes
*A preliminary version of this paper was presented at the 1983 meetings of the American
Sociological Association. I am grateful to Robert K. Merton for his comments and to
Prentice-Hall for permission to use materials from my book.
1. This, of course, coincides with the importance that Gordon (1964) attributes to
'structural assimilation,' that is, large-scale primary relations across ethnic boundaries.
2. Although American immigration authoiities began to keep statistics on 'southern'
and 'northern' Italians in 1899, the racial intent of the distinction distorted the definition
of a 'southern' Italian to include anyone from the 'peninsula proper' (as well as the islands
of Sicily and Sardinia). According to the Bureau of Immigration's definition, 'even
Genoa is South Italian'(U.S. Senate, 1911: 81)! While American statistics were, weakened
in this way, Italian statistics depend largely on applications for the nulla osta, or exit
permit, which required a destination to be stated. But many applicants either did not
subsequently leave or went somewhere other than where they stated (Caroli, 1973:
30; Sori, 1979).
Nonetheless, both sources, though imprecise, are broadly consistent.
3. Jerre Mangione's (1981) memoir of Italian-American life in Rochester paints a very
clear portrait of the sojourner's mentality among his Sicilian relatives.
4. These figures are for the New Yoik, N.Y. Northeastern New Jersey Standard Con-
solidated Area, which in 1970 contained 1.4 million foreign-stock Italian Americans.
The figures are my calculations from Tables 17, 23, and 81 of the Characteristics of the
Population, Parts 32 and 34 (Bureau of the Census, 1973).
5. This survey included the same ethnic ancestry question that appeared in the long
form of the 1980 Census. This question, 'What is . . .'s ancestry?' is superior to questions
asked in previous Current Population Surveys and decennial Censuses, because it does
not constrain answers by a predefined list of responses and hence does not eliminate
the many individuals with mixed ancestry. However, by the same token, it offers a too
inclusive definition of the Italian-American group, since it forces the inclusion of
individuals with any reported degree of Italian ancestry, regardless of its magnitude and
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 155
of the extent of their identification with the Italian group (for a more detailed discussion,
see Alba, 1985).
A virtue of this survey for the study of socioeconomic change is that its large sample
size allows for refined breakdowns.
6. The focus here must be on this ethos, rather than the outward forms of culture,
since these tend to wither away within the first two generations. This is true, for example,
of the everyday-use of Italian. According to the Current Population Survey, over 4 million
claim Italian as a mother tongue, a language spoken in their childhood home, but only
1.4 million (about 12 percent of the group) claim to speak it in their current home
(U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1982: 14). Since the total size of the ancestry group is
around 12 million and that of the first generation, whose members are very likely to
continue to speak their native tongue at home, is 800 thousand, it is clear that only a
small part of the second and third generations continues to use the language on an
everyday basis. For further analysis of external culture, see Crispino (1980).
7. The General Social Surveys offer a narrower definition of the Italian-American
group than does the November, 1979, Current Population Survey. The GSS ask individuals
with mixed ethnic ancestry to identify, if they can, the group to which they feel closer.
This is then reported as their ethnic category.
8. The adjusted difference between the groups reported in the table is the coefficient
for the Italian dummy variable taken from a regression analysis. To achieve stable
estimates of the effects of the control variables, the regression analysis includes all whites;
the comparison to WASPs is effected by making them the omitted category.
9. The value of the anti-abortion scale is the number of times the respondent would
deny a legal abortion in three situations where a prc sumably healthy pregnancy has
resulted from voluntary sexual activity (Davis et al., 1980: 143-4). Such situations are
the litmus test for abortion attitudes, as most Americans would allow an abortion for
such circumstances as a life-endangering pregnancy, or one resulting from rape.
10. The anti-feminism scale is a summative scale compo sed of responses to four questions
such as 'Do you approve or disapprove of a married woman earning money in business
or industry if she has a husband capable of supporting her?' For the wordings of the
other three, see Davis et al (1980: 142).
11. These items are derived from the well-known ones developed by Melvin Kohn and
his colleagues. But there is no pretense here of replicating Kohn's work, since he has
explicitly confined the validity of his scale to parents with children in a certain age
range (Kohn, 1976). Such a limitation is not feasible here.
The scale I report is calculated by counting a +1 for each time a respondent rated
as desirable a trait associated with self-direction and also each time he or she rated as
undesirable a trait associated with conformity, and counting a - 1 when the reverses
occurred. Positive numbers on the scale thus indicate- a valence toward self-direction.
12. Since simultaneous controls for both current and original location amount to
controls for mobility itself, one has to be removed from the list of independent variables
for this analysis; current location (both region and size of place) has been deleted.
13. This does not imply that Italians and WASPs are similar in all ways. For one, they
differ in their political party allegiances, with Italians notably more tied to the Demo-
cratic Party. But the crucial point is that they are similar on many traits bearing on the
family-centered ethos. (For more details and discussion, see Alba, 1985).
14. The 1980 Census yields a somewhat lower estimate of the percentage of Italians with
mixed ancestry, 43.5 (versus 48.0), and presumably will show lower rates of mixed
ancestry in younger cohorts when tables of ancestry byage become available. Nevertheless,
there appears to be good reason to give greater credence to the CPS rather than the
decennial census in this case. The markedly lower estimates of mixed ancestry in general
in the census suggest that ancestry responses were more cursory to the census's mail
survey than to the face-to-face interviewing of the CPS (for further discussion of the
differences between the two, see Bureau of the Census, 1983: 4-5).
156 Richard D. Alba
References

ALBA, RICHARD 1976 'Social assimilation among American Catholic national-


origin groups,' American Sociological Review 41 (December): 1030-46.
ALBA, RICHARD 1981 "The twilight of ethnicity among American Catholics of
European ancestry.' The Annals 454 (March): 86-97.
ALBA, RICHARD 1985 Italian Americans: Into the Twilight of Ethnicity. Englewood
Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.
ALBA, RICHARD, and MITCHELL CHAMLIN 1983. 'A preliminary examination of
ethnic identification among whites.' American Sociological Review 48 (April): 240-7.
ALBA, RICHARD, and REID GOLDEN 1984 'Patterns of ethnic marriage in the United
States.' Paper presented at the annual meetings of the American Sociological Association.
ALBA, RICHARD, and RONALD KESSLER 1979 'Patterns of interethnic marriage
among American Catholics.' Social Forces 57 (June): 1124-40.
BANFIELD, ROBERT 1958 The Moral Basis of a Backward Society. New York: The
Free Press.
BARTH, FREDRIK 1969 'Introduction,' pp. 9-38 in Fredrik Barth (ed.), Ethnic Groups
and Boundaries. Boston: Little, Brown.
BERRY, BRIAN, and JOHN KASARDA 1977 Contemporary Urban Ecology. New
York: Macmillan.
BLAU, PETER 1977 Inequality and Heterogeneity: A Primitive Theory of Social
Structure. New York: The Free Press.
BLAUNER, ROBERT 1972 Racial Oppression in America. New York: Harper & Row.
BLUM, JOHN MORTON 1976 V Was for Victory: Politics and American Culture During
World War II. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
BRETON, RAYMOND 1964 'Institutional completeness of ethnic communities and the
personal relations of immigrants.' American Journal of Sociology 70 (July): 193-205.
BUREAU OF THE CENSUS 1973 1970 Census of the Population, Volume I, Character-
istics of the Population. Washington: U.S. Government Printing Office.
BUREAU OF THE CENSUS 1975 Historical Statistics of the United States, Colonial
Times to 1970. Bicentennial edition, Part 1. Washington: U.S. Government Printing
Office.
BUREAU OF THE CENSUS 1982 'Ancestry and language in the United States:
November 1979.' Current Population Reports, Special Studies, Series P-23, No. 116.
Washington: U.S. Government Printing Office.
BUREAU OF THE CENSUS 1983 1980 Census of the Population, Ancestry of the
Population by State: 1980, Supplementary Report. Washington: U.S. Government
Printing Office.
CAROLI, BETTY BOYD 1973 Italian Repatriation from the United States 1900-1914.
Staten Island: Center for Migration Studies.
CHAPMAN, CHARLOTTE GOWER 1971 Milocca: A Sicilian Village. Cambridge,
Mass.: Schenkman.
CHILD, IRVIN 1943 Italian or American? Second Generation in Conflict. New Haven:
Yale University Press.
COHEN, STEVEN 1983 American Modernity and Jewish Identity. New York and
London: Tavistock.
COVELLO, LEONARD 1972 The Social Background of the halo-American School
Child. Totowa: Rowman & Littlefield.
CRAIGIE, WILLIAM, and JAMES HULBERT 1940 A Dictionary of American English
on Historical Principles, Vol. II. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
CRISPINO, JAMES 1980 The Assimilation of Ethnic Groups: The Italian Case. Staten
Island: Center for Migration Studies.
DAVIS, JAMES, TOM SMITH, and C. BRUCE STEPHENSON 1980 General Social
Surveys, 1972-1980: Cumulative Codebook. Chicago: NORC.
FARLEY, REYNOLDS 1985 'Recent changes in the social and economic status of
Ethnicity among Americans of European ancestry 157
blacks: three steps forward and two back?' Ethnic and Racial Studies (January).
FEMMINELLA, FRANK; and JILL QUADAGNO 1976 'The Italian American family,'
in Charles Mindell and Robert Habenstein (eds.), Ethnic Families in America: Patterns
and Variations. New York: Elsevier.
FOERSTER, ROBERT 1924 The Italian Emigration of Our Times. Cambridge, Mass.:
Harvard University Press.
GAMBINO, RICHARD 1974 Blood of My Blood. Garden City, N.Y.: Doubleday.
GANS, HERBERT 1962 The Urban Villagers: Group and Class in the Life of Italian-
Americans. New York: The Free Press.
GANS, HERBERT 1979 'Symbolic ethnicity: The future of ethnic groups and cultures
in America.' Ethnic and Racial Studies 2 (January): 1-20.
GLAZER, NATHAN, and DANIEL MOYNIHAN 1970 Beyond the Melting Pot. Rev.
ed. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.
GORDON, MILTON 1964 Assimilation in American Life. New York: Oxford University
Press.
GREELEY, ANDREW 1971 Why Can't They Be Like Us? New York: Dutton.
GREELEY, ANDREW 1974 Ethnicity in the United State::: A Preliminary Reconnaisance.
New York: Wiley.
GREELEY, ANDREW 1977 The American Catholic: A Social Portrait. New York:.
Basic Books.
GREELEY, ANDREW, and WILLIAM McCREADY 197.5 "The transmission of cultural
heritages: The case of the Irish and the Italians,' pp. 209-35 in Nathan Glazer and
Daniel Moynihan (eds.), Ethnicity: Theory and Experience. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard
University Press.
HECHTER, MICHAEL 1978 'Group formation and the cultural division of labor.'
American Journal of Sociology 84 (September): 293-318.
HEER, DAVID 1980 'Intermarriage,' in Stephan Thernsirom, Ann Orlov, and Oscar
Handlin (eds.), Harvard Encyclopedia of American Ethnic Groups. Cambridge, Mass.:
Harvard University Press.
HIGHAM, JOHN 1970 Strangers in the Land: Patterns of American Nativism 1860-
1925. New York: Atheneum.
JOHNSON, COLLEEN LEAHY 1982 'Sibling solidarity: its origin and functioning in
Italian-American families.' Journal of Marriage and the Family (February): 155-67.
KESSNER, THOMAS 1977 The Golden Door: Italian ar.d Jewish Immigrant Mobility
in New York City 1880-1915. New York: Oxford Univsrsity Press.
KOHN, MELVIN 1976 'Social class and parental values: another confirmation of the
relationship.' American Sociological Review 41 (June): .'¡38-45.
LIEBERSON, STANLEY 1963 Ethnic Patterns in American Cities. New York: The Free
Press.
LIEBERSON, STANLEY 1980 A Piece of the Pie: Blacks and White Immigrants since
1880. Berkeley: University of California Press.
MANGIONE, JERRE 1981 Mount Allegro: A Memoir of Italian American Life. New
York: Columbia University Press.
MENCKEN, H.L. 1963 The American Language. Abridg. ed. New York: Knopf.
MERTON, ROBERT 1941 'Intermarriage and the social structure: fact and theory.'
Psychiatry 4 (August): 361-74.
MILLER, HERMAN P. 1971 RichMan, Poor Man. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell.
MONTERO, DARREL 1981 "The Japanese Americans: Changing patterns of assimilation
over three generations.' American Sociological Review 46 (December): 829-39.
NEWMAN, WILLIAM 1973 American Pluralism: A Study of Minority Groups and Social
Theory. New York: Harper & Row.
NOVAK, MICHAEL 1972 The Rise of the Unmeltable Ethnics. New York: Macmillan.
PANUNZIO, CONSTANTINE 1928 The Soul of an Immigrant. New York.
POLENBERG, RICHARD 1980 One Nation Divisible: Class, Race, and Ethnicity in
the United States. New York: Viking.
158 Richard D. Alba
ROSEN, BERNARD 1959 'Race, ethnicity, and the achievement syndrome.' American
Sociological Review 24 (February): 47-60.
SCHNEIDER, JANE, and PETER SCHNEIDER 1976 Culture and Political Economy in
Western Sicily. New York: Academic Press.
SCHOOLER, CARMI 1976 'Serfdom's legacy: An ethnic continuum.' American Journal
of Sociology 81 (May): 1265-86.
SORI, ERCOLE 1979 L'emigrazione italiana dall' Unita alla seconda guerra mondiale.
Bologna: II Mulino.
ST ANBACK, THOMAS, and RICHARD KNIGHT 1970 The Metropolitan Economy.
New York: Columbia University Press.
STEINBERG, STEPHEN 1982 The Ethnic Myth: Race, Ethnicity, and Class in America.
Boston: Beacon Press.
TROW, MARTIN 1961 'The second transformation of American secondary education.'
Internationaljournal of Comparative Sociology 2: 144-66.
U.S. SENATE 1911 Reports of the Immigration Commission: Dictionary of Races or
Peoples. Washington: Government Printing Office.
VECOLI, RUDOLPH J. 1978 'The coming of age of the Italian Americans.' Ethnicity
5 (June).
WARE, CAROLINE 1935 Greenwich Village, 1920-1930. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
WHYTE, WILLIAM FOOTE 1955 Street Comer Society. Sec. ed. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
WOODRUM, ERIC 1981 'An assessment of Japanese American assimilation, pluralism,
and subordination.' American Journal of Sociology 87 (July): 157-69.
YANCEY, WILLIAM, EUGENE ERICKSEN, and RICHARD JULIANI 1976 'Emergent
ethnicity: a review and a reformulation.' American Sociological Review 41 (June):
391-403.
YANS-MCLAUGHLIN, VIRGINIA 1977 Family and Community: Italian Immigrants in
Buffalo 1880-1930. Urbana: University of Illinois.

You might also like