Aks1998 Excercise Visual Search
Aks1998 Excercise Visual Search
DEBORAH J . AKS
Ut~iuerxityof Wiscorzsi~z-Wblirwater
Thus, in these large set-size trials, the test may have tapped
--
into effects of
exercise on grouping rather than attention. An analysis comparing one and
five-item trials shows the greatest exercise effects in the one-item trials. Re-
moving these methodological limitations and improving upon other aspects
of the design (described later in the method section) may yield reliable
exercise effects on visual-search slopes.
One additional difference in design that may be instrumental in uncov-
ering the influence of exercise on attention is the use of a feature and con-
junction task of intermediate difficulty in which neither is subject to a floor
or ceiling effect. Thus, we selected to study a challenging feature task with
low target-distractor discriminability, and a conjunction search task of inter-
mediate difficulty that could be facilitated through dlstractor grouping of
the same colored items (i.e., Egeth, Virzi, & Garbart, 1984). Therefore, the
goal of the present experiment was to extend the assessment of the effect of
exercise on visual search, while using distinct exercise conditions and novel
search tasks that optimize our abdity to detect exercise influences on atten-
tion. We expect that search w d become more efficient following exercise
and that response times and slopes will decrease as attention is distributed
over the items present in the &splay.
METHOD
Subjects
Eighteen members of the university community (10 female, age range =
19-3 1 years; M = 22, SD = 3 1 participated in two sessions. None had previous
experience with visual search tasks, and all reported normal or corrected-to-
normal vision. A health survey, administered prior to participation, indicated
that all subjects were healthy, had no preexisting heart conditions, rated
themselves as average or better than average in fitness, and regularly exer-
cised between 3 and 12 hours per week. This study had University of Wis-
consin Institutional Review Board approval on the use of human subjects.
S&zmuliand Apparatus
Exercise conditions were performed on a cycle ergometer (Monark
Model 818). Display presentation and data collection were controlled by a
Macintosh computer running VSearch software (Enns, Ochs, & Rensink,
1990). The feature task consisted of 2, 6, or 10 red items with a large circu-
lar target (7 mm) and small circular dstractors (6 rnm) that were identical
except for their l-mm difference in size. In the conjunction task, targets
were distinguished from distractors on the basis of both size and color.
Items consisted of the large, red 7-mm targets and any of the following three
dstractors: small and blue, small and red, or large and blue.
Procedure
Subjects performed feature and conjunction search tasks before and af-
EXERCISE AND VISUAL SEARCH IN COGNITION 775
ter ten minutes of submaximal exercise on the cycle ergometer. Each trial
began with a central fixation symbol (black dot) lit for 720 msec., followed
by the &splay, which remained visible until the subject responded or 2.9
sec. had elapsed. On each of the search tasks, subjects made speeded biman-
ual responses to indicate whether the target was present in the &splay. On a
random half of the displays the target was present, with the total number of
items varying randomly among 2, 6, and 10. Following the response, visual
feedback was presented at the center of the screen ("+" for correct, "-" for
incorrect). Subjects were instructed to maintain fixation throughout the trial
sequence, to respond as rapidly as possible, and to keep errors below 10%.
Exercise exertion was manipulated so that the low-exertion condition
consisted primarily of aerobic processes, and high-exertion consisted prima-
rily of anaerobic processes. The dstinction between these two forms of exer-
cise is far from absolute and typically defined in terms of differing energy
sources. Ln brief, energy for aerobic metabolism where oxygen is readily
available is derived from glycogen and fat. Energy for aerobic metabolism
wherein oxygen is not r e a d y available and must instead be generated by the
muscles is derived from the phosphate and the lactate system.' For compre-
hensive descriptions of these processes see McArdel, Katch, and Katch (1996)
and Plowman and Smith (1997).
In the low-exertion exercise condition, subjects cycled at a steady pace
of 50 revolutions per minute in synchrony with a metronome set at 100
beats per minute; thus one revolution of cycling corresponded to 2 beats on
the metronome. Women exerted 93 Watts of power over the 10-min. exer-
cise period, and men exerted 147 Watts. In the high-exertion condition, the
first 8 min. were the same as in the low-exertion condition. For the last 2
min., however, the work load was increased by a factor of two so women
now exerted 186 Watts, and men exerted 294 Watts. Levels of exertion were
maintained over the designated (8- and 2-rnin.) time periods by having an
experimenter continuously monitor and provide feedback to subjects to syn-
chronize cycling with the metronome. After each exercise condition, the sub-
ject returned to the computer to perform a second feature and conjunction
visual-search task. Changes in response time as a function of Set Size were
used to assess the efficiency of search and changes in attentional focus.
Subjects were tested in two 40-min. sessions. The low- and high-exer-
'Although it is not clear how changes in glycogen, lactate, and phosphate levels relate to cogni-
tive funcrion, it is reasonable to expect moderare increases in oxy en transport and metabolic
rate to be associated with speeded and perhaps parallel visual searcl since such exercise condi-
tions have sbo\lv large increases in cerebral blood flow (e.g., Jorgensen, Perko, & Secher,
1992), nerve rr.rnsn~issionspeed (Astrand & Rod.~hl,1977), and reduced response rime (Travlos
& Marisjsi. 1996) O n the other hand, the o\ygc.n deficit associated with anaerobic exercise
may lead to rhc opposite effect of slowed cognlrl!c performance (F. D. Aks, personal commu-
nication, A u g ~ ~20,
s r 1997).
776 D. J. AKS
tion sessions were run at the same time on separate days within a 2-wk. pe-
riod. In both sessions, subjects practiced p e d a h g to the metronome for 30
sec. Immediately aftenvards, subjects rested for 2.5 min. while completing a
brief health questionnaire containing questions about their exercise habits
and perceived level of fitness. Following the rest period, subjects partici-
pated in the Feature and Conjunction search tasks with each consisting of
three sets of 40 trials. Order of exercise condtions, search tasks and hand of
response (left vs right) were counterbalanced across subjects.
Statirtical Procedure
A repeated, 5-way analysis of variance was conducted on five factors,
type of exercise (low vs high exertion) x search test (pre vs post) x task (fea-
ture vs conjunction) x target (present vs absent) x set size (2, 6, 10). An al-
pha level of .05 was used for all statistical procedures. Estimated power
prior to the experiment was .51.
Table 1 shows subjects were both faster and more accurate on visual-
search performance after exercise.' Fig. 1 further shows mean correct re-
sponse time and % errors across all five factors noted above. The fastest
search on trials with the fewest items resulted in steeper slopes following ex-
ercise as is shown in Table 2. The greatest increases in slopes occurred on
the target-absent trials following low-exertion exercise, and on conjunction
target-present trials following low- and high-exertion exercise.
TABLE 1
OVER-ALL EFFECTSO F EXERCISEON VISUALSEARCHPERFORMANCE BEFOREAND
A F ~ E REXERCISE:
MEANS A N D STAND~RD
DEVIATIONSFOR RESPONSE
TIMES(MSEC.).SLOPES(MSEC./JTEM),
A N D % ERRORS
'Two of the 18 subjects participated only in the high-exertion exercise conditions. Thus, in the
combined analysis, the coral number of subjects was 16. Also, missing dare were observed in
0.13% of the cells in this design (representing one subjecr on 3 of 48 conditions). These cells
were filled b the grou mean for statistical analyses, but in no case was the reported signifi-
cance level a&ecred by ttis procedure.
EXERCISE AND VISUAL SEARCH IN COGNITION
-2
0 0
1200
LOW ~xertion 1 Feature Search
a;
Conjunction Search 16
FIG. 1. Mean correct response time and % Error for Feature (1A) and Conjuncrion (1B)
search trials before and after low- and high-exertion exercise. Error bars indicate SEM. Faster
search after exercise was significant in nwo- ( ~ , ~ = 6 .p<.001)
7, and six-item (t,>=5.0,p<.001)
(pre-post search x set size: F,,,,=7.5, p < ,01). Errors were fairly consistent across set
size (pre-post search x set size: F,,,,=0.9, ns). Exercise: pre ( ) and post ( )
778 D. J. AKS
between target presence and set size reflect the typical 2:1 ratio in slopes be-
tween target-absent ( M = 57 msec./item) and target-present trials (M = 29
msec./item; F,,,, = 106, p < .001)-a trend representative of a serial self-termi-
nating search.
TABLE 2
MFANCORRECT
SLOPES A N D STANDARD D ~ ~ O N S
(MSEC./ITEM)
SearchExertion Target
Present /I, Absent '15
Pretest Posttest Pretest Posttest
Feature
Low M 33.5 34.8 0.3 55.8 67.9" 2.1"
SD 16.8 17.6 32.8 39.6
High IM 30.8 35.2 1.0 61.1 62.9 0.2
SD 14.8 17.6 28.8 30.4
Conjunction
Low M 23.9 31.5" 2.07 43.1 57.1" 5.2*
SD 12.0 14.0 13.6 14.0
High M 19.3 29.5" 2.2$ 59.1 57.5 0.4$
SD 12.4 11.6 23.2 18.8
*p< .05. t p < .O6. t d f = 17. Pre-postsearch: F,,15=9.6, p < .01. Type of exercise x pre-post-
search x target: F1,,,=5.4,pc.05. Errors: pre.postsearch x set size: FI,,,=0.9, ns).
There were also three unexpected findings in the data set.4 The most
important was the stronger high-exertion exercise effects on target-present
trials containing either two (t,,=4.0, p < ,001) or six items (t,,=4.7: p < ,001).
This 4-way interaction also reflects the reversed effect on 10 item target-ab-
sent trials following low-exertion exercise (t,, = 2.8, p < .01). Analyses based
on simple effects of Exercise Type (low-exertion vs high-exertion) and
Search Task (feature vs conjunction) support these trends. One final analysis
presented below further confirms these trends, ensures the four-way interac-
'Subjects were substantially slower and slightly less accurate in detecting the Feature than the
,,,,
Conjunction target RT: F =30.0, pc.001; Slope: F I , 1 5 = 17.3, p<.01; Error: FI,IS=4.2, p=.06.
Faster and more accurate search for the Conjunction search task is likely due to groupin of
the same colored items and inhibition of the irrelevant color feature (i.e., Egeth, Virzi, & t a r -
barc, 1984). This interpretation is corroborated by the compressed effects in rhe 10-item targer-
present pretesr (baseline) condition. There was also some variation across low- and high-exer-
tion prerest b a s e h e target-absent conditions with slower performance in high-exertion trials
(MdiB=67msec./item, f 5=2.4, pc.05). However, this difference wis not significant as a main
effect (F, ,,=2.4, ns). \jarlability within individuals can be accounted for b the fact that low-
and high-exertion exercise were performed on separate days. Nevertheless, tKe critical compari-
son between y,n- and. postexercise conditions w m run on the same day; rhus, rhese were not
subject to suc var~abll~ty. One final analysis examined the effect o l sex which was a nonsignifi-
cant effect (F,,,=0.5, ns): yet h e r e was a significant 4 - w interaction
~ among sex xerercise
t pe x re vs postexercise x set size (F2,,,,=3.6, p < ,051, re ectlng rhe tendency lor women to
SLOW ;st;r RTs from high-exertion exerclse on 6- and 10-irern trials. This interaction should
be interpreted cautiously as additional confounding variables such as subjects' physical firness
were not balanced across all conditions.
EXERCISE AND VISUAL SEARCH IN COGNITION 779
tion is not spurious and by collapsing across exercise conditions has the
benefit of reducing the number of factors in the analysis.
The Exercise Effect: Difference Scores
Differences between exercise and no exercise conditions shown in Fig.
2 were calculated to isolate and compare further effects of low- vs high-exer-
tion exercise. A positive score indicates faster search from exercise; a nega-
tive score indicates slowed search; and a zero score indicates no effect of ex-
ercise. High-exenion exercise poduced consistently high scores (Merr=60
msec.) that significantly deviated from a zero basehe (t,,,=4.0, p<.001).
The weakest effects occurred on target present ten-item conditions (t,, = 0.0,
Set Size 2
Set Size 6
Set Size 10
Feature
FIG. 2. Difference in response time before and after exercise. Error bars indicate SEM.
Significance of univariate f tests: ' p < .06, * p < .05, **p< .01.
780 D.J. AKS
ment lasted only 10 minutes, it is likely that they quahfy as intermediate ex-
ercise intensities. The relatively small work load difference between the low-
and high-exertion conditions (20% dfiference for 2 of 10 min.), did show
subtle influences on performance. The most pronounced difference is that
high-exertion exercise ~ r o d u c e dfaster and more reliable search. Low-exer-
tion exercise, on the other hand, while still speeding search (albeit less in
magnitude), produced steeper slopes indicative of a narrower attentional fo-
cus.
The few existing studes assessing the effects of exercise on attention
have reported conflicting effects (e.g., Allard, et a/., 1989; Cote, Salmela, &
Papathanasoupou, 1992 vs Salmela & Ndoye, 1986). Ln the present experi-
ment, we were surprised to find slopes increased following both forms of
exercise. One interpretation of the steeper slopes is that attention may have
become less efficient following exercise (i.e., Ashby & Townsend, 1986;
Wolfe, 1997, 1998), perhaps to provide a more careful sequential examina-
tion of indvidual items. One problem with this interpretation is that a less
efficient search strategy seems to contradict the findmg that absolute re-
sponse times were faster after exercise.
Alternatively, improved performance under two-item conditions may be
the result of either an exogenous or endogenous attentional mechanism (e.g.,
Easterbrook, 1959; Nideffer, 1979; Treisman & Gelade, 1980; Treisman,
1992). In exogenous attention, stimuli drive the distribution of attention. In
this view, exercise may only improve efficiency when attention is already
narrowly focused. This may occur when a subject detects a target in a local
region of the display as in the case of trials with a small number of items
(LaBerge & Brown, 1989). Two-item trials, since they most frequently clus-
ter together in a small region of the display, possess the local stimulus
property needed to narrow attention. Analysis of search performance before
and after exercise supports this theory by showing that exercise helps per-
formance on trials containing the fewest number of items. As mentioned ear-
lier, a careful analysis of Allard, et al.'s (1989) work shows a similar trend-
performance was better following exercise on single-item trials than five-item
trials.
A second interpretation of performance gains in two-item conditions is
that exercise may directly influence attention (e.g., Easterbrook, 1959). Ac-
cordmg to this endogenous view of attention, internal cognitive factors drive
or modulate the distribution of attention. Exercise may narrow attention re-
sulting in further performance gains in small set-size trials. A distinguishing
characteristic of an endogenous process is that impaired performance is ex-
pected when stirnd are more widely dstributed across the display as occurs
in the ten-item conditions.
In addition to finding faster search when there are fewer items in the
782 D. J. AKS
display, the data from high-exertion trials is most consistent with the exoge-
nous interpretation since performance was not impaired in multiple-item con-
ditions following exercise. On the other hand, the data from low-exertion
trials is most consistent with the endogenous interpretation since perform-
ance is both improved when fewer items are present, and more importantly,
impaired as more items are present in the &splay. This cross-over interac-
tion involving exercise and set-size supports the possibility of two mecha-
nisms, each being activated by varying intensities of exercise. Nevertheless,
ddferences between the two exercise (and search type) conditions appear to
be subtle and require additional testing to v e r b unique effects.
On the whole, this study dustrates that ten minutes of exercise results
in faster response times, fewer errors and steeper slopes. These trends sug-
gest that ten minutes of moderate exercise speeds us up and makes us more
accurate when we are engaged in a search task. This gain in performance is
especially helpful in improving cognitive processing within focused attention.
REFERENCES
ALIARD,F., BRAWLEY, L., DEAKIN, I., &ELLIOT,F. (1989) The effect of exercise on visual atten-
tion performance. Human Performance, 2, 131-145.
ASHBY.F. G., & TOWNSEND, J. T. (1986) Varieties of perceptual independence. Psychological
Review, 93, 154.179.
RAND, F! O., &RODAHL. K. (1977) Test of working physiology: physiological bases of exercise.
New York: McGraw-Hill.
COTE.1.. SALMEIA, J., & PAPATHANASOUPOU, K. I? (1992) Effects of progressive exercise on ar-
rentional focus. Perceptzral and Motor Skills, 75, 351-354.
DAVEY,C. l? (1973) Physical exertion and mental performance. Ergoizoinics, 16, 595-599.
EASTERBROOK. J. A. (1959) The effects OF emotion on cue utilization and the organization of
behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183-281.
EGETH,H., VIRZI,R.. &GARBART. H. (1984) Searching for conjuncrively d e f i e d rargets. ]orrnzal
of Experierztal Pxychology: Hzlmarz Perceptzot~O Perfonnonce, 10, 32-39.
ENNS.J. 1. OCHS,E. F!, &&NSINK,R. A. (1990) VSearch: Macintosh sofnvare for experiments
in visual search. Behavior Research Methods, lnsfru?nenfs& Compzrters, 22. 1 18- 122.
ETNIER,J. L., SALAZAR. W., LANDERS, D. M., PETRUZZELLO, S. I., HAN, M.. & NOWELL. l? (1997)
The influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive Functioning. 4 rneta-analysis.
Jozrnral of Spori G Exercise Psychology, 19, 249-277.
GUTIN,B. (1973) Exercise-induced activation and human performance: a review. Review Quar-
terly, 44, 256-268.
HANCOCK, S., & MCNAUGHTON. L. (1986) Effects of Fatigue on abhty to process visual infor-
mation by experienced orienteers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 491 -498
HECKLER, B., &GROCE,R. (1992) Effects of time of posttesr after two durdr~onsOF exercise on
speed and accuracy OF addirion and subrraction by fit and less-fit women. Percepttral and
motor Skills, 75, 1059-1065.
HOGERVORST, E., RIEDEL,W., JEUKENDRUP, A., & JOLLES,J. (1996) Cognitive performance after
strenuous physical exercise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83, 479-488.
HUMPHREYS, G . W., QUINIAN,I? T., & RIDDOCH,M. 1. (1989) Grouping processes in visual
search: effects with single and combined-feature targets. Jounlaf ofExperimenta1 Psycholo-
gy: General, 118, 258-279.
JORGENSEN, L. G., PERKO.M., &SECHER, N. H. (1992) Regional cerebral artery mean flow ve-
locity and blood flow during dynamic exercise in humans. ]ourizal of Applied Physiology.
73, 1825-1830.
EXERCISE AND VISUAL SEARCH IN COGNITION 7 83
KUBITZ, K. A., &POTHAKOS, K. (1997) Does aerobic exercise decrease brain activation. Journal
of Sport & Exernie Psychology, 19, 291-301.
LABERCE, D., &BROWN,V. (1989) Theory of attentional operations in shape idendication. Psy-
chological Review, 96, 101-124.
LEVITK,S., &GUTIN,B. (1971) Multiple choice reaction time and nlovement tinie during physi-
cal exertion. Research Quarterly. 42, 405-411.
MCARDEL,W. D., KATCH,E I., & KATCH.V. L. (1996) Exercise Physlblogy (4th ed.) Baltimore,
MD: Wdhams & Wilkins.
MEYERS, C. R, ZIMMERLI, W., FARR,S. D.. & BASCHNAGEL, N. A. (1969) Effect of strenuous
physical activity upon reaction time. Research Quarterly, 40, 332-337.
NCISSER, U. (1967) Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
NIDEFFER, R. M. (1976) Test of Attentional and hterpersonal Style. Journal of Persotzaliiy and
social Psychofogy, 34, 3 94-404.
PASHLER, H. (1987) Detectin con'unctions of color and form: reassessing the serial search hy-
pothesis. Perception ond:~sycLo~hysics, 41, 191-201.
PLOWMAN, S. A,, &SMITH.S. L. (1997) Exercise physiology for health, fitrzess, and perforronce.
Needham, MA: Prent~ceHall.
REILLY, T., &SM[TH,D. (ITS&) Effect of work intensity on performance in a psychomotor task
during exercise. Ergorzo~nics,29, 601-606.
SALMELA, J . H., &NDOYE,0. D. (1986) Cognitive distoaions during progressive exercise. Per-
ceptual and motor Skills, 63, 1067-1072.
SCHMIDT, R. A. (1982) Motor control and learning: a behavioral emwphosis. C h a r n p ~ ~ p IL:
1 . Hu-
man Kineucs.
SIOEERC, H. (1980) Physical fitness and mental performance during and after work. Ergonom-
ics, 23, 977-985.
THAYER, R. E. (1978) Toward a psychological theory of multidimensional acuvation (arousal).
Motivation and Emotion, 2, 1-34.
TOMPOROWSKI. I? D.. &ELLIS,N.R. (1986) Effects of exercise on cognitive processes: a review.
Psychological Btrlletin, 99, 338-346.
TOWNSEND, J. T. (1971) A note on the idendication of parallel and serial processes. Perception
and Psychophysics. 10, 161-163.
TOWNSEND, J. T (1976) Serial and within-sta e independent XaraUel model equivalence on the
minimum completion time. Jot~rnolof dthernatical Pryc ology, 14, 219-239.
TRAVI.OS. A K., & MARLSI, D. Q. (1995) Information processin and concentration as a func-
[Ion of fitness level and exercise-induced activation to exaaustion. Perceptual and Motor
Skrll\. 80, 15-26.
TREISMAN, A (1992) Spreading su pression or feature integration? A reply to Duncan and
Humphreys (1992). Journal o f ~ x ~ e r i m e n t aPsychology:
l Human Perception and Perform-
ance, 18, 589-593.
TREISMAN. A,, &GELADE, G. (1980) A fearure-incegrauon theory of attention. Cognifiue Prychol-
0 0 , 12, 97-136.
WEINCARTEN. G., &ALEXANDER, J . F. (1970) Effects of physical exertion on mental performance
of college males of different physical fitness levels. Perceptual and Motor Skifls, 31, 371-
378.
WOLFE,J . M. (1997) Visual search. In H. Pashler (Ed.), A!ferrtion. London, UK: Univer. Col-
lege London Press.
WOLFE,1. M. (1998) What can one million trials tell us about visual search? Psychological Sci-
ence, 9, 33-39.
YERKES. R., & DODSON, . D. (1908) The relation of stren rh of stimulus rapidity of habit for-
mation. ]ot,rml o)CotBparafivc Neurology and ~ s ~ i b i i f &18.
v . 459-482.