Suprasegmental Phonology
Suprasegmental Phonology
Phonology
Compiled By
Dr. Amaal Tayea
English Department
Ain Shams University
0
Table of Contents
3. Word Stress 28
6. Tone Unit 70 68
8. Bibliography 93
1
Preface
2
Consonant and Vowel Sounds
of English
The consonant phonemes of Received
Pronunciation
Table 1 presents the IPA symbols for the 24 consonant phonemes
of RP.
TABLE 1: THE INTERNATIONAL PHONETIC ALPHABET
SYMBOLS FOR THE 24 CONSONANT PHONEMES OF THE
RECEIVED PRONUNCIATION ACCENT OF ENGLISH
Consonant phonemes with doubled spellings* which are rare in one-syllable words
Consonant phonemes with doubled spellings* which are regular at the end of one-syllable words
after a short vowel phoneme spelt with one letter
3
/s/ as in the first sound of sue /suː/ voiceless
4
The Vowel Phonemes
5
individual speakers will differ in their production.
Individual authorities will also differ slightly in exactly where
all the vowels are positioned on the grid. In the diagram, for
the sake of clarity, we have not allowed the sounds to overlap
but /i/ and /iː/ are formed in the same way and are
differentiated only by length and the same can be said for /ə/
and /ɜː/.
If you try saying beat, bit, bet, boot, verse, cup, cap, the, noose,
foot, hot, fought, bark you will feel the tongue position change
from left to right, top to bottom of the grid. It'll also move up and
down and forward and back depending on the vowel. Try it.
The vowels in those words are:
In that list is one vowel, /ə/ which is the first vowel sound in the
word about and the last in the word father. The example above
is the vowel sound in an unstressed version of the which occurs
before a consonant as in, e.g., the man. As you can see, it lies in
the centre of the diagram. It is the most common vowel in
6
English.
We can also add a 13th vowel, /i/, which is the sound of the letter
'y' at the end of party. It is a shortened version of /iː/. Some will
not recognise this sound as a distinct vowel, conflating it with /iː/
and it is true that speakers differ in how long they make the
sound and it is also true that both sounds are high, front vowels
formed with the tongue in an identical position.
Short Long
Cap cab
7
catch cadge
Safe save
mace maze
Lock log
Cart card
8
vii. /ə/ as in the first syllable of about and the last
of father.
viii. /i/ as at the end of happy or savvy
b. Long vowels
The following are the long vowels which are all marked
to show length with the length mark: 'ː'. The length
mark is actually only for convenience in terms of
remembering that the vowel is long. The symbols are,
with one exception, different from the short vowels even
without the length mark. The following five are the long
vowels:
i. /iː/ as in sheep or neat. It is the lengthened
version of /i/ and /ɪ/.
ii. /ɜː/ as in verse or nurse. It is the lengthened
version of /ə/ and is sometimes transcribed as /əː/.
iii. /ɑː/ as in car or bar.
iv. /ɔː/ as in taught or bought. It is the lengthened
form of /ɒ/ and that vowel is sometimes transcribed
as /ɔ/.
v. /uː/ as in moose or shoe. It is the lengthened form
of /ʊ/ and that vowel is sometimes transcribed as
/u/.
3. Lip rounding: some vowels are formed in English with
rounded lips. These are /ʊ/, /uː/, /ɔː/ and /əʊ/. Try looking
in a mirror and saying, foot, loose, caught, load. You can see
9
the lips rounding. The amount of rounding varies and this
affects the sound. Other vowels, such as /e/ and /iː/ are not
rounded but the latter requires lateral stretching of the mouth
– say cheese.
There are three alternatives, although there is a cline from
fully rounded to fully stretched via the neutral position. If you
look at yourself in a mirror while pronouncing the vowels in
the table above, you will be able to see how your lips are
positioned for each sound:
a. rounding as in /ʊ/ or /ɔː/ etc.
b. stretching as in /iː/ or /e/ etc.
c. neutral as in /ə/ or /ɜː/ etc.
10
minim
Vow heig positi leng roundedne
al
el ht on th ss
pairs
shor
/ʊ/ high back rounded full /
t
fool
/uː/ high back long rounded
shor errand
/e/ mid front neutral
t /
centra
/ɜː/ mid long neutral
l hurt /
shor hot
/ɒ/ mid back rounded
t
11
centra shor
/ʌ/ low neutral
l t cup /
shor cap
/æ/ low front neutral
t
The /i/ sound appears in this table but does not form part of a
minimal pair except when it is contrasted with /ɪ/ or /iː/.
Diphthongs
As we said, these are sounds made by starting with one pure vowel
and gliding towards another. There are eight of these in
English. The easiest way to remember them is to see where they
are going. There are three sorts:
12
i. /ɪə/ as in here or beer. The sound starts with the short
vowel /ɪ/ and glides at the end to the /ə/ sound.
ii. /eə/ as in lair or pair. Start with /e/ and move to the
/ə/.
iii. /ʊə/ as the first vowel in during or (in some people's
production) pour. Start with /ʊ/ and move to /ə/.
This sound is now becoming quite rare, being confined
mostly to dialect or very careful RP speech. These days,
the vowel on words such as sure and poor is usually
transcribable as /ɔː/. It is more often present in longer
words such as individual (/ˌɪn.dɪ.ˈvɪ.dʒʊəl/).
2. Ending in /ɪ/. These are described as closing diphthongs
because the tongue moves towards the roof of the mouth,
closing off the airflow. There are also three of these:
i. /eɪ/ as in day or hay. Start with /e/ and move to /ɪ/.
ii. /aɪ/ as in price or nice. Start with a shortened /aː/ and
move to /ɪ/.
iii. /ɔɪ/ as in boy or coy. Start with /ɔ/ and move to /ɪ/. In
this diphthong, the first sound is a little more open and
shorter than the sound /ɔː/ in bought or caught.
3. Ending in /ʊ/. These are also closing diphthongs because the
tongue moves towards the roof of the mouth. There are only
two (in English):
i. /əʊ/ as in boat or vote. Start with /ə/ and move to /ʊ/.
13
ii. /aʊ/ as in south or louse. Start with a shortened /aː/
and move to /ʊ/.
14
words cot and caught, while in other dialects these words
are homophones. People who study the differences between
the dialects of English often study the different way vowel
sounds are pronounced.
15
16
EThe Basics on Syllabic Structure
A syllable (σ) is a phonological unit of sonority. Sonority can be
described by the degree of airflow obstruction and voicing that
occurs during phonation. Sonority is inversely correlated with
constriction of the articulators in the oral cavity. Sonorous sounds
have a more ‘sing-able’ quality, that is they are more prominent in
amplitude and length than less sonorous sounds. Sonority shows
the resonance of one sound segment in relation to another.
Rime
17
[+son] or obstruents [-son].
(2)
Note that:
1. Voiced obstruents rank higher in sonority than their voiceless
counterparts.
18
2. Round vowels are ranked higher in sonority than their
unrounded counterparts.
Syllable Structure
When we represent syllable structure as in (1), the nucleus and
coda are right-branching forming the ‘rime.’ This type of
branching is the most common across languages. However, there
are some languages, such as Japanese and Korean, for which a
case has been made for a left-branching structure (4) in which
the onset and nucleus would form a ‘body.’
Body
All languages require syllable nuclei. In fact, the nucleus is the only
universally obligatory component of a syllable. In most languages
onsets are preferred yet optional – although they are required in
some languages. No language requires codas. In most languages
19
codas are optional, and they are restricted or even prohibited in
others.
Onset
CVC
The onset is the beginning of a syllable boundary, and is the
strongest consonantal position. The onset is required in many
languages and is optional or restricted in others. Onsets are almost
always preferred to codas; when a C is found intervocalically it will
be parsed to the subsequent onset rather than the previous coda.
20
For instance, a CVCV structure would be syllabified as CV.CV, and
not as CVC.V.
Nucleus
CVC
Nuclei are generally filled by vowels.
A vowel cannot fill any position of a syllable besides the nucleus. If
a vowel were to be in either the onset or coda position, the nucleus
would be required to contain a sound more sonorous than a vowel
in order to not violate the SSP. However, there are no sounds more
sonorous than vowels. If a vowel is present, it must be in the
nucleus of the syllable.
Every vowel will fill a nucleus, but not every nucleus will be filled
by a vowel. Some languages do not require a vowel in the nucleus
and instead permit certain consonants (C). Languages have
different rules for determining what is allowed to occupy the
nucleus position. In English, in addition to vowels, syllabic liquids
and nasals are permitted to fill the nucleus. Other languages that
permit non-vocalic content in their nuclei have far more complex
syllabification processes. In languages such as Nuxálk (Bella Coola)
(5a) and Berber (5b) there exist entire words and phrases without
vowels. They require much more complex algorithms to become
syllabified.
21
(5a) Nuxálk
[sxs] ‘seal blubber’
[xɬpʼχʷɬtʰɬpʰɬːskʷʰt͡sʼ] ‘He had in his possession a bunchberry plant’
(5b) Berber
[tftktst tfktstt] ‘you sprained it and then gave it‘
[rkkm] ‘rot’ (imperf.)
Coda
CVC
The coda is optional in most languages. In some languages, it is
restricted or even prohibited. Old Bulgarian does not permit codas,
and will therefore syllabify (parse) a structure such as CVCCCV to
CV.CCCV. On the other hand, some languages such as Persian do
not permit complex onsets but do allow complex codas. Persian
speakers would most likely syllabify CVCCCV as CVCC.CV.
22
(6) English
[aj.skɹim] ‘I scream’
VG.CCVC
Q Q
/ \ / \
onset rime onset rime
/ / \
nucleus nucleus coda
[ aj skɹ i m ]
23
• The second consonant in the onset is called initial; this can
basically be any English consonant, especially if the onset only
consists of this initial consonant
• The third consonant in the onset is called post-initial; this is
exclusively represented by /l/, /r/, /w/ and /j/;
• ONSET: (pre-initial) - (initial) - (post-initial)
• The onset without consonant is called zero onset; obviously,
some combinations are preferable, some combinations of
consonants are impossible in English: *s3w *mr *hl (NB: /
/∫w/ is only present in the vowel name ‘schwa’ and the
brandname of the soft drinks, Schweppes: /∫w ə / , /∫weps/.)
The structure of the CODA: o the maximum number of the
consonants within the coda part is four; o the individual ‘slots’
bear the following names: pre-final consonants (only a small
set: /m/, /n/, /ŋ/, /l/, /s/); final consonants (basically any
English consonant – but combinations within consonant
clusters are limited); post-final consonants – again a small set
/s/, /z/, /t/, /d/, /θ/. More examples:
• fifths /fІfθ/ - 2 post-final consonants.
• next /nekst/ - 2 post-final consonants
• twelfths /twelfθs/ - 2 post-final consonants
• sixths /sІkθs/ - 3 post-final consonants.
• texts /teksts/ - 3 post-final consonants
24
• NOTE: if the peak (centre) of the syllable is not carried by a
vowel we need to consider a syllabic consonant /l/, /m/, /n/,
/ŋ/, /r/, eg. middle /midl/
• BECOME A MEMBER
Both /m/ and /ŋ/ can occur as syllabic, but only as a result
of processes such as assimilation and elision that are
introduced later. We find them sometimes in words like
‘happen’, which can be pronounced hæpm, though hæpn and
hæpən are equally acceptable, and ‘uppermost’, which could
be pronounced as ʌpməust, though ʌpməust, would be more
usual. Examples of possible syllabic velar nasals would be
‘broken key’ braukəŋ ki:, where the nasal consonant occurs
between velar consonants .
Ambisyllabicity
Sometimes breaking a consonant cluster will result in lexical
contrast, but other times it won’t. In the instances that lexical
contrast is not created, it can be difficult to decide where to parse
a syllable. In cases where parsing is not clear, medial Cs are
referred to as ambisyllabic. In (7), the [s]s would be considered
ambisyllabic.
(7) English
[æs.prin] vs. [æ.sprin] ‘aspirin’
[sis.təɹn] vs. [si.stərn] ‘cistern’
25
These different syllabifications and parsings can be considered
correct as they meet the constraints of their language, but the
differences in syllabification will affect the weights of the syllables.
Syllable Weight
Syllable weight is measured in ‘moras.’ A mora is a phonological
unit used to distinguish various syllable structures from one
another. Syllable weight is important because it can clarify issues
pertaining to language-specific constraints on syllable structure.
All syllables have at least one mora, which comes from the nucleus
of the syllable. In English all of the sound segments in the rime
contribute toward syllable weight. Therefore, CV and a CCCV
syllables have the same weight as well as number of moras
because the complexity of the onset has no bearing on how the
weight of the syllable is measured. When syllables are identified by
their weight, they are classified as light, heavy, or superheavy.
Light syllables have only one mora (i.e. a short vowel and no coda).
One example would be ‘me’ [mi] or CV.
26
Heavy syllables have two moras. The two moras could both be
found in the nucleus in the form of a long vowel or diphthong as in
‘my’ [maj], CVV or one mora could be found in the nucleus and the
other in the coda as in ‘mean’ [min], CVC.
27
Word Stress
i) Loudness
Most people seem to feel that stressed syllables are louder
than unstressed syllables; in other words, loudness is a
component of prominence. In a sequence of identical
syllables (e.g. ba:ba:ba:ba:), if one syllable is made louder
than the others, it will be heard as stressed. However, it is
important to realise that it is very difficult for a speaker to
make a syllable louder without changing other characteristics
of the syllable such as those explained below (ii-iv); if one
literally changes only the loudness, the perceptual effect is
not very strong.
29
ii) The length of syllables has an important part to play in
prominence. If one of the syllables in our “nonsense word”
ba:ba:ba:ba: is made longer than the others, there is quite
a strong tendency for that syllable to be heard as stressed.
iii) Pitch is a very important part of perceptual characteristic of
speech sound. Each syllable of the word is produced either
as low or high pitched. Stressed syllable is resulted as higher
pitch which makes it prominent. Every voiced syllable is said
on some pitch; pitch in speech is closely related to the
frequency of vibration of the vocal folds and to the musical
notion of low- and Sigh-pitched notes. It is essentially a
perceptual characteristic of speech. If one syllable of our
“nonsense word” is said with a pitch that is noticeably
different from that of the others, this will have a strong
tendency to produce the effect of prominence. For example,
if all syllables are said with low pitch except for one said with
high pitch, then the high-pitched syllable will be heard as
stressed and the others as unstressed. To place some
movement of pitch (e.g. rising or falling) on a syllable is even
more effective in making it sound prominent.
iv) Vowel quality
A syllable will tend to be prominent if it contains a vowel that
is different in quality from neighbouring vowels. If we change
one of the vowels in our “nonsense word” (e.g. ba:bi:ba:bai)
the “odd” syllable bi: will tend to be heard as stressed. This
30
effect is not very powerful, but there is one particular way in
which it is relevant in English: the previous chapter explained
how the most frequently encountered vowels in weak
syllables are i, ə , u (syllabic consonants are also common).
We can look on stressed syllables as occurring against a
“background” of these weak syllables, so that their
prominence is increased by contrast with these background
qualities. Prominence, then, is produced by four main
factors: (i) loudness, (ii) length, (iii) pitch and (iv) quality.
Generally these four factors work together in combination,
although syllables may sometimes be made prominent by
means of only one or two of them. Experimental work has
shown that these factors are not equally important; the
strongest effect is produced by pitch, and length is also a
powerful factor. Loudness and quality have much less effect.
Levels of stress
31
isolation, except for a few such as ‘yes’, ‘no ’, ‘possibly ’, ‘please’ a
and interrogative word s such as ‘what’, ‘who ’, etc. However,
looking at word s in isolation does help us to see stress placement
and stress levels more clearly than studying them in the context of
continuous speech.
32
Unstressed
Placement of stress
Roach divides the languages of the world into two groups on the
basis of placement of the stress. The first group contains languages
which have fixed place for the stress in words like, French language
in which the last syllable is usually stressed. The second group
contains languages in which there is no fixed place for stress in
words. For example, in English language there is a difficulty to
predict the placement of word stress (2009:76).
Simple words
1. One word has only one stress. (One word cannot have
two stresses. If you hear two stresses, you hear two words.
Two stresses cannot be one word. It is true that there can
be a "secondary" stress in some words. But a secondary
stress is much smaller than the main [primary] stress, and
is only used in long words.)
Here are some more, rather complicated, rules that can help you
understand where to put the stress. But do not rely on them too
33
much, because there are many exceptions. It is better to try to
"feel" the music of the language and to add the stress naturally.
34
Two-syllable words
'bɒrəu). If the final syllable is strong, then that syllable is stressed even if the first
Nouns require a different rule: stress will fall on the first syllable
unless the first syllable is weak and the second syllable is strong.
35
Thus: ‘money’ 'mʌni ‘divan’ di'vn ‘product’ 'prɒdʌkt ‘balloon’ bə'lu:n
‘larynx’ 'læriŋks ‘design’ di'zain Other two-syllable words such as
adverbs seem to behave like verbs and adjectives
Three-syllable words
36
produce stress patterns such as: ‘opportune’ 'opɒtju:n ‘insolent’
'insələnt ‘derelict’ 'derəlikt ‘anthropoid’ 'ænθrəpɔɪd
Written exercises
1 Verbs
a) protect
b) clamber
c) festoon
d) detest
e) bellow
f) menace
g) disconnect
h) enter
37
Complex words
38
the distinction between “simple” and “complex” words is difficult to
draw. Complex words are of two major types:
i) words made from a basic word form (which we will call the
stem), with the addition of an affix; and
ii) compound words, which are made of two (or occasionally
more) independent English words (e.g. ‘ice cream’,
‘armchair’)
We will look first at the words made with affixes. Affixes are of
two sorts in English:
prefixes, which come before the stem (e.g. prefix ‘un-’ + stem
‘pleasant’ —> ‘unpleasant’) and suffixes, which come after the
stem (e.g. stem ‘good’ + suffix ‘-ness’ —> ‘goodness’). Affixes
have one of three possible effects on word stress:
The affix itself receives the primary stress (e.g. ‘semi-’ + ‘circle’
S3:kl —> ‘semicircle’ 'serms3:kl; ‘-ality’ + ‘person’ 'p3:sn —>
‘personality’ p3:sn'ael3ti). ii) The word is stressed as if the affix
were not there (e.g. ‘pleasant’ 'pleznt, ‘unpleasant’
An'pleznt;‘market’ 'm aikit,‘marketing’ 'maikitirj). iii) The stress
remains on the stem, not the affix, but is shifted to a different
syllable (e.g. ‘magnet’ 'masgnat, ‘magnetic’ maeg'netik).
Suffixes
39
primary stress is on the first syllable of the suffix. If the stem
consists of more than one syllable there will be a secondary
stress on one of the syllables of the stem. This cannot fall on the
last syllable of the stem and is, if necessary, moved to an earlier
syllable. For example, in ‘Japan’ d33'paen the primary stress is
on the last syllable, but when we add the stress-carrying suffix
‘-ese’ the primary stress is on the suffix and the secondary stress
is placed not on the second syllable but on the first: ‘Japanese’
,dʒæpə'ni:z.
40
-like’: ‘bird’ 'b3:d; ‘birdlike’ 'b3:dlaik
41
‘-ial’: ‘proverb’ 'prɒv3:b; ‘proverbial’ prə'v3:biəl
Finally, when the suffixes ‘-ance’, ‘-ant’ and ‘-ary’ are attached
to single-syllable stems, the stress is almost always placed on
the stem (e.g. ‘guidance’, ‘sealant’, ‘dietary’). When the stem
has more than one syllable, the stress is on one of the syllables
in the stem. To explain this, we need to use a rule based on
syllable structure, as was done for simple words in the previous
chapter. If the final syllable of the stem is strong, that syllable
receives the stress. For example: ‘importance’ im'pɔ:tns,
‘centenary’ sen'tinri. Otherwise the syllable before the last one
receives the stress: ‘inheritance’ in'heritəns, ‘military’ 'militri
Prefixes We will look only briefly at prefixes. Their effect on
stress does not have the comparative regularity, independence
and predictability of suffixes, and there is no prefix of one or two
syllables that always carries primary stress. Consequently, the
best treatment seems to be to say that stress in words with
prefixes is governed by the same rules as those for polysyllabic
words without prefixes.
42
So far as stress is concerned, the question is quite simple. When
is primary stress placed on the first constituent word of the
compound and when on the second? Both patterns are found. A
few rules can be given, although these are not completely
reliable. Perhaps the most familiar type of compound is the one
which combines two nouns and which normally has the stress on
the first element, as in: ‘typewriter’ 'taipraitə ‘car ferry’ 'ka:feri
‘sunrise’ 'sʌnraiz ‘suitcase’ 'su:tkeis ‘teacup’ 'ti:kʌp It is probably
safest to assume that stress will normally fall in this way on other
compounds; however, a number of compounds receive stress
instead on the second element. The first 86 English Phonetics
and Phonology words in such compounds often have secondary
stress. For example, compounds with an adjectival first element
and the -ed morpheme at the end have this pattern (given in
spelling only): .bad- 'tempered .half- 'timbered .heavy- 'handed
Compounds in which the first element is a number in some form
also tend to have final stress: .three- 'wheeler .second-'class
.five-'finger Compounds functioning as adverbs are usually final-
stressed: .head'first .North-'East .down'stream Finally,
compounds which function as verbs and have an adverbial first
element take final stress: .down'grade .back-'pedal .ill-'treat
43
Variable stress
44
Word-class pairs
45
object 'ɒbd3ekt (N) әb'd3ekt (V)
Compound words
46
/ bed-ˈtempəd /. Roach (2009:86) also suggests another rules for
compound stress. These rules are : compound words have primary
stress on the second word if the first word is number as in (second
class) / sekənd 'klɑːs / ; compound words have a primary stress on
the second word if the compounds function as adverbs as in (north-
East)/ nɔːθ 'iːst / ; finally, when compounds function as verb and
has an adverb, then stress is on the second word as in the example
(downgrade) / daʊn'greɪd /. It is good to note that Collins and Mess
( 2013:132-133) mentions other rules concerning this point. They
are : the stress is on the second element when the compound
words refer to name of towns as seen in (New York) / njuː jɔːk / ;
the stress is on the second word when the compounds refer to the
part of building as in (back door) / bæk dɔː / ; finally, the names of
magazines and newspapers have a primary stress on the second
word as in ( Daily post) / deɪli 'pəʊst /. 3.3 Sentence stress Unlike
word stress, sentence stress has flexibility in the placement of
stress. Kenworthy ( 1987:32) concludes that there are several
ways in which the speaker is capable of sending ideas or important
information by focusing the main stress of the sentence on specific
words. However, the placement of sentence stress is affected by
the classes of the word. In this regard, Collins and Mess
(2013:135) list two type of words: function words and content
words. Function words include ( articles, prepositions, pronouns,
conjunctions and auxiliary verbs). They are normally unstressed.
Content words are those which carry the meaning of the sentence
47
such as (nouns, main verbs, adjective and adverbs). Those content
words are normally stressed. Example: I've 'heard that 'Jack and
'Jane 'spent their 'holidays in 'Jamaica (Collins and Mess, ibid) In
the sentence above, the content words heard, Jack, Jane, spent,
holidays, and Jamaica) are stressed (strong) whereas the function
words ( I, have, that, and, their and in) are normally unstressed
(weak). However, there are exceptions to the rules mentioned
above specially what is concerned with the stress of function words.
Roach ( 2009:90-91) indicates that those function words are
normally stressed in certain circumstances. They are : 1. Many
function words are stressed when they occur at the end of the
sentence : Chips are what I'm fond of. / 'ʧɪps ə 'wɒt aɪm 'fᴐnd 'ɒv /
2. When function words show a contrast : The letters from him not
to him. / ðə ˈlɛtəz frəm ɪm nɒt 'tə ɪm / 3. When function words are
emphasized : You must give me more money. / jʊ 'mʌst gɪv mi mɔː
ˈmʌni / 4. When the function word is being "cited" or " quoted":
You shouldn't put "and" at the end of a sentence. / jʊ 'ʃʊdnt 'pʊt
'ænd ət ði 'end əv ə ˈsentəns / 4. Stress and Meaning There is a
strong relationship between the placement of stress and the
meaning. However, Gill (n. d) mentions that there are words of
twosyllable in which the placement of stress does not only affect
their syntactic category, but their meaning as well, for example,
there is a huge difference in meaning between ('address vs.
add'ress ) and ('content vs. con'tent ). The difference of meaning
can appear when these words are put in the sentences as below :
48
One of the important functions of stress is to distinguish between
compounds and phrases (McMahon, 2002 :123) and (Ladefoged &
Johnson, 2011 :112 ). Compounds generally have a primary stress
on the first word while phrases have a primary stress on the second
word. The difference in stress placement gives a clear indication of
the meaning changes. Kushinka ( 2016 ) presents more than one
example to emphsize this fact. For the first example, there is a
semantic difference between 'greenhouse / 'gri:nhaʋs /, a
compound ( a building used to growing plants ) vs. green'house /
gri:n'haʋs / a phrase ( a house that its color is green ). The second
example includes 'Whitehouse / 'waıthaʋs/, a compound noun
which means ( the house where the U.S president lives ) which has
a different meaning from white'house / waıt'haʋs /, a phrase which
means ( the house that its color is green ). The last one is 'blackbird
/ 'blækbɜ:rd /, a compound ( a type of bird ) which has a different
meaning from black'bird / blæk'bɜ:rd / a phrase ( a bird that its
color is black ). Concerning sentence stress , some words seem to
be more prominent than others depending on the information the
speaker wants to convey. One Address / 'ædres /(n.) Do you know
Ali’s address? ( The name of the place ) / ə'dres / (v.) You are not
allowed to address the president. ( to direct speech to someone )
Content / 'kɒntent /(n.) The content of your essay is fine. ( what it
contains ) / kən'tent /(adj.) She was sitting reading a book , looking
very content. ( relaxed , peaceful ) of the difficult things to control
when learning English language is stress. Stressing different word
49
in the same sentence each time can completely change the
meaning of it. Commenting on the fact above, Reed & and Levis
(2015:178) argue that sentence stress is “ manipulated by the
speaker, and is strongly related to the structuring of information in
discourse ʼʼ . The information that is indented to be conveyed by
the speaker must match with what the listener has in his mind.
This view is supported by Reed & Levis (2015:178) who state that
“ the placement of sentence stress reflects what a speaker assumes
is in the consciousness of the hearer at the same time… ʼʼ. For
instance , Roach (2009:261) presents a sentence ( you didn’t say
anything about rates. ). Mustafa & faris explain this sentence in
four ways: 1. You didn’t say anything about rates. 2. You didn’t say
anything about rates. 3. You didn’t say anything about rates. 4.
You didn’t say anything about rates. The first utterance where the
main sentence stress is placed on ( you ) is not you but maybe
somebody else. The second utterance means that you didn’t want
to say. The third one refers that you didn’t say ( say ) not ( see or
hear ). The last one means that you didn’t say anything about (
rates ) not about models or colors ( 2011:805). Beare ( 2018 ) also
presents a sentence ( I don’t think he should get that job ) and he
explains it in seven ways:
1. I don’t think he should get that job. Meaning not ( I ) but maybe
Ali or Nada thinks about this idea.
5. I don’t think he should get that job. Meaning he must work hard
to get that job.
51
Exceptions to the Compounds
Stress Rule
(a) two-part place-names, such as Botany Bay, Buckingham
Palace, East Anglia, Los Angeles, Mount
Everest, New York, Niagara Falls, San Francisco. These include
street names, e.g. Blackberry Way, Fifth Avenue, London Road,
Mornington Crescent, Mulholland Drive, Penny Lane, Peyton Place,
Trafalgar Square. The only exception to this are large numbers of
street names ending in the word street: in my home town of
Edinburgh, London Road (where Road is most prominent) is not far
from London Street (where London is most prominent).
There are sets of compounds which systematically violate the
Compound Stress Rule. We now list these.
(b) compounds with a participial second element, ending in -ed, -
en or -ing. The -ed compounds are more common than the latter.
Many of these are based on parts of the body, and some of these
are more metaphorical than others. Examples are: ˌbare-ˈfaced,
ˌbig-ˈeared, ˌbig- ˈheaded, ˌbroken-ˈhearted, ˌcack-ˈhanded, ˌdim-
ˈwitted, ˌdouble-ˈjointed, ˌempty-ˈheaded, ˌeven-ˈhanded, ˌeven-
ˈtempered, ˌfaint-ˈhearted, ˌfair-ˈhaired, ˌfar-ˈsighted, ˌflat-
ˈchested, ˌflat-ˈfooted, ˌfleet-ˈfooted, ˌfoul-ˈmouthed, ˌgood-
ˈnatured, ˌhard-ˈnosed, ˌhigh-ˈpitched, ˌhot- ˈheaded, ˌill-
diˈsposed, ˌill-ˈtempered, ˌkind-ˈhearted,
ˌleft-ˈhanded, ˌlevel-ˈheaded, ˌlily-ˈlivered, ˌlimp- ˈwristed, ˌlong-
ˈhaired, ˌlong-ˈlegged, ˌlong-ˈwinded,
ˌnarrow-ˈshouldered, ˌold-ˈfashioned, ˌone-ˈeyed, ˌone- ˈlegged,
ˌpig-ˈheaded, ˌred-ˈhanded, ˌred-ˈheaded, ˌsqueaky-ˈvoiced,
ˌstrong-ˈminded, ˌstrong-ˈwilled, ˌthin- ˈlipped, ˌweak-ˈkneed,
ˌweak-ˈwilled, ˌwrong-ˈfooted, ˌwrong-ˈheaded, ˌclean-ˈshaven,
52
ˌlong-ˈproven, ˌsoft- ˈspoken, ˌclear-ˈthinking, ˌfar-ˈreaching,
ˌgood-ˈlooking.
(c) compounds in which the first part expresses what the object is
made from. Examples are: apple pie, brick wall, cotton socks, ham
sandwich, iron filings, paper napkin, pork pie, olive oil. Notice that
these are distinct from similar compounds in which the first part
does not express what the object is made from, as in paper clip,
which is not made from paper, cotton reel, which is not made from
cotton, and olive tree, which is not made of olives. Note that, in
American English, the first element can be the most prominent in
such compounds.
(d) compounds in which some kind of concrete or abstract
positioning is involved. Examples are: April showers, Christmas
break, evening meal, middle-class, second- rate, winter holiday.
(e) compounds which are two-part colour words.
Examples are: dark-green, deep-yellow, light-green, pale- blue.
(f) compounds derived from phrasal verbs. Examples are
compound nouns derived from phrasal verbs: chucker-out (from
chuck out), hanger-on (from hang on), passer-by (from pass by),
washing-up (from wash up); and compound adjectives derived
from phrasal verbs: finished- off (from finish-off), knocked-out,
pared-down (from pare down), rolled-up (from roll up), tired-out,
wiped-out, wrapped-up (from wrap up)
53
Word stress and the metrical foot
In each word, one of the feet is stronger than the other feet. Its
head is more prominent because it is assigned intonational tone
54
or extra length. This strong syllable has primary word stress and
the heads of the other feet have secondary stress.
55
Most words in English have one foot. Obviously all monosyllables
are one-footed, but so are also the large majority of two syllable
('pattern') and three-syllable ('Pamela') and even many four-
syllable words ('America'). However, many words also have two
feet: for example, 'imagination', 'orthodox', 'altitude'. One of
these feet is always stronger relative to the other and is
marked Fs (strong foot) as opposed to Fw (weak foot). The
strong foot always includes the primary stressed syllable while
the other weak foot (or feet) includes the syllable(s) with
secondary stress. In bipedal words, the order of the feet can be
either Fs Fw (i.e., with the strong foot first): these include e.g.
'altitude' and 'orthodox') or they can be Fw Fs (e.g.,
'chimpanzee', 'imagination'). There are a few long words with
three or more feet: these always have the strongest foot as the
last foot (e.g., 'reconciliation' which is Fw ('recon'), followed by
Fw ('cili') followed by Fs ('ation').
There are more than a few words in English that begin with a
weak syllable. Since feet are left-dominant, and since every foot
has to begin with a strong syllable, this will mean that a word-
56
initial weak syllable is unfooted (not associated with a foot).
Examples of such initial weak syllables occur in e.g. the first
syllable of 'America', 'medicinal', 'pedestrian'.
57
This near-alternation of s and w is the basis for our perception of
rhythm in English.
antidisestablishmentarianism
5 feet, 12 syllables
s w s w s w w sww s w
58
Stress patterns associated with the foot determine the
characteristic rhythm of spoken English. A foot can comprise just
a single word or a group of words. In English there are some
words that are generally unstressed. They are high frequency,
usually monosyllabic function words like "the, a, is, to, and, that".
These words can in exceptional circumstances be stressed for
particular semantic intent but generally speaking they remain
unstressed.
Quantity-sensitive feet
59
Heavy and light syllables
All other types of syllables - that is (C)VC syllables which are not
word-final, (C)VCC syllables, (C)V: syllables where V: is any other
vowel or diphthong not listed above, or (C)V:C syllables all count
as heavy.
In order to be able to work out the prosodic tree structure for any
word, it's obviously important to be able to identify which
syllables are strong and weak. This is fact quite easy because,
apart from all weak syllables necessarily being Light (see above),
the very large majority of weak syllables have a /ə/ vowel, or a
vowel that can reduce to schwa (for example, the second syllable
of 'minimum' which can be either /ɪ/ or /ə/). There are a few
other kinds of weak syllables that don't have a /ə/ as their vowel.
These are listed below:
60
• /iː/ in 'city', 'happy', 'very'. These are metrically weak
because in many accents (not Australian) they can be
reduced to quite a central vowel. But a clearer indication is
given by the realisation of /t/ in words like 'city': certainly in
American English, and increasingly in Australian English, it
can be produced as an alveolar flap which is voiced and
unaspirated (and weakly contacted with the roof of the
mouth). And since alveolar flaps can only ever occur in
unstressed syllables in English, the syllable in these words is
likely to be metrically weak.
• /əʉ/ in words like 'rainbow', 'shadow', 'window'. Word-final
/əʉ/ is metrically weak for the same reason as the /iː/ in
words like 'city' and 'happy' above. /əʉ/ is often reduced to a
centralised monophthong and /t/ can be produced as a flap
preceding word final /əʉ/ in words like 'ditto' and 'potato' in
some accents.
• /iː/ or /ɪ/ when it precedes /ə/ in words like 'Daniel',
'pedestrian'. This is certainly metrically weak both because it
is quite short in duration, and because it can often be
produced as a glide /j/, thus, /dænjəl/ is certainly a possible
two-syllable production of this word.
• /ʉː/ or /ʊ/ when it precedes /ə/ in words like 'annual' and for
the same reason as above: these vowels are very short in
duration and can even be deleted resulting in a range of
61
productions from three-syllable /ænjʉːəl/ to two syllable
/ænjəl/.
English words of Latin origin (and Latin and Germanic languages)
have quantity-sensitive feet. i.e. The phonemic structure of the
rhyme contributes to the determination of stress.
are strong
Morphology and word stress
English word stress is dependent on:
62
• rhythmic factors (as we have seen: In Latin base words non-
final heavy syllables like to be strong)
Morphological factors
The suffixes -ion, -ity, -ic, -ify, -ible, -igible, -ish, require stress
to be on the preceding syllable
63
words of two syllables ending in ate place the accent on -ate eg
translate, dictate, debate.
Example 1: "Turramurra"
(1) Begin by identifying whether there are any syllables that are
schwa vowels, or which can reduce to schwa, because these have
64
to be metrically weak: for this word, this applies to the second
and fourth syllables. Confirm that the other syllables cannot
reduce to schwa. If this is the case, they are likely to be
metrically strong. We therefore have four syllables which are s w
s w.
(3) Associate any weak syllables with the foot that precedes
them. As a result of this, we get two binary feet:
(4) If there is more than one syllable, one of the feet has to
marked strong, and the other(s) as weak. The foot that is marked
strong is the one that dominates the primary stressed syllable
(the third syllable in this example). So the first foot is weak. We
therefore arrive at:
65
(5) Join up the feet to form word tree. If there is an initial weak syllable
(doesn't apply in this case, but it would in e.g. 'asparagus') join that to the
word level. We therefore have the following with the transcription included:
Example 2: "pedestrian"
Draw a prosodic word tree for 'pedestrian'. Following through the above
five steps.
(1) 'pedestrian' = w s w w
(2)
66
(3)
(i.e. a ternary foot)
67
Tone Unit
Sometimes there is even a “tail”, that is, some syllables following the
tonic syllable up to the end of the tone-unit. So, the structure of a tone-
unit is (pre-head) (head) tonic syllable (tail). Intonation is very
important for communication, as it helps the addressee interpret the
message
Suprasegmental phonology
Stress : applied to units larger than phonemes (segmental
phonology), i.e. syllables
Intonation : pitch of voice plays an important part; it is
constantly changing during speech; analysing intonation refers to
listening to the speaker´s pitch and recognising what it is doing
Pitch
Defined in terms of high and low (arbitrary choices for end-
points of the pitch scale)
Auditory sensation experienced by the hearer
We are not interested in all aspects of a speaker´s pitch, but in
those that carry some linguistic information
Speakers have control over their own pitch of voice, and the
possibility of choice (this may have linguistic significance)
Fundamental frequency of voiced sounds ;should not be called
͞pitch
68
The rate of vibration of the vocal folds, physically measureable
and related to speaker´s activity
The distinction must be made between the two
As long as the distinction is made it is of minor importance if
we informally use the same term pitch for both
Necessary conditions for pitch differences to be
linguistically relevant
Being under speaker´s control
Pitch differences must be perceptible (great enough to be heard
by a listener as differences in pitch)
Significance in linguistics lies in contrasts (a set of items a unit
contrasts with) Form and function of intonation
In the shortest piece of speech – single syllable
A continuous piece of speech beginning and ending with a
pause – utterance
One syllable utterances like ‘yes’ and ‘no’
Even in one syllable words we can either remain at a constant
pitch level or change it
Tone
Is the term used for the overall behaviour of the pitch
It can be level or moving
The latter are more
Level tone does not sound natural
When saying yes or no in a final manner, falling tone is usually
used
For questioning manner rising tone is used (now we are already
talking about function) Tone and tone languages
69
Tone is marked before the syllable: level _yes falling yes rising
yes In this way we can also mark the high tone level and low tone
level
The case for some other languages is different:
In these the tone can determine on the meaning of the word in
question, in Croatian as well Speech may be divided into tone
units. Each tone unit is composed of:
A tonic syllable (obligatory)
Optional elements which precede the tonic syllable (pre-head,
head)
Optional elements which follow the tonic syllable (tail) In
synthesis:
(pre-head) (head) tonic syllable (tail) Complex tones and pitch
height
Each of these may express particular attitudes:
Fall: neutral statement
Rise: neutral question, doubt
Fall-Rise: scepticism
Rise-Fall: emphatic statement
Level: boredom, disinterest
In ordinary speech intonation tends to take place within the lower
part of the speaker´s pitch range. Only with strong feelings we
use extra pitch height.
70
I have answered your question and do not intend to add anything
else.
71
Level: boredom, lack of interest _______________
Can you remember Peter Jackson, the cost consultant for our
company in Taiwan? The other day in the office I invited him for
dinner, he’ll be coming tomorrow.
Yes.
yes
Things become more complicated when we add syllables. In this
case there are no stressed syllables before the tonic:
__ ____
I said yes
‘ I said’ is the pre-head.
Here we added a stressed syllable ,’told’ before the tonic, which
is called the head. Notice how the intonation rises from the pre-
head.
____ _____ _ _____
__
I told you I said yes
72
Here we have added syllables after the tonic, this is called the
tail. Note how it tends to follow the intonation pattern of the
tonic.
I might buy it
No matter how many syllables there are in the tail, the rise
finishes on the last
73
Use punctuation to help your pronunciation skills by adding a
pause after each period, comma, semi-colon or colon. By using
punctuation to guide when you pause while reading, you will
begin to speak in a more natural manner. Make sure to read the
example sentences on this page out loud using the pronunciation
tips provided. Let's look at an example sentence:
In this example, pause after 'Chicago' and 'house.' This will help
anyone who's listening to you follow you more easily. On the
other hand, if you rush through the periods and commas (and
other punctuation marks), your pronunciation will sound
unnatural and it will be difficult for listeners to follow your
thoughts.
74
• Do you like living in Portland?
• Have you lived here a long time?
• Did you visit your friends last month?
Question Tags
If you think you know something, but would like to confirm it, let
the voice fall in the question tag.
75
Question Tags to Ask for Clarification
When using a question tag to clarify, let the voice rise to let the
listener know that you expect more information.
End of Sentences
• That's great!
• I'm free!
• I bought a new car.
• Mary is happy.
• We're married.
• They're exhausted.
76
Commas
In this example, the voice rises after each item in the list. For the
final item, let the voice fall. In other words, 'tennis,' 'swimming,'
and 'hiking' all rise in intonation. The final activity, 'biking,' falls in
intonation. Practice with a few more examples:
• When you read this letter, I will have left you forever.
77
• Because it's so expensive to travel in Europe, I have decided
to go to Mexico for my vacation.
• Although the test was very hard, I got an A on it.
78
The Rules of Intonation in English
Have you heard about the rules of intonation in English? If you
have not heard of them yet, then let me walk you through them.
Here’s what you need to know!
Imagine yourself asking someone and realize was it exactly the
way it should sound? Sometimes, the way we said things actually
gets us really anxious in a way that we get scared if we said it
wrongly or sarcastically, or jokingly. The point is, we want to make
sure that we say things with the correct intonation.
In particular, you talk to a friend and say, “My shoes are nice,
aren’t they?” Yet, you felt bewildered whether you are trying to
confirm it or simply trying to ask about his/her point of view.
Truthfully, almost all of us struggle to distinguish when to use the
rising and falling intonation whenever we utter a statement or a
question. Subsequently, this continues to confuse people with what
we truly want to convey. That’s why in this article, we are going to
learn the different types of intonation.
What is Intonation?
Intonation is the way the voice rises or falls when
communicating. In short, it is the music of speech.
It focuses more on how you say it rather than what is being said.
This is important in English communication because sentences may
vary in meaning depending on the intonation. It also yields
dynamics on your speech and makes it more compelling to hear.
79
So what are common types and rules of intonation?
The most common types of Intonation we have in English are the
following:
• Falling intonation
• Rising intonation
• Falling–rising intonation
• Rising–falling intonation
Falling Intonation
One of the most common intonation patterns in the English
language is the rules of falling intonation.
This is when the pitch of the voice falls at the end of a sentence.
We commonly use them in statements, commands, WH-questions,
confirmatory question tags, and exclamations.
Below are some examples where we use the falling
intonation:
In statements, we say:
Pleased to meet ↓you.
Mom wants to buy that ↓bag.
We should keep ↓going.
In commands, we say:
Show me the way to the ↓station.
Take your shoes ↓off.
Leave them on the ↓desk.
In WH-questions, we say:
What food do you ↓want?
80
Where do you come ↓from?
Whose book is ↓this?
In question tags, we use the rules of falling intonation only when
we seek confirmation or invite agreement.
For example, we say:
He thinks he’s so smart, doesn’t ↓he?
They didn’t enjoy the party, did ↓they?
She’s the one you told me about, isn’t ↓she?
In exclamations, we say:
That’s a perfect ↓gift!
How nice of ↓you!
What a beautiful ↓performance!
Rising Intonation
Another intonation marking used in the English language is the
rules of rising intonation.
This is when the pitch of the voice rises at the end of the sentence.
It is commonly used in yes or no questions, and question tags that
show uncertainty.
In question tags showing uncertainty, we say:
You’re new here, aren’t ↑you?
He prefers the beach, doesn’t ↑he?
The rules were changed, weren’t ↑they?
In yes or no questions, we say: Do you like your new ↑shirt? May
I borrow your ↑book?
Will she ↑agree?
81
Rising-Falling Intonation
We do not only use rising or falling intonations alone, but we can
likewise use a combination of both. Consider the rules of rising-
falling intonation.
This is generally used for giving out a list, a set of choices, partial
and conditional statements.
In stating a list, use rising intonation in the first series of items.
Then, use a falling intonation on the last item to indicate that the
list has ended. For instance:
She bought some ↑bread, ↑meat, ↑pasta, and ↓apples.
He enjoys outdoor activities such as ↑mountain
climbing, ↑trekking, and ↓camping.
This bag comes in colors of ↑white, ↑gray, ↑black, and ↓yellow.
When used in choices, we say:
Would you like to have ↑tea or ↓coffee?
Is Jane coming ↑today or ↓tomorrow?
Did you say ↑east or ↓west?
We also use the rising-falling intonation in partial statements.
This is when the speaker is hesitant to fully express his ideas and
is unable to finish his statement.
For example:
What was the place like? –Well, it ↑seemed ↓nice… (but it’s too
small.)
So did you enjoy the trip? –Hmmm, ↑yes I ↓did… (but it was too
short.)
82
Do Ethan and Ricky know you? –Well, ↑Ethan ↓does… (but not
Ricky.)
In conditional statements, we use the rising intonation in the
first clause, then gradually shift to the falling intonation in the
second clause.
So we say:
If you have any ↑questions, send me a ↓message.
Ever since you came ↑here, everything’s doing ↓well.
Unless she ↑asks, I’ll stay ↓silent.
Falling-rising intonation
Finally, one of the familiar rules of intonation is the usage of
the falling–rising intonation within a word. This is often used
when the speaker is uncertain of an answer to a question or shows
reluctance.
For example:
Should we ↓re↑port this incident?
Do you think it is ↓va↑lid?
Would he ↓su↑pport the team?
Why learn the Rules of Intonation?
Unlike other languages, the English language follows several rules
of Intonation. We use a different intonation in every different
situation in English. And while other people don’t give so much
importance to the rules, having the right intonation helps us sound
clearer and more natural.
83
Below are the five benefits of having a very good intonation.
1. Awareness of intonation aids communication.
3. Intonation allows you to play well in tune. So, the better the
tune, the more you’ll sound better and will be perceived as a better
speaker.
5. You will feel more confident and at ease when you know your
intonation is good. When you have good intonation, speaking in
English will become more fun!
Now that you already know the reasons for the importance of
intonation in English, make sure you practice and learn them. This
will be a little challenging at first, but trust, it will be totally useful
if you already get the hang of it. So, keep practicing.
84
Summary and Examples
What is intonation?
85
This explanation on intonation is intended to serve as a
general guide to help learners.
It should in no way make them unnecessarily anxious!
the ➘ week.
86
o We should work together more ➘ often
o I'm going for a walk in the ➘ park.
• Commands
o Write your name ➘ here.
87
agreement, in which case we use a falling tone at the
end.
o He thinks he’s so clever, doesn’t ➘ he?
did ➘ he?
o It doesn't seem to bother him much, does ➘ it?
• Exclamations
o How nice of ➘ you!
o That's a ➘ surprise!
88
o Do you sell ➚ stamps?
89
o We've got ➚ apples, pears, bananas
and ➘ oranges
o The sweater comes in ➚ blue, white, pink
and ➘ black
o I like ➚ football, tennis, basketball
and ➘ volleyball.
o I bought ➚ a tee-shirt, a skirt and a ➘ handbag.
• Unfinished thoughts (partial statements)
In the responses to the following questions, the rise-fall
intonation indicates reservation.
The speaker hesitates to fully express his/her thoughts.
o Do you like my new handbag? Well the ➚ leather
is ➘ nice... ( but I don't like it.)
o What was the meal like? Hmm, the ➚ fish was ➘ good...
(but the rest wasn't great).
o So you both live in Los Angeles? Well ➚ Alex ➘ does ...
(but I don't).
• Conditional sentences
(The tone rises in the first clause and falls gradually in the
second clause.)
o If he ➚ calls, ask him to leave a ➘ message.
o Unless he ➚ insists, I'm not going to ➘ go.
o If you have any ➚ problems, just ➘ contact us.
• Hesitation/reluctance:
90
o So you'd be willing to confirm that? ...Well ...
I ➘sup➚pose so ...
o You didn't see him on Monday? I don't
quite ➘re➚member ...
91
You will be surprised how quickly your pronunciation will improve
with the help of audio materials.
It will be a reward for all your hard work!
Bibliography
92
Ainsworth, W.A. (1976) Mechanisms of speech recognition. Oxford:
Pergamon.
Obler, L.K. and Gjerlow, K. (1999) Language and the brain. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
95