0% found this document useful (0 votes)
20 views

Study Pointers (Working Memory)

This paper is my own study pointers of the topic working memory in psychology of languages
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
20 views

Study Pointers (Working Memory)

This paper is my own study pointers of the topic working memory in psychology of languages
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as DOCX, PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 6

Lexical Access and Sentence Processing

Understanding language involves two core cognitive processes: lexical access (how we
recognize and retrieve words from memory) and sentence processing (how we parse and
understand the structure and meaning of sentences). Both processes are essential for fluent
communication and comprehension.

Lexical Access
Lexical access refers to the process by which a listener or reader retrieves a word from their
mental lexicon (the mental dictionary of stored words) based on auditory or visual input. This
involves recognizing the word, understanding its meaning, and integrating it into the current
context.
Key Steps in Lexical Access
1. Word Recognition
2. Lexical Retrieval
3. Contextual Integration

1. Word Recognition and Lexical Access


Word recognition is the initial step of lexical access, where the listener or reader identifies the
word from sensory input, such as hearing a spoken word or seeing it in print.
 Phonological Processing: For spoken language, listeners break down the speech stream
into phonemes and syllables and compare them to known sound patterns stored in
memory.
 Orthographic Processing: In written language, readers recognize words by identifying
letters or clusters of letters and matching them to their mental representation.
 Activation of Multiple Candidates: When recognizing a word, multiple possible word
candidates are activated in the brain. The correct word is selected based on context and
additional linguistic cues.
2. Lexical Retrieval
Once the word is recognized, the next step is lexical retrieval, which involves accessing the
meaning, grammatical properties, and phonological information stored in the lexicon.
 Semantic Access: The brain retrieves the meaning of the word, allowing it to be
integrated into the overall understanding of the sentence.
 Syntactic Properties: Along with meaning, the brain retrieves the syntactic properties of
the word, such as its grammatical role (e.g., noun, verb).
 Phonological Information: In spoken word production, the brain retrieves the correct
sequence of phonemes and syllables for articulation.
3. Contextual Integration
After retrieving the word, the final step in lexical access is integrating it into the current context,
which involves making sense of the word based on the sentence or discourse.
 Disambiguation through Context: Words often have multiple meanings, and context
helps select the appropriate one.
 Priming: The context can also prime certain words, making it easier to recognize and
retrieve related terms.
Sentence Processing
Sentence processing refers to the way we understand and interpret sentences by combining
words into meaningful structures.
Key Processes in Sentence Processing
1. Sentence Parsing
2. Syntactic Ambiguity
3. The Role of Working Memory
4. Top-Down and Bottom-Up Processing

1. Sentence Parsing and Processing


Sentence parsing is the process of assigning a grammatical structure to a string of words.
 Immediate Parsing: As soon as a word is encountered, the brain begins assigning it a
role in the sentence. This happens incrementally, meaning that we interpret sentences
word by word rather than waiting until the sentence is complete.
 Syntactic Structure Building: The brain uses grammatical rules to group words into
meaningful units. In English, for example, we expect a subject-verb-object structure for
declarative sentences.
2. Syntactic Ambiguity
Some sentences can be ambiguous because they can be parsed in more than one way, leading to
temporary confusion until additional information resolves the ambiguity. This is known as
syntactic ambiguity.
 Garden Path Sentences: These are sentences that initially lead the reader or listener
toward one interpretation, only to require reanalysis later. They often create temporary
syntactic ambiguity.
 Attachment Ambiguities: These occur when a phrase could attach to more than one part
of the sentence.
3. The Role of Working Memory
Working memory plays a crucial role in sentence processing, especially for longer and more
complex sentences. It helps store and manage linguistic information temporarily while we parse
and interpret sentences.
 Memory Load in Complex Sentences: Sentences with embedded clauses or multiple
phrases increase the working memory load, making them harder to process.
 Chunking: To make sentence processing easier, the brain often groups words or phrases
into manageable "chunks."
4. Top-Down and Bottom-Up Processing
Sentence processing involves both bottom-up and top-down approaches.
 Bottom-Up Processing: This involves processing linguistic input from the smallest units
(phonemes or morphemes) up to larger structures (phrases and sentences). It is data-
driven and relies on the actual words and sounds.
Example: In bottom-up processing, you hear the sound /kæt/ and recognize it as the word
"cat," then build the meaning based on the following words.
 Top-Down Processing: This relies on prior knowledge, context, and expectations to
interpret sentences. It allows the brain to fill in missing information or anticipate
upcoming words.
Example: If someone begins a sentence with "At the zoo, I saw a…," you expect the next
word to be an animal name, such as "lion" or "elephant."

The Role of Memory in Language


Memory plays a crucial role in language comprehension and learning. Two primary types of
memory are involved in these processes: working memory, which deals with real-time language
comprehension and processing, and long-term memory, which underlies language acquisition,
vocabulary growth, and the retention of linguistic knowledge. Both types of memory work in
tandem to support fluent language use.

1. Working Memory and Language Comprehension


Working memory (WM) refers to a temporary storage system that holds and processes
information during cognitive tasks such as reasoning, learning, and comprehension.
Key Components of Working Memory in Language
1. Phonological Loop
2. Central Executive
3. Visuospatial Sketchpad

Phonological Loop
The phonological loop is a sub-system of working memory responsible for temporarily storing
verbal information.
 Verbal Storage: The phonological loop can hold a sequence of sounds (e.g., words,
syllables) for a few seconds, allowing the listener to maintain speech input long enough
for comprehension.
 Subvocal Rehearsal: This process involves silently repeating words in your mind,
refreshing the stored information to prevent it from fading.
Central Executive
The central executive is the component that manages and coordinates various cognitive
processes, including attention, inhibition, and task switching. It also directs the flow of
information between the phonological loop and long-term memory.
 Managing Syntactic Structures: During sentence comprehension, the central executive
organizes how different parts of a sentence (e.g., subjects, objects, verbs) fit together into
a grammatical whole.
 Integrating Context: The central executive also allows for the integration of prior
knowledge and context into language comprehension. It helps monitor coherence as new
information is added.
Visuospatial Sketchpad
Although primarily involved in processing visual information, the visuospatial sketchpad can
also contribute to language comprehension, especially when language relates to spatial concepts
or objects. For example, understanding prepositions like "above," "next to," or "under" often
involves a spatial understanding.

2. Long-Term Memory and Language Learning


While working memory supports immediate comprehension, long-term memory (LTM) stores
the linguistic knowledge that individuals accumulate over time. This includes vocabulary,
grammatical rules, and knowledge of language use.
Key Components of Long-Term Memory in Language Learning
1. Declarative Memory
2. Procedural Memory
3. Semantic Memory
4. Episodic Memory

Declarative Memory
Declarative memory is responsible for the storage of explicit facts and knowledge, including
language-related information such as vocabulary, facts about language, and rules of grammar.
 Vocabulary Learning: As we encounter new words, they are stored in declarative
memory. This memory system allows us to recall word meanings and definitions when
needed.
 Grammatical Rules: Knowledge of syntax and morphology is also stored in declarative
memory. This system allows speakers to apply rules such as verb conjugation (e.g., past
tense “walked”) or pluralization (e.g., “cats”).
Procedural Memory
Procedural memory involves the acquisition of skills and habits, often without conscious
awareness.
 Grammar in Action: Procedural memory enables automatic application of grammatical
rules, such as sentence structuring and word order, without needing to think about them
explicitly.
 Speaking Fluency: Over time, repetitive use of language forms (e.g., verb conjugations,
sentence structures) becomes ingrained in procedural memory, leading to fluent and
automatic speech.
Semantic Memory
Semantic memory is a subtype of declarative memory that deals with knowledge of facts and
concepts, including word meanings and general knowledge about the world. It plays a critical
role in language learning.
 Word Meanings: Semantic memory stores the meanings of words, which are retrieved
when needed for comprehension and communication.
 Conceptual Knowledge: Beyond individual words, semantic memory also stores broader
concepts that help with language use.
Episodic Memory
Episodic memory refers to the storage of personal experiences and events.
 Contextual Learning: Words and phrases learned in specific contexts (e.g., during
conversations, at school, or in travel experiences) are stored in episodic memory, which
helps recall the language in similar situations.
1. Vocabulary Acquisition: When learning a new language, long-term memory stores
words and their meanings.
2. Grammar Learning: The rules of a language, such as verb conjugation or word order,
are stored in long-term memory.
3. Language Proficiency: The more words, phrases, and structures are stored in long-term
memory, the more proficient a person becomes in both understanding and producing
language.

Components of the Phonological Loop


The phonological loop is typically divided into two subcomponents:
Phonological Store (aka "inner ear"): This holds speech-based information for a brief
duration (about 1-2 seconds) and is responsible for retaining sounds in memory.
Articulatory Rehearsal Process (aka "inner voice"): This involves subvocal rehearsal,
which refreshes the stored information by mentally repeating it, preventing it from fading
from memory.

Importance of the Phonological Loop


The phonological loop plays a central role in:

Language Acquisition: Especially in children, the phonological loop helps them retain
and rehearse new words, making it easier to acquire vocabulary and language skills.


Second Language Learning: For adults learning a second language, the phonological
loop helps retain unfamiliar sounds and words long enough for them to be processed,
rehearsed, and eventually stored in long-term memory.


Verbal Reasoning and Problem-Solving: Many cognitive tasks that require mental
manipulation of language (e.g., solving verbal puzzles, making inferences, or forming
complex sentences) rely on the phonological loop to hold and process linguistic
information in real time.
The visuospatial sketchpad is a component of working memory that is responsible for
temporarily holding and processing visual and spatial information. It allows us to create mental
images, understand spatial relationships, and navigate through our environment. The visuospatial
sketchpad works alongside other parts of working memory, such as the phonological loop, but it
specializes in dealing with non-verbal, visual, and spatial data.
Components of the Visuospatial Sketchpad
1.
Visual Cache: Stores information about the form and color of objects (e.g., what things
look like).
Inner Scribe: Deals with spatial and movement information. It tracks the arrangement of
objects in space and helps guide physical navigation.

Importance of the Visuospatial Sketchpad



Everyday Navigation: It plays a key role in our ability to navigate through
environments, whether it’s remembering how to get to a destination or avoiding obstacles
while walking.


Spatial Reasoning: Tasks like arranging furniture, solving puzzles, or even packing
efficiently rely on the visuospatial sketchpad to mentally manipulate objects and predict
outcomes.


Learning and Problem Solving: Many learning activities, especially those involving
geometry, maps, and graphs, require the visuospatial sketchpad to understand and
manipulate spatial relationships.

In summary, the visuospatial sketchpad is essential for processing and storing visual and spatial
information, enabling us to navigate, remember visual details, manipulate objects mentally, and
engage in various spatial tasks.

You might also like