NeurocognitiveDance - ACTA Psychologica 2012
NeurocognitiveDance - ACTA Psychologica 2012
Accepted Version
It is advisable to refer to the publisher’s version if you intend to cite from the
work. See Guidance on citing .
Publisher: Elsevier
www.reading.ac.uk/centaur
CentAUR
Central Archive at the University of Reading
Reading’s research outputs online
Neurocognitive control in dance 1
1
Department of Sport Science and Center of Excellence Cognitive Interaction Technology
Australia
Abstract
Dance is a rich source of material for researchers interested in the integration of movement
and cognition. The multiple aspects of embodied cognition involved in performing and
perceiving dance have inspired scientists to use dance as a means for studying motor control,
expertise, and action-perception links. The aim of this review is to present basic research on
cognitive and neural processes implicated in the execution, expression, and observation of
dance, and to bring into relief contemporary issues and open research questions. The review
addresses six topics: 1) dancers’ exemplary motor control, in terms of postural control,
synchronization are influenced by attention demands and motor experience; 3) the critical
roles played by sequence learning and memory; 4) how dancers make strategic use of visual
and motor imagery; 5) the insights into the neural coupling between action and perception
yielded through exploration of the brain architecture mediating dance observation; and 6) a
neuroaesthetics perspective that sheds new light on the way audiences perceive and evaluate
dance expression. Current and emerging issues are presented regarding future directions that
Keywords
synchronization, aesthetics.
Neurocognitive control in dance 3
Highlights
1. Dance is a valuable source for studying the integration of movement and cognition.
2. Dance expertise involves motor control, memory for movement, strategic use of
Dance is a universal form of human expression that has been cultivated into various forms
and functions (Hanna, 1979). Its origin is intrinsically linked to social interactions (Brown et
al., 2005), and it has been thought to exist in similar forms universally across cultures (Grahn
& McAuley, 2009; Phillips-Silver et al. 2011). With the evolution of human societies, the
characteristics of dance and dancers have changed over time (Daprati et al., 2009), but
typically, dance is associated with one or multiple bodies moving in a specified rhythmical
manner with or without music. Dance expertise can be acquired to different degrees of
limb coordination, flexibility, and strength, as well as other performative and aesthetic
elements that are more subjectively determined. These latter components are the ones that
make the dancer distinguishable from other motor experts such as athletes or martial artists
(Yarrow et al., 2009), and they are crucial for psychology and neuroscience studies that aim to
investigate the integration of physical virtuosity with aesthetic, affective, communicative and
social elements. Both the physical and the artistic demands of dance require manifold
cognitive abilities that can be studied using behavioral and neuroscientific methods (Bläsing,
Dancers, for example, must learn complex movement sequences by efficiently reproducing
movements they observe, which can include the transfer of visual and verbal information into
motor action. Dancers modify movements with respect to direction in space, speed, rhythm,
and amplitude, and express them precisely as observed from the choreographer’s
choreographer and/or the dance style. When dancing in an ensemble, dancers must remain
Neurocognitive control in dance 5
attentive to their fellow dancers in order to synchronize their movement. Thus, dancers are
demanding the integration of physical and cognitive skills. Dancers’ expertise in several
This review examines a number of basic research findings in those areas of dance that can
contribute to our understanding of the complex processes required to coordinate the brain and
on cognitive and neural processes implicated in the generation, execution, expression, and
observation of dance movements by the dancer and dance spectator. Further, we bring into
relief contemporary issues and open research questions in this domain. Along these lines, we
ask in what way the study of dance contributes to advancements in knowledge of human
cognition and behavior. We examine the scientific validity of using dance-related stimuli and
dancers’ expertise in order to investigate the nature of the various processes involved in such
highly sophisticated motor and cognitive skills. In the spirit of “artscience” (Edwards, 2008)
the review may also prompt questions from dance that provoke new ideas and approaches in
From “toe to head”, we begin by considering processes of motor control, specifically how
dancers maintain balance (equilibrium) in difficult postures, such as in pointe work for female
ballet dancers, or in pirouette turns on one leg. The way dancers synchronize their movements
with a dance partner, dance ensemble, or a musical beat, is then reviewed. Next, we consider
the strategic use of dance sequences as stimuli to shed light on memory encoding, followed by
research into visual and motor imagery for dance and in dancers. The close coupling of
perception and action in observing and in performing dance is then discussed with particular
Neurocognitive control in dance 6
attention given to the neural substrates involved in these processes. Finally, research from the
nascent field of dance neuroaesthetics is introduced, with attention focused on what can be
learned from the relationship between the dance creator and the dance spectator.
1. Motor Control
The increased demand for verticality of ballet postures (Daprati, Iosa, & Haggard, 2009) puts
strenuous requirements on female ballet dancers’ flexibility, strength, and balance. One may
thus assume that dancers show enhanced abilities in posture control and equilibrium
maintenance as a vital part of their expertise. A number of studies have investigated the
cognitive control functions that underlie dancers’ exceptional physical skill, with a focus on
the learning and maintenance of these control functions, and how they may affect other
sensorimotor processes.
Dance training enhances sensorimotor control functions underlying static as well as dynamic
equilibrium. For example, classically-trained dancers exhibit better postural control (Rein,
Fabian, Zwipp, Rammelt, & Weindel, 2011), can maintain given postures for longer durations
(Crotts, Thompson, Nahom, Ryan, & Newton, 1996), and show more vertical alignment
during stepping than non-dancers (Chatfield, Krasnow, Herman, & Blessing, 2007). Several
studies revealed better balance skills in dancers compared to non-dancers (Golomer, Dupui, &
Monod, 1997a,b; Golomer, Cremieux, Dupui, Isableu, & Ohlmann 1999a), in adult dancers
compared to younger and less experienced dancers (Bruyneel, Mesure, Paré, & Bertrand,
2010), and in female compared to male dancers in equilibrium reactions (Golomer, Dupui, &
Monod, 1997b). Even short episodes of breakdance training have been found to increase
with physical training, leading to a changed balance of the individual senses in multimodal
processing. For example, enhanced proprioceptive skills associated with dancers’ heightened
posture control have been suggested to interfere with other sensory processes such as vision
(Golomer et al., 1999a; Jola, Davis, & Haggard, 2011). Dance training has been claimed to
increase the relative influence of somatosensation and to shift sensorimotor dominance from
vision to proprioception (e.g., Golomer & Dupui, 2000). Therefore, skilled dancers should
have a more accurate position sense based on proprioceptive information, and should rely
more on proprioception than on vision compared to non-dancers. Empirical evidence for this
hypothesis has been found for dynamic equilibrium tasks among professional ballet dancers
(Golomer & Dupui, 2000), and for position-matching tasks involving matching the hand
location in space (Jola, Davis, & Haggard, 2011; Ramsay & Riddoch, 2001). In the latter,
dancers performed significantly better than controls when only proprioceptive information
was available. Surprisingly, however, dancers seemed to rely more on proprioception even
when vision was available, leading to a tendency toward higher error rates in the vision-only
In contrast, studies using static equilibrium tasks showed that dancers’ balance strategies
Hence, in balance tasks with closed eyes, dancers perform no better than controls (Hugel,
Cadopi, Kohler & Perrin, 1999; Golomer, Dupui, Sereni, & Monod 1999b) and worse than
judo experts (Perrin, Deviterne, Hugel & Perrot 2002). Nevertheless, dancers’ dynamic
patterns of postural sway are modulated by visual input in different ways from those of non-
dancers. Dancers’ sway patterns while standing on a foam surface with their eyes closed were
found to be more stationary (showing lower trend) and less regular, stable and complex
(lower recurrence, mathematical stability, and entropy) than those of track athletes (Schmit,
Neurocognitive control in dance 8
Regis, & Riley, 2005). While dancers generally showed smaller pitch sway oscillations than
untrained controls, pitch and roll sway were increased in dancers standing on one leg with the
left hemifield occluded, whereas only pitch sway was increased in untrained controls under
the same conditions (Golomer, Mbongo, Toussaint, Cadiou & Israel, 2010).
These findings suggest that sensory control strategies might be task-specific, and that dance
multimodal integration for dynamic equilibrium tasks and position-matching, but not for static
equilibrium tasks. Taken together, these studies demonstrate that basic functions underlying
the control of equilibrium, posture, and sway are sensitive to training effects, and that dance
training has the potential to stabilize and align dancers’ performance via these functions. The
of vision and somatosensation deserve further clarification. Such research could focus on
issues such as contributions of individual sensory modalities and their integration in balance
and posture control. A subsequent question that emerges asks how multimodal integration is
skilled dance experts. Dancers achieve this goal by optimizing motor synergies and
consequently reducing energy costs in terms of force and muscle tension. In general,
kinematic analysis shows that classical dancers have the ability to efficiently combine
movements of related joints into single motor synergies, thus reducing the number of degrees
of freedom at the level of neuronal control. This leads to highly accurate reproduction of the
orientation and shape of the required trajectories (Thullier & Moufti, 2004; Wilson, Lim, &
Kwon, 2004). Lepelley, Thullier, Koral, and Lestienne (2006) found that skilled ballet
Neurocognitive control in dance 9
dancers selectively applied minimal muscle tension at the reversal point of ballistic leg
movements from their classical repertoire, even though torque was maximal in this position.
Whole body rotations, especially unipedal turns such as pirouettes, require stabilization of the
turning axis through the supporting leg, as well as alignment of shoulders and hips over the
same axis. Skilled dancers, in contrast to controls, were capable of maintaining shoulders and
hips en bloc during different kinds of turns, independent of turning direction and laterality of
the supporting leg (Golomer, Touissant, Bouillette & Keller, 2009b). The ability to control
unipedal turns also depends on the starting posture, and dancers have been found to optimize
the relation of foot distance and weight distribution during the preparation of the turn (Sugano
Interestingly, individual preference for turning direction might strongly influence dancers’
skills in performing whole body turns. A rightward turning bias has been described for adult
ballet dancers (Golomer, Rosey, Dizac, Mertz & Fagard, 2009a; Starosta, 2000), whereas
untrained controls predominantly showed a leftward turning bias and a weaker dependency
between turning bias and leg preference than dancers (Golomer et al., 2009a). From these
results it has been concluded that turning bias is sensitive to training effects and that dancers
who are specifically trained for symmetry of movement can partly counterbalance such
biases. Empirical findings seem to suggest that classical dance training induces a rightward
turning tendency; however, it cannot be excluded that individuals with a natural rightward
As shown in this section, research investigating the control of specific dance movements
revealed dancers’ skills in optimizing motor synergies, which leads to reduced muscle tension
and increased accuracy of movement. For rotational movements, dancers seem to develop
Neurocognitive control in dance 10
specific strategies to stabilize the turning axis and to overcome individual turning biases.
Current research issues include the adaptation of neurocognitive control functions to dancers’
modified physical abilities, and to challenges such as increased flexibility, efficient patterns of
muscle activation, and coordination of novel movements. We should ask how cognitive
strategies utilize these functions to advance performance even further, and how such strategies
Taken together, the studies presented here show that dance training has the potential to
influence basic functions underlying motor control, including multimodal integration as well
as posture and equilibrium control, facilitating the performance of complex movements via
dancers’ special skills in body alignment and balance tasks. Building up on these conditions,
dancers can apply specific strategies such as the optimization of motor synergies when
develop and apply these strategies in an explicit way that requires attentional processes and
makes them accessible for higher cognitive processes, such as the use of imagery, and
adaptable to external acoustic or visual cues. Therefore, even though dancers’ movement
expertise can be examined and described via biomechanical measures (see Krasnow,
Wilmerding, Stecyk, Wyon, & Koutedakis, 2011), physical skills in dance can hardly be
regarded separatedly from the cognitive functions and strategies that enable dancers to make
One crucial aspect in any dance performance is timing, which refers to either the
accompanying music. For example, two dancers in a dance ensemble may start a movement
sequence at the same time, follow a dynamic path which refers to the speed and the trajectory
Neurocognitive control in dance 11
of the movement, and finish the sequence at the same time. Even in a dance piece that has
across a four-minute piece of only 5% (Stevens, Schubert, Wang, Kross, & Halovic, 2009).
This negligible variation appears more related to memory lapse than to a miscalibrated
internal clock.
dance sequences and the effects of choreographic complexity on dancers’ timing in solo
performances, showing that short walking distances as well as high movement complexity
increased timing errors due to interference with dancers’ attention. As there are observable
likely even more complex due to the need for synchrony with other dancers. A first step into
exploring the dynamics of dance ensemble performance was made by Maduell and Wing
(2007), who provided a detailed feedback model with hierarchical control structures in terms
ensemble. Different degrees of control between ensemble members suggested the use of
distinct attentional strategies for integrating information from other members based on the
member’s status within the ensemble. Musical cues have been shown to indicate the
appropriateness of specific moves at specific points, but physical contact between dancers
facilitates accuracy timing and is robust to differences in musical structure (Gentry & Feron,
Understanding the interactions and control levels within dance ensembles is important to
explain how dancers achieve and maintain synchrony within a pair or group. Honisch, Roach,
and Wing (2009) investigated interpersonal interactions and their effects on ensemble
synchrony. Temporal accuracy was their focus; specifically events such as target positions or
dynamic cues to which dancers may synchronize their movements. Expert ballet dancers not
only synchronized better with familiar compared with less familiar movements, but they were
also more accurate in synchronizing to the dynamics of the movement (e.g. its peak velocity)
compared to the target position (endpoint in space; Honisch, Roach, & Wing, 2009). The
results suggest that dancers’ timing skills are modulated by motor experience with particular
movements. In addition, dancers’ anticipation of target positions may enable faster detection
Dance ensemble coordination and timing is complex, influenced by factors such as attention
demands, performer motor experience, and status within an ensemble. Constructing predictive
models of real life performances will help explain how successful coordination between
multiple dancers can be achieved and facilitated. Such emerging research will also inform,
expectations.
The complex movement sequences executed by dancers in solos, duets, and ensemble pieces
epitomize the human capacity for sequence learning. Research using dance material as stimuli
is significant for memory research because, unlike digits, letters, or spatial locations, the to-
be-remembered movement items not only extend in time for some seconds, but the sequences
of items also unfold over time. A question that arises is how sequences of complex dance
unstructured sequences. For example, recall performance of expert dancers who had
comparable experience in both ballet and modern dance, were significantly greater for
sequences of ballet than modern dance (Jean, Cadopi, & Ille, 2001). A concurrent verbal
interference task lowered recall rates slightly, suggesting some verbal rehearsal of to-be-
remembered dance items and, in a control (no interference) condition, structured sequences
were recalled better than unstructured sequences. Working with 11-year-old expert ballet and
novice dancers, Strakes, Deakin, Lindley, and Crisp (1987) showed advantages in recall by
experts compared with novices, and for structured compared with unstructured ballet
sequences. The last elements in the ballet sequence stimuli were also recalled less often,
especially when the previously-accompanying music was absent. Smyth and Pendleton (1994)
showed that professional ballet dancers recorded longer memory spans than those of non-
dancers for both ballet and nonsense movements. Relative to non-dancers, the dancers
appeared to have enhanced encoding of movement items in general, i.e., dance and non-dance
items. In an experiment by Cross, Hamilton, Kraemer, Kelley, and Grafton (2009), non-
dancers trained on dance steps with accompanying techno music performed significantly
better when a human model also performed the dance steps than when visual directional cues
(i.e., scrolling arrows) were presented. Individual components of the action observation
network appear to respond differently to the human form and to dance training.
Dance experts use a variety of strategies and techniques to encode sequences of movement.
For example, they indicate body movements with the hands – so-called “marking” (Allard &
Starkes, 1991; Kirsh, Muntanyola, Jao, Lew, & Sugihara, 2009), a labor-saving means of
rehearsal using reduced range of motion and energy expenditure, which helps to verify
Neurocognitive control in dance 14
movement rhythm, direction, and spacing. In addition to its utility as a rehearsal technique,
Articulatory suppression and interference have been used as methods to investigate the
contribution of verbal, spatial, and/or motor codes in encoding, rehearsing, and recalling
series of dance items from working memory (e.g., Jean et al. 2001; Rossi-Arnaud, Cortese, &
Cestari, 2004). Suppression tasks involve performing a task at the same time as observing the
to-be-remembered material, and are used to disrupt encoding of the to-be-remembered (TBR)
presentation of the material and recall, and are used to disrupt rehearsal using a verbal or
spatial process, respectively. The logic of these paradigms is that if, for example, the TBR
material is encoded and/or rehearsed using a verbal code, then a concurrent verbal task but not
a concurrent motor or spatial task, should reduce recall of the TBR material. Smyth and
proposed that in spatial memory the location of a target in space is the goal for an action,
whereas in memory for movement the configuration of the body parts is the goal. In a recent
experiment, concurrent spatial interference (tapping four visuo-spatial targets) did not affect
memory for ballet moves by ballet dancers; this result was interpreted as evidence in support
of a system for motor configurations in working memory (Cortese & Rossi-Arnaud, 2010).
Serial position data tend to reveal primacy but not recency effects in short-term memory for
ballet steps (Allard & Starkes, 1991; Starkes et al. 1987), implying that movement items are
chained. An informative contrasting result is the presence of both primacy and recency effects
when the to-be-remembered material is modern dance (Starkes, Caicco, Boutilier, & Sevsek,
1990). Starkes et al. (1990) interpret this finding in light of modern dance differing from
ballet with fewer established verbal labels in modern dance and the possibility that “lack of
Neurocognitive control in dance 15
structure may become an important cue in and of itself” (p. 320). Thus, conditions under
which primacy and recency effects occur warrant further investigation. Recall may be greater
when there is some higher-level ordinal and/or temporal structure including more biologically
plausible flow between movement items. The use of more ecologically valid dance material as
stimuli in memory studies is increasing and will enable comparison across dance genres.
Primacy and recency effects are likely to be useful tools to examine chaining or hierarchical
Knowledge structures in long-term memory for dance are influenced by dance expertise
(Bläsing, 2010). For example, Bläsing, Tenenbaum, and Schack (2009) compared the
hierarchical structure of basic action concepts for ballet movements in professional dancers,
amateurs, and non-dancers. The cognitive movement structures of experts and advanced
amateurs, but not beginners or novices, were consistent with functional movement structures
based on biomechanical principles. When spatial directions linked to movements were used as
stimuli (Bläsing & Schack, 2011), only professional dancers, as opposed to beginners or
In situations where dance material has been crafted and sequenced according to an underlying
organizational structure, the structure can be considered a grammar or a rule that governs
transitions. Studies are emerging that investigate the assumption that expert dancers predict
when they watch dance and this prediction is based on memory for the dance material and
transitions between movements, phrases and sections. For example, Opacic, Stevens, &
Tillmann (2009) have demonstrated that after intensive exposure to examples of dance
transitions that conform to an artificial grammar, novice dance observers in a test phase are
increasingly accurate at selecting new grammatical sequences. The greater accuracy of the
exposure group versus the no-exposure control group is taken as evidence of the development
Neurocognitive control in dance 16
of expectations or memory that enables prediction of dance material. Such a result is also
develops through visual experience that, in turn, heightens preference and liking for the dance
material.
In experts, intensive and extended exposure to dance likely develops schematic expectations
for dance styles, choreographic traditions, and so on. Expectations about a particular dance
piece, or so-called veridical expectations, may also develop while watching a performance.
The hypothesis that dance experts have acquired expectations about dance that facilitate
perception has been studied by measuring observers’ eye movements. As in the comparison
between fixation times in the eye movements of expert and novice drivers, athletes, and
pilots, the fixation times of dance experts watching a dance film were significantly shorter
than those of novice observers (Stevens, Winskel, Howell, Vidal, Latimer, & Milne-Home,
2010). The dance experts’ enhanced speed of visual processing suggests that they are adept at
anticipating and processing dance material, possibly aided by acquired expectations in long-
term memory concerning body and movement configurations. Contextual cues to long-term
memory for dance movement, such as accompanying music, are being explored (Stevens,
Ginsborg, & Lester, 2011; Stevens, Schubert, Wang, Kroos, & Halovic, 2009).
In this section, we have considered memory for dance material, item order, and transitions,
and have seen that sequence structure, sometimes verbal rehearsal, and accompanying cues
such as music aid encoding and recall. Current issues include the multimodal codes and cues
in memory for movement, and the relationship between mere exposure, perceptual fluency,
and preference. Research concerning imagery and spatial transformation in dance is reviewed
In dance training and performance, mental imagery of movement is frequently used as a tool
for learning and optimizing movements. Dancers use mental imagery in creating new material
(e.g., Fink, Graif, & Neubauer, 2009; May, Calvo-Merino, deLahunta, Cusack, Owen, et al.
2011), to exercise the memorization of long complex phrases, and to improve movement
quality in terms of spatiotemporal adaptation and artistic expression. Dance training has been
found to increase the amount and efficiency of kinaesthetic imagery used and to enhance the
imagery of kinaesthetic sensations, making images more complex and vivid (Golomer,
Bouillette, Mertz, & Keller, 2008; Nordin & Cumming, 2007). In order to decrease physical
stress, especially during recovery from injury, alternative dance training methods based on
mental imagery have frequently been recommended (Krasnow, 1997). These studies
corroborate evidence from dance practice by showing that dancers have learned to apply
mental imagery more successfully and more consistently than dance novices and that they can
even reproduce this ability under laboratory conditions. In this context it is feasible to ask:
What mechanisms underlie the different types and aspects of motor imagery, and how and
The theory that motor imagery is based on simulation processes that recruit motor
imagery, increased cardiac and muscular activity can be observed, as well as increased
cortical activity of high frequencies (beta activity) in a broad range of cortical areas,
indicating states of high concentration and attention comparable to active movement (Blaser
& Hökelmann, 2004, 2009). Cortical circuits activated during motor imagery (to be described
in the next section) overlap to a large extent with those activated during movement generation
and movement observation. Imagery in the absence of sensory input specifically necessitates
Neurocognitive control in dance 18
internal motor attention processes, evidenced by specific activation in the posterior insula and
anterior cingulate gyrus (May et al. 2011; Munzert, Zentgraf, Stark, & Vaitl, 2008).
It has previously been stated that experienced dancers, compared to novices, show increased
expertise in kinesthetic imagery tasks, based on the common use of motor imagery in dance
training. This notion has led to the assumption that dancers should also show enhanced skills
presented stimuli have to be mentally manipulated in spatial orientation from the observer’s
perspective. Studies involving mental rotation of visual stimuli revealed different cognitive
processes for objects (e.g., Shepard & Metzler, 1971) from that of human bodies or body parts
(e.g., Parsons, 1987). Jola and Mast (2005) assumed that dancers should perform extremely
well in mental rotation tasks involving human bodies, but their study found no such expertise
effects in dancers compared to controls. In contrast, gymnasts and judo experts performed
better than controls under comparable conditions (Weigelt, Steggemann, Bläsing & Schack,
The fact that dancers show an advantage in studies investigating motor imagery, but display
each kind of task. A comparison of findings from studies investigating active and attentive
motor imagery and those investigating visually-presented mental transformation tasks on the
other side suggests that dancers’ expertise is more likely to involve the conscious and
strategic use of motor imagery and motor simulation (as used in dance training), but does not
seem to generalize to the mostly unconscious processes of covert action, as other classic
motor preparation and mental transformation studies have suggested (Jeannerod, 2004).
Current issues deriving from these results include the role of attentional focus in motor
learning and performance, the relative efficiency of different types of motor imagery differing
Neurocognitive control in dance 19
that link motor simulation to motor execution. As observation and motor imagery appear to
share neural substrates, further related issues will be discussed in the following section.
While not all dance is performed for a large audience (such as participatory folk dances or
social dancing in a nightclub), dance as a performing art implies the role or involvement of
have been widely studied in the human and non-human primate brain (for a review, see
Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010). Evidence from these studies suggests that when observing
action, we internally simulate the observed movement using similar brain regions used to
execute the movement with our own body. The network of regions shown to be active during
movement execution, observation, and imagery includes inferior parietal and premotor
cortices as core nodes, and has been described as the human mirror system (Grèzes & Decety,
2001). Most early human mirror neuron studies used everyday hand actions (e.g., grasping) as
stimuli, investigating neural responses to only a very limited portion of the complex human
motor repertoire. Recently, several laboratories have turned to populations of expert and
novice dancers to further delineate how the brain links action with perception, and how the
mirror system may be engaged in the learning and observation of the coordinated full-body
The earliest work with dancers that used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to
explore how motor expertise shapes brain activity in action observation demonstrated that
expert ballet and capoeira dancers (Calvo-Merino, Glaser, Grèzes, Passingham, & Haggard,
2005), as well as contemporary dancers (Cross, Hamilton & Grafton, 2006), show increased
activity in brain areas considered to be part of the human mirror system while watching
Neurocognitive control in dance 20
movements that they have learnt to perform (i.e., that have been acquired in their motor
repertoire), compared with similar movements that they have not performed before.
rhythmical brain activity when expert contemporary dancers and non-dancers watch dance
movements and everyday actions (Orgs, Dombrowski, Heil, & Jansen-Osmann, 2008).
measure of motor simulation, while watching dance compared to non-dancers. Together, these
studies using the expertise model highlight the utility of working with specialized populations
of dancers to explore fundamental questions about how the brain links movement experience
Another line of research has attempted to further dissociate how responses within motor
regions of the brain during action observation are modified by visual or motor experience. In
one study, expert ballet dancers watched gender-neutral movements, observed and performed
regularly by both male and female dancers, and gender-specific movements, observed
regularly but never performed by the opposite sex (Calvo-Merino, Grèzes, Glaser,
Passingham & Haggard, 2006). The authors found that premotor and parietal regions
demonstrate responses specifically tuned to motor familiarity of the observer, over and above
responses seen in the same mirror system regions when the dancers watched movements that
were visually familiar, but never executed. The second study used novice dancers learning
simple dance sequences in a dance video game context, either by physical practice or passive
observation (Cross, Kraemer, Hamilton, Kelley, & Grafton, 2009b). Among this population,
there were similarities between physical and observational learning within parietal and
premotor mirror system regions, with performance data adding additional support to the
notion that physical and observational learning shape the brain and behavior in a similar
manner. These two studies suggest that the human action observation network may be more
Neurocognitive control in dance 21
thought. Further evidence illustrating how dance experience shapes the nervous system is
provided by a study investigating the impact of professional ballet training on the structure of
and connectivity between sensorimotor brain regions (Hänggi, Koeneke, Bezzola, & Jäncke,
2010). Hänggi and colleagues report clear evidence of intensive dance training reducing the
volume of grey and white matter within sensorimotor cortical and subcortical regions,
compared to non-dancers. Additional studies with expert dancers will help to enlighten the
underlying components supporting the complex mechanisms linking dance experience with
A number of challenges and opportunities exist for researchers who wish to work with
observation (e.g., Calvo-Merino, 2010; Cross, 2010; Jola, 2010). While exploring the neural
links, such experimentation will be improved by the development of behavioral measures that
quantify how motor experience shapes perception. One recent study provides preliminary
support for this claim, by demonstrating that ballet dancers’ physical experience shapes their
A further feature of action observation ripe for future exploration concerns how dance, which
is often seen as a uniquely human expression, can help us to better understand how we
perceive robotic agents (Cross, Liepelt, Hamilton, Parkinson, Ramsey, Stadler, & Prinz, 2011;
Miura, Sugiura, Takahashi, Sassa, Miyamoto, Sato, et al. 2010). A recent such investigation
demonstrated that robots whose breakdancing movements are closely matched to a human
breakdancer’s movements are perceived as highly animate, human-like agents (Cross et al.,
Neurocognitive control in dance 22
2011). A challenge remains for future studies to advance these preliminary investigations,
perhaps by exploring the neural and behavioral consequences of the social nature of dance.
Further, a particularly intriguing and formidable issue for future work in this domain [it
otherwise sounds very strange to me.] will be to bridge empirical research on dance
observation with actual dance performance, in order to draw stronger conclusions about how
motor and visual experience shapes perception not only among experienced dancers, but also
While movement restrictions make it a daunting task to record brain responses in naturalistic
dance contexts, researchers might take inspiration from Brown and colleagues, who pioneered
a technique for studying the performance of tango steps while scanning dancers’ brains with
positron emission tomography (Brown, Martinez, & Parsons, 2006). Another approach is to
study actual dance performance from the spectators’ point of view. These studies investigate
how the brains of spectators who are not dancers themselves respond to watching live dance
performances (Jola, Ehrenberg, & Reynolds 2011; Jola, Pollick, & Grosbras, 2011). This is
indeed one of the most exciting new directions being pursued within this field, only just
beginning to yield the first results. Development and testing of such paradigms promise to
shed light on how non-dancers perceive dance, as well as to transcend typically reductionist
evaluations of watching dance that are classically used in laboratory experiments. The aim of
such studies is to perform experimental work while maintaining the fidelity of the dance
performance (i.e., the choreographer’s and performers’ intentions). In the final section, we
examine issues of aesthetic appreciation in more detail. [It’s not correct otherwise and it is
As is clear from the evidence reviewed in the previous section, the perception of another
processes between the actor and observer (e.g., Blake & Shiffrar, 2007). Dancers and
choreographers apply this principle in their art, deliberately creating, modifying and shaping
implicit and explicit messages of the moving body (see Stevens & McKechnie, 2005).
Studies have corroborated that dance conveys information about emotional states (Chicchella
& Bianchini 2004; Dittrich, Troscianko, Lea, & Morgan, 1996; Sawada, Suda, & Ishii, 2003)
and the dancer’s physical condition (Brown, Cronk, Grochow, Jacobson, Liu, Popovic, et al.
2005) to the observer. Laws and colleagues applied basic mechanical principles to analyse
how dancers achieve typical aesthetic qualities when performing different types of ballet
movements (Laws 1995, 1998; Laws & Petrie 1999). Stevens, Malloch, McKechnie and
Steven (2003) point out that the essence of novelty in artistic creativity may be metaphorical
To shed light on the art of dance in the context of neuroaesthetics, Calvo-Merino, Jola,
Glaser, and Haggard (2008) used fMRI to determine brain activity related to subjective
tasks, and later rated the movements along various aesthetic dimensions. High aesthetics
ratings correlated with increased activity in the occipital cortices and in right premotor portion
of the mirror system, suggesting that visual and sensorimotor brain areas might play a role in
an automatic aesthetic response to dance. Another recent study aimed to quantify the
and how much he or she liked watching it (Cross, Kirsch, Ticini, & Schütz-Bosbach, 2011).
These authors report that not only do dance-naïve participants enjoy watching difficult dance
Neurocognitive control in dance 24
movements that they cannot physically perform, but this relationship between liking and a
et al., 2008; Cross et al., 2011) is that they used a subjective approach that enabled them to
closely examine which movements appealed most to their participants. In this way, Calvo-
Merino et al. (2008) were able to tell that the premotor region showed a preference for fast
moves with vertical displacement, and Cross et al. (2011) reported that the more physically
difficult participants perceived the movements to be, the more they were enjoyed. This type of
subjective approach can be communicated to the dance community and, where there is
interest, such information could be used to create a dance phrase aesthetically pleasant for the
human brain (Calvo-Merino, 2010; Cross and Ticini, 2011; but see Jola, 2011; Jola,
Ehrenberg, & Reynolds, 2011 regarding the complexity of combining neuroscience and
choreography). Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) has been used recently to interfere
with aesthetic judgments (Calvo-Merino, Urgesi, Orgs, Aglioti, & Haggard, 2010).
body area, showed the existence of a complementary network for aesthetic evaluation of
dance body postures, hypothesized to include visual and motor regions. The accumulating set
of fMRI and TMS experiments highlight the importance of sensorimotor mechanisms for the
aesthetic experience of dance. This sensorimotor experience may be a reflection of high levels
during the aesthetic perception compared to mere visual perception (Calvo-Merino et al.,
2011).
The intersection of dance and brain-based models of aesthetic appreciation is one that is ripe
for further inquiry. As discussed and debated elsewhere (Calvo-Merino, 2010; Cross &
Neurocognitive control in dance 25
Ticini, 2011; Jackobson & Calvo-Merino, submitted; Jola, Pollick & Grosbras, 2011), the
nascent field of neuroaesthetics offers opportunities for dancers and scientists to collaborate in
order to gain a better understanding of how dance creation and expression is perceived and
evaluated by audiences, and how this relationship might be modulated on both sides of the
scientific methods to better understand our relationship to dance will continue to attract
interest from domains ranging from cognitive psychology, neuroscience, and cross-cultural
7. Conclusion
Performing and perceiving dance epitomizes embodied cognitive processes including those
perception, and motor simulation. Dance thus sheds a critical light on current experimental
dancers’ outstanding motor and cognitive skills. Therefore, dance has not only the potential to
provide insights into cognitive, emotional, and aesthetic function and behaviour, but also it
has the potential to impact contemporary scientific approaches. For this reason, the areas
highlighted in this review are by no means the only avenues where future work in the
psychological, cognitive and brain sciences might benefit from establishing liaisons with
dance. Based on the research reviewed here, many new issues centered on the neurocognitive
processes engaged by dance emerge that are ripe for future exploration, such as the effects of
training on multimodal integration and memory encoding for movement, the role of
attentional focus in motor learning and performance, the effects of motor experience on brain
activity in response to live dance performance, the factors that shape neural responses in
aesthetic experience, and positive effects of dance activity on wellbeing across the lifespan
and applications in rehabilitation. Finally, as avatars and humanoid robots become more
Neurocognitive control in dance 26
commonplace, there is an increasing demand for the generation of authentic biological motion
in non-biological agents. Modelling the rich, complex biological motion patterns inherent in
human dance might also help to enhance robot and avatar naturalness, which in turn should
help us to further understand the neural and behavioral consequences of the social nature of
dance. As interest from both the scientific and artistic communities for pursuing
reciprocal benefits to both fields. Such benefits will be further propagated by the fact that
dance is an ever-changing art form. Although dance is a form of human expression that has
been around since the dawn of human culture (Stehle, 1997), it has boomed in the 20th
century (Copeland and Cohen, 1983) with continually evolving styles and expressions
(Daprati et al., 2009). This ever-expanding vocabulary of human expression will benefit from
scientific investigation to remain adaptive, and will provide psychological and brain scientists
a practically never-ending source of study. Taking the studies highlighted in this review as a
point of departure, we encourage researchers from the behavioral and brain sciences to
consider dance paradigms as a means to study questions ranging from motor control to the
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to all the dancers and choreographers who participated in the studies
described here. Beatriz Calvo-Merino was supported by the subprogram Ramon Y Cajal
(MICINN-RYC) and a City University Fellowship (City University London). Emily S. Cross
was supported by a fellowship from the Alexander von Humboldt Foundation. Juliane
Honisch was supported by Qualisys (Sweden). Corinne Jola was supported by the Arts and
Humanities Research Council project Watching Dance: Kinesthetic Empathy and University
of Surrey Research Support. The authors thank Lauren R. Alpert for assistance with
manuscript preparation.
Neurocognitive control in dance 28
References
Allard, F., & Starkes, J. L. (1991). Motor skill expertise in sports, dance and other domains.
In K. A. Ericsson & J. Smith (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: prospects
and limits (pp. 126-171). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Blaser, P., & Hökelmann, A. (2004). Relationships between load and demand under the
condition of the mental representation of a dance. Journal of Human Kinetics 12, 15-
30.
Blaser, P., & Hökelmann, A. (2009). Mental reproduction of a dance choreography and its
effects on physiological fatigue in dancers. Journal of Human Sport and Exercise,
4(2), 129-141.
Bläsing, B. (2010) The dancer’s memory. In B. Bläsing, M. Puttke, & T. Schack (Eds.), The
neurocognition of dance: Mind, movement and motor skills (pp. 75-98). London:
Psychology Press.
Bläsing, B., Puttke, M., & Schack, T. (Eds.), The neurocognition of dance: Mind, movement
and motor skills. London: Psychology Press.
Bläsing, B., & Schack, T. (2011). Mental representations of spatial movement parameters in
dance. Spatial Cognition and Computation. DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2011.626095.
Bläsing, B., Tenenbaum, G., & Schack, T. (2009). The cognitive structure of movements in
classical dance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10(3), 350-360.
Blake, R., & Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annual Review of Psychology,
58, 47-73.
Brown, S., Martinez, M. J., & Parsons, L. M. (2006). The neural basis of human dance.
Cerebral Cortex, 16(8), 1157-1167.
Brown, W. M., Cronk, L., Grochow, K., Jacobson, A., Liu, C. K., Popovic, Z., et al. (2005).
Dance reveals symmetry especially in young men. Nature, 438(7071), 1148-1150.
Bruyneel, A.V., Mesure, S., Paré, J.C., & Bertrand M. (2010) Organization of postural
equilibrium in several planes in ballet dancers. Neuroscience Letters, 485 (3), 228-
232.
Calvo-Merino, B. (2010). Neural mechanisms for seeing dance. In B. Bläsing, M. Puttke, &
T. Schack (Eds.), The neurocognition of dance: Mind, movement and motor skills (pp.
153-176). London: Psychology Press.
Calvo-Merino, B., Ehrenberg, S., Leung, D., & Haggard, P. (2010). Experts see it all:
configural effects in action observation. Psychological Research, 74(4), 400-406.
Neurocognitive control in dance 29
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action
observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral
Cortex, 15(8), 1243-1249.
Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing
or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current
Biology, 16(19), 1905-1910.
Calvo-Merino, B., Jola, C., Glaser, D. E., & Haggard, P. (2008). Towards a sensorimotor
aesthetics of performing art. Consciousness & Cognition, 17(3), 911-922.
Calvo-Merino, B., Urgesi, C., Orgs, G., Aglioti, S. M., & Haggard, P. (2010). Extrastriate
body area underlies aesthetic evaluation of body stimuli. Experimental Brain
Research, 204(3), 447-456.
Calvo-Merino, B., Gillmeister, H., Jones, A., Tziraki, M., Haggard, P., Forster, B. (2011).
Somatotopic embodiment of aesthetic processing of human body postures. Front.
Hum. Neurosci. Conference Abstract: XI International Conference on Cognitive
Neuroscience (ICON XI). In press
Chatfield, S. J., Krasnow, D. H., Herman, A., & Blessing, G. (2007). A descriptive analysis of
kinematic and electromyographic relationships of the core during forward stepping in
beginning and expert dancers. Journal of Dance Medicine and Science, 11(3), 9.
Chichella, C., & Bianchini, K. (2004). Characteristics of movement and emotions elicited by
two different kinds of dance. Journal of Human Kinetics 11, 59-68.
Cortese, A., & Rossi-Arnaud, C. (2010). Working memory for ballet moves and spatial
locations in professional ballet dancers. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 266-286.
Copeland, R. & Cohen, M. (1983) What Is Dance?. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cross, E. S. (2010). Building a dance in the human brain: Insights from expert and novice
dancers. In B. Bläsing, M. Puttke, & T. Schack (Eds.), The neurocognition of dance:
Mind, movement and motor skills (pp. 177-202). London: Psychology Press.
Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo:
Observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage, 31(3), 1257-1267.
Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F., Kraemer, D. J., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009a).
Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action
observation network. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(7),1383-92.
Cross, E. S., Kirsch, L., Ticini, L., & Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2011). The impact of aesthetic
appreciation and physical ability on dance perception. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 2011;5:102
Neurocognitive control in dance 30
Cross, E. S., Kraemer, D. J., Hamilton, A. F., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009b).
Sensitivity of the action observation network to physical and observational learning.
Cerebral Cortex, 19(2), 315-326.
Cross, E. S., Liepelt, R., Hamilton, A. F. d. C., Parkinson, J., Ramsey, R., Stadler, W., et al.
(2011). Robotic actions preferentially engage the action observation network. Human
Brain Mapping, Sep 6. DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21361.
Cross, E. S., & Ticini, L. F. (2011). Neuroaesthetics and beyond: New horizons in applying
the science of the brain to the art of dance. Phenomenology and the Cognitive
Sciences, DOI: 10.1007/s11097-010-9190-y
Crotts, D., Thompson, B., Nahom, M., Ryan, S., & Newton, R. A. (1996). Balance abilities of
professional dancers on select balance tests. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther, 23(1), 12-17.
Daprati, E., Iosa, M., Haggard, P. (2009). A dance to the music of time: Aesthetically-relevant
changes in body posture in performing art. PLoS One 4(3), e5023.
Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from
dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25(6), 727-738.
Edwards, D. (2008). Artscience: Creativity in the post-Google generation. Cambridge:
Harvard University Press.
Fink, A., Graif, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2009). Brain correlates underlying creative thinking:
EEG alpha activity in professional vs. novice dancers. NeuroImage 46, 854-862.
Gentry, S., & Feron, E. (2004). Musicality experiments in lead and follow dance. Paper
presented at the IEEE Systems, Man and Cybernetics Conference.
Golomer, E., Bouillette, A., Mertz, C., & Keller, J. (2008). Effects of mental imagery styles
on shoulder and hip rotations during preparation of pirouettes. Journal of Motor
Behavior, 40(4), 281-290.
Golomer, E., Cremieux, J., Dupui, P., Isableu, B., & Ohlmann, T. (1999a). Visual
contribution to self-induced body sway frequencies and visual perception of male
professional dancers. Neuroscience Letters, 267(3), 189-192.
Golomer, E., & Dupui, P. (2000). Spectral analysis of adult dancers' sways: sex and
interaction vision-proprioception. International Journal of Neuroscience, 105(1-4),
15-26.
Golomer, E., Dupui, P., & Monod, H. (1997a). Sex-linked differences in equilibrium
reactions among adolescents performing complex sensorimotor tasks. J Physiol Paris,
91(2), 49-55.
Neurocognitive control in dance 31
Golomer, E., Dupui, P., & Monod, H. (1997b). The effects of maturation on self-induced
dynamic body sway frequencies of girls performing acrobatics or classical dance. Eur
J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol, 76(2), 140-144.
Golomer, E., Dupui, P., Sereni, P., & Monod, H. (1999b). The contribution of vision in
dynamic spontaneous sways of male classical dancers according to student or
professional level. J Physiol Paris, 93(3), 233-237.
Golomer, E., Mbongo, F., Toussaint, Y., Cadiou, M., & Israel, I. (2010). Right hemisphere in
visual regulation of complex equilibrium: the female ballet dancers' experience.
Neurological Research, 32(4), 409-415.
Golomer, E., Rosey, F., Dizac, H., Mertz, C., & Fagard, J. (2009a). The influence of classical
dance training on preferred supporting leg and whole body turning bias. Laterality,
14(2), 165-177.
Golomer, E., Toussaint, Y., Bouillette, A., & Keller, J. (2009b). Spontaneous whole body
rotations and classical dance expertise: how shoulder-hip coordination influences
supporting leg displacements. J Electromyogr Kinesiol, 19(2), 314-321.
Grahn, J. A., & McAuley, J. D. (2009). Neural bases of individual differences in beat
perception. Neuroimage, 47, 1894-1903.
Grèzes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation,
observation, and verb generation of actions: a meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping,
12(1), 1-19.
Hänggi, J., Koeneke, S., Bezzola, L., Jäncke, L. (2010). Structural neuroplasticity in the
sensorimotor network of professional female ballet dancers. Human Brain Mapping,
31(8), 1196-1206.
Hanna, J. L. (1979). To dance is human: A theory of nonverbal communication. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press.
Honisch, J. J., Roach, N., & Wing, A. M. (2009). Movement synchronization to a virtual
dancer: How do expert dancers adjust to perceived temporal and spatial changes
whilst performing ballet versus abstract dance sequences? Paper presented at the ISSP
12th World Congress of Sport Psychology.
Hugel, F., Cadopi, M., Kohler, F., & Perrin, P. (1999). Postural control of ballet dancers: a
specific use of visual input for artistic purposes. International Journal of Sports
Medicine, 20(2), 86-92.
Jackobson & Calvo-Merino, B. (submitted). Dance: from movement to aesthetics.
Neurocognitive control in dance 32
Jean, J., Cadopi, M., & Ille, A. (2001). How are dance sequences encoded and recalled by
expert dancers? Current Psychology of Cognition, 20, 325-337.
Jeannerod, M. (1995). Mental imagery in the motor context. Neuropsychologia, 33(11), 1419-
1432.
Jeannerod, M. (2004). Actions from within. International Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 2(4), 376 – 402.
Jola, C. (2010). Research and choreography – merging dance and cognitive neuroscience. In
B. Bläsing, M. Puttke, & Th. Schack (Eds.), The neurocognition of dance. Mind,
movement and motor skills (pp. 203-234). Psychology Press: Hove, UK.
Jola, C., Davis, A., Haggard, P. (2011). Proprioceptive integration and body representation:
insights into dancers’ expertise. Experimental Brain Research, 213(2-3), 257-265.
Jola, C., Ehrenberg, S., Reynolds, D. (2011). The experience of watching dance:
phenomenological-neuroscience duets. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences,
DOI: 10.1007/s11097-010-9191-x.
Jola, C., & Mast, F. W. (2005). Mental object rotation and egocentric body transformation:
Two dissociable processes? Spatial Cognition and Computation, .5(2-3), 217 – 237.
Jola, C., Pollick, F., & Grosbras, M. H. (2011). Arousal decrease in 'Sleeping Beauty':
audiences' neurophysiological correlates to watching a narrative dance performance of
2.5 hrs. Dance Research Electronic, forthcoming.
Kirsh, D., Muntanyola, D., Jao, R. J., Lew, A., Sugihara, M. (2009). Choreographic methods
for creating novel, high quality dance. In L.-L. Chen, L. Feijs, M. Hessler, S. Kyffin,
P. L. Liu, K. Overbeeke & B. Young (Eds.), Proceedings of the 5th International
Workshop on Design and Semantics of Form and Movement (DeSForM). (pp. 188-
195). Taipei: National Taiwan University of Science.
Krasnow, D. (1997). C-I Training: The merger of conditioning and imagery as an alternative
training methodology for dance. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 12, 3-8.
Krasnow, D., Wilmerding, M. V., Stecyk, S., Wyon, M., Koutedakis, Y. (2011).
Biomechanical Research in Dance: A Literature Review. Medical Problems of
Performing Artists, 26(1), 3-23.
Laws, K. (1995). The physics and aesthetics of vertical movements in dance. Medical
Problems of Performing Artists, 14, 41-47.
Laws, K. (1998). Momentum transfer in dance movement. Medical Problems of Performing
Artists, 14, 136-145.
Neurocognitive control in dance 33
Laws, K., & Petrie, C. (1999). Momentum transfer in dance movement: vertical jumps.
Medical Problems in Performing Artists, 14(3), 138-140.
Lepelley, M. C., Thullier, F., Koral, J., & Lestienne, F. G. (2006). Muscle coordination in
complex movements during Jete in skilled ballet dancers. Experimental Brain
Research, 175(2), 321-331.
Maduell, M., & Wing, A. M. (2007). The dynamics of ensemble: The case for flamenco.
Psychology of Music, 35(4), 591-627.
May, J., Calvo-Merino, B., deLahunta, S., McGregor, W., Cusack, R., Owen, A., et al. (2011).
Points in mental space: an interdisciplinary study of imagery in movement creation.
Dance Research Electronic, forthcoming.
Minvielle-Moncla, J., Audiffren, M., Macar, F., & Vallet, C. (2008). Overproduction timing
errors in expert dancers. Journal of Motor Behavior, 40(4), 291-300.
Miura, N., Sugiura, M., Takahashi, M., Sassa, Y., Miyamoto, A., Sato, S., et al. (2010). Effect
of motion smoothness on brain activity while observing a dance: an fMRI study using
a humanoid robot. Social Neuroscience, 5(1), 40-58.
Munzert, J., Zentgraf, K., Stark, R., & Vaitl, D. (2008). Neural activation in cognitive motor
processes: comparing motor imagery and observation of gymnastic movements.
Experimental Brain Research, 188(3), 437-444.
Nordin, S. M., & Cumming, J. (2007) Where, when, and how: A quantitative account of
dance imagery. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78(4), 390-395.
Opacic, T., Stevens, C., & Tillmann, B. (2009). Unspoken knowledge: Implicit learning of
structured human dance movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 35(6), 1570-1577.
Orgs, G., Dombrowski, J. H., Heil, M., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Expertise in dance
modulates alpha/beta event-related desynchronization during action observation.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 27(12), 3380-3384.
Parsons, L. M. (1987). Imagined spatial transformation of one's body. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 116(2), 172-191.
Perrin, P., Deviterne, D., Hugel, F., & Perrot, C. (2002). Judo, better than dance, develops
sensorimotor adaptabilities involved in balance control. Gait & Posture, 15(2), 187-
194.
Phillips-Silver, J., Toiviainen, P., Gosselin N., Piche, N., Nozaradan, S., Palmer, C., &
Peretiz, I. (2011). Born to dance but beat deaf: a new form of congenital amusia.
Neuropsychologia, forthcoming.
Neurocognitive control in dance 34
Ramsay, J. R., & Riddoch, M. J. (2001). Position-matching in the upper limb: professional
ballet dancers perform with outstanding accuracy. Clinical Rehabilitation, 15(3), 324-
330.
Rein, S., Fabian, T., Zwipp, H., Rammelt, S., & Weindel, S. (2011) Postural control and
functional ankle stability in professional and amateur dancers. Clinical
Neurophysiology, forthcoming.
Ricotti, L., & Ravaschio, A. (2011). Break dance significantly increases static balance in 9
years-old soccer players. Gait & Posture 33(3), 462-465.
Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror
circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(4),
264-274.
Rossi-Arnaud, C. M., Cortese, A., & Cestari, V. (2004). Memory span for movement
configurations: the effects of concurrent verbal, motor and visual interference. Current
Psychology of Cognition, 22, 335-349.
Sawada, M., Suda, K., & Ishii, M. (2003). Expression of emotions in dance: relation between
arm movement characteristics and emotion. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 97(3 Pt 1),
697-708.
Schmit, J. M., Regis, D. I., & Riley, M. A. (2005). Dynamic patterns of postural sway in
ballet dancers and track athletes. Experimental Brain Research, 163(3), 370-378.
Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science,
171(972), 701-703.
Smyth, M. M., & Pendleton, L. R. (1990). Space and movement in working memory.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 42(2), 291-304.
Smyth, M. M., & Pendleton, L. R. (1994). Memory for movement in professional ballet
dancers. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 25, 282-94.
Starkes, J. L., Caicco, M., Boutilier, C., & Sevsek, B. (1990). Motor recall of experts for
structured and unstructured sequences in creative modern dance. Journal of Sport &
Exercise Psychology, 12, 317-321.
Starkes, J. L., Deakin, J. M., Lindley, S., & Crisp, F. (1987). Motor versus verbal recall of
ballet sequences by young expert dancers. Journal of Sport Psychology, 9, 222-230.
Starosta, W. (2000). Genetic or social determination of the direction of turns during physical
exercises. Kineziologija, 18, 33-40.
Stehle, E. (1997). Performance and Gender in Ancient Greece: nondramatic poetry in its
setting. Princeton University Press.
Neurocognitive control in dance 35
Stevens, C., Ginsborg, J., & Lester, G. (2011). Backwards and forwards in space and time:
recalling dance movement from long-term memory. Memory Studies, 4, 234-250.
Stevens, C., Malloch, S., McKechnie, S., & Steven, N. (2003). Choreographic cognition: The
time-course and phenomenology of creating a dance. Pragmatics & Cognition, 11(2),
297-326.
Stevens, C. & McKechnie, S. (2005). Thinking in action: thought made visible in
contemporary dance. Cognitive Processing, 6, 243–252.
Stevens, C., Schubert, E., Wang, S., Kroos, C., & Halovic, S. (2009). Moving with and
without music: scaling and lapsing in time in the performance of contemporary dance.
Music Perception, 26(5), 451-464.
Stevens, C., Winskel, H., Howell, C., Vidal, L. M., Latimer, C., & Milne-Home, J. (2010).
Perceiving dance: schematic expectations guide experts' scanning of a contemporary
dance film. Journal of Dance Medicine and Science, 14(1), 19-25.
Sugano, A., & Laws, K. (2002). Physical analysis as a foundation for pirouette training.
Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 17(1).
Thullier, F., & Moufti, H. (2004). Multi-joint coordination in ballet dancers. Neuroscience
Letters, 369(1), 80-84.
Toiviainen, P., Luck, G., & Thompson, M. (2010). Embodied meter: hierarchical eigenmodes
in music-induced movement. Music Perception, 28(1), 59-70.
Weigelt, M., Steggemann, Y., Bläsing, B., & Schack, T. (2008). Über die Wahrnehmung
menschlicher Figuren: eine Untersuchung zum Zusammenhang von
Bewegungsexpertise und mentaler Rotation. In P. Khader, K. Jost, H. Lachnit & F.
Rosler (Eds.), Experimentelle Psychologie. Lengerich: Papst Science Publishers.
Wilson, M., Lim, B., & Kwon, Y. (2004). 3-Dimensional kinematic analysis of grand rond de
jambe en l’air: skilled versus novice dancers. Journal of Dance Medicine and Science,
8, 108-115.
Yarrow, K., Brown, P., & Krakauer, J. W. (2009). Inside the brain of an elite athlete: the
neural processes that support high achievement in sports. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 10(8), 585-596.