Core 1
Core 1
(aggression)
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression
through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of abnormal and social
psychology, 63(3), 575–582.
Children copy adults. One reason for this is because the immediate
social setting encourages the child to imitate what he or she is watching
(Figure 4.1). This helps influence their behavior, making it more likely
that the child will do what others are doing around them. Alternatively,
the observation of a behavior can lead a child to acquire a new response
that he or she could reproduce independently. If this is the case, the new
behavior should generalize to new settings and so would be produced in
the absence of an adult model.
Key Words
Trẻ em bắt chước người lớn. Một lý do cho điều này là do môi trường xã
hội xung quanh khuyến khích trẻ bắt chước những gì mà chúng đang
quan sát (Hình 4.1). Điều này ảnh hưởng đến hành vi của chúng, làm
tăng khả năng trẻ sẽ làm những gì người khác đang làm xung quanh.
Ngoài ra, việc quan sát một hành vi có thể dẫn đến việc trẻ tiếp thu một
phản ứng mới mà chúng có thể tự tái hiện. Nếu đúng như vậy, hành vi
mới sẽ được khái quát hóa sang các bối cảnh mới và sẽ được sản sinh
mà không cần có sự hiện diện của một mô hình người lớn.
Albert Bandura trước đây đã thực hiện các thí nghiệm về học tập xã hội
và quan tâm đến việc nghiên cứu học tập xã hội trong bối cảnh hành vi
hung hăng. Để học từ người khác, người quan sát (ví dụ: một đứa trẻ)
phải chú ý đến hành vi của một mô hình. Chúng phải lưu giữ (ghi nhớ)
hành vi mà chúng đã quan sát được, để có thể tái hiện lại. Khi học tập
xã hội diễn ra, nó có thể dẫn đến hành vi hung hăng hoặc không hung
hăng. Bandura cho rằng việc quan sát một mô hình hung hăng sẽ dẫn
đến việc biểu hiện nhiều hành vi hung hăng hơn, trong khi quan sát một
mô hình không hung hăng sẽ dẫn đến việc biểu hiện nhiều hành vi
không hung hăng hơn, tức là hành vi ít hung hăng hơn bình thường.
Từ khóa
Children are rewarded in different ways for imitating adults. In general (at
least, in the USA in the 1960s when this study took place), boys were
rewarded for behaviors considered to be sex-appropriate and punished
for inappropriate ones, such as cooking or “playing mother.” Similarly, for
girls, rewards and punishments would be applied to discourage what
was considered sex-inappropriate behaviors. This, Bandura et al.
suggested, would lead to two kinds of differences.
First, boys and girls should be more likely to imitate same-sex models
and, second, they should differ in the readiness with which they imitate
aggression, with boys doing so more readily as this was seen as a more
masculine-type behavior.
Aim
Key Words
Method
This was a laboratory experiment; the environment was not the normal
place where the children played, and the situation was controlled. The
design of the experiment was that of independent measures as different
children were used in each of the levels of the independent variable.
Variables (IVs)
Although these children were matched for aggression in threes (see the
Research Methods box), there were three IVs:
• Model type: whether the child saw an aggressive model, a
non-aggressive model, or no model.
• Model sex: same sex as the child (boys watching a male
model and girls watching a female model) or different sex (boys
watching a female model and girls watching a male model).
• Learner sex: whether the child was a boy or a girl.
The dependent variable (DV) was the behavior the child displayed. This
was measured through a controlled observation of the children, and
measures of aggressive behavior were recorded.
Research Methods
In the matched participants design described here, the participants were
divided into threes, all with very similar initial aggression levels. One of
each of these individuals was placed into each of the three different
conditions of model type (aggressive model, non-aggressive model, and
control).
Sample
Seventy-two children aged 3–6 years (36 boys and 36 girls) were
recruited from Stanford University nursery school.
Procedure
Prior to the experimental part of the study, the children were observed in
their nursery school by the experimenter and a teacher who knew them
well. They were rated on four different measures of physical aggression,
verbal aggression, aggression to inanimate objects, and aggression
inhibition (anxiety), each on a five-point scale. They were then assigned
to three groups, ensuring that the aggression levels of the children in
each group were matched.
Of the 51 children rated by both observers (the rest were rated by only
one observer), similar ratings were generally produced. Their ratings
were compared as a measure of inter-rater reliability, which showed a
high correlation between the observers, of r = 0.89.
Research Methods
Twelve boys and 12 girls were allocated to control groups who saw no
model. The remaining children were divided equally by sex between
aggressive and non-aggressive model groups and, within those,
between same- and opposite-sex models.
The experimenter and child entered the observation room, where the
experimenter showed the child to a table and chair in their “play area,”
where they were shown how to make potato prints and sticker pictures:
activities previously identified as interesting for children.
The opposite corner of the room also contained a table and chair, a
Tinkertoy set (a wooden building kit), a mallet, and a five-foot (152 cm)
Bobo doll: an inflatable clown-like doll that bounced back when hit
(Figure 4.2). This is where the model sat, in those conditions where
there was one. The experimenter remained in the room so that the child
would not refuse to be alone or try to leave early, but they appeared to
be working quietly at their desk.
The doll was laid on its side, sat on, and punched in the nose, picked up
and hit on the head with a mallet, tossed up in the air, and kicked. This
sequence was performed three times over 9 minutes, accompanied by
aggressive comments such as “Kick him” and two non-aggressive
comments such as “He sure is a tough fella.” Of children in the model
groups, half saw a same-sex model, and the others saw a model of the
opposite sex. A control group did not see any model and, therefore, saw
no aggression.
Research Methods
A test of the child’s aggression then followed in which the child was
observed for 20 minutes using a one-way mirror. The mirror appeared
transparent on the researcher’s side (so they could observe behavior)
but appeared as a normal mirror on the child’s side (so they could not
see that they were being observed from another room). For the
aggressive model group, this was a test of delayed imitation.
Finally, behavior units were also counted for non-aggressive play and
sitting quietly not playing at all, and records were kept of the children’s
remarks about the situation.
The male model scored all the children’s behaviors. Except for those
conditions in which the male was the model, he was unaware of which
condition the child had been in. However, the condition in which the child
had been was visually obvious, as in the case of the aggressive model
children as they performed the specific behaviors exhibited by the
model. To test the reliability of the scorer, a second scorer independently
rated the behavior of half of the children. The reliability score was high at
approximately r = 0.9 for different categories of behavior.
Results
The mean for imitative physical aggression for boys with a male model
(25.8) was much higher than that for girls (7.2). This indicates that the
boys imitated the physical aggression of a male model more than the
girls did. However, with a female model, girls imitated less (5.5) than with
the male model. Girls imitated more verbal aggression of the same-sex
model than boys (although not significantly so). Children were also more
likely to imitate a same-sex model than an opposite-sex model; this
effect was stronger for boys than for girls.
Activity 4.1
Consider the data for the mean aggressive behaviors and the non-
imitative verbal responses.
1. Which data is qualitative and which is quantitative?
2. Draw a bar chart showing the results for imitative physical
aggression. Include the mean scores for the experimental groups and
the control group. Make sure you give the chart labels and an
appropriate title.
In the nature versus nurture debate, we can consider why the boys and
girls showed different responses. This could be because they are
genetically different; a nature explanation. Boys might be biologically
predisposed to be aggressive, so more likely to copy aggressive models.
Alternatively, boys might be more likely to copy aggressive models
because they have been rewarded for aggressive behaviors more than
girls have. This would be a nurture argument.
Conclusion
The results strongly suggest that observation and imitation can account
for the learning of specific acts without reinforcement of either the model
or observer. All four hypotheses were supported:
• Observed aggressive behaviors are imitated: children who
see aggressive models are likely to be more aggressive than those
seeing a non-aggressive model or no model.
• Observed non-aggressive behaviors are imitated: children
seeing non-aggressive models will be less aggressive than those seeing
no model.
• Children are more likely to copy a same-sex model, although
this may depend on the extent to which this behavior is sex-typed.
• Boys are more likely to copy aggression than girls.
Strengths and Weaknesses
The main method was a laboratory experiment. This means that it was
possible to control extraneous variables such as ensuring there was a
possibility that the children in any condition would show aggressive
behavior. This was done by showing them nice toys but then taking them
to another room. Also, all children in both experimental groups saw a
model for the same length of time, and in each condition their behaviors
were standardized. This means the research was more valid—the
researchers could be sure that the differences in results between
conditions were due to the differences between the models—and more
reliable, because each child within a condition experienced exactly the
same exposure. Inter-observer reliability was also checked for both
the initial observations of aggressiveness and for the data recording, and
was very high.
Among the weaknesses of the study is that only six children were used
in each experimental condition and, although they were matched to
reduce the risk of participant variables confounding the results, it is still
a small sample. Furthermore, it is possible that the children were quite
similar, as they all attended the same nursery.
Key Word
Ethical Issues
One ethical issue with the study was some of the children might have
been harmed by becoming more aggressive during the research. For
example, they could have physically injured themselves with the toys
they were given to play with after watching an aggressive model. Even if
this were not the case, the children were still deliberately annoyed in the
procedure of the study. This could have been psychologically distressing
for the children. These aspects of the study go against the ethical
guideline of protecting participants from physical and psychological
harm.
Summary
Bandura et al.’s study used adults being aggressive to a Bobo doll to
show that children’s behavior can be affected by the behavior of a
model. Exact aggressive behaviors were imitated although the study
showed that non-aggressive modeling was also effective. Children were
more likely to copy a same-sex model. Additionally, boys engaged in
more aggressive imitation than girls. This was a well-controlled
laboratory experiment measuring the dependent variable through
objective observations that were reliable. Qualitative data suggested that
the children recognized sex-typing and were surprised by behavior that
did not fit the pattern. The findings suggest practical applications for
protecting children, e.g., through film certification and the use of parental
controls on media devices.
Questions