Ai24 937
Ai24 937
24-937
Using a natural experiment which randomized class times to students, this study reveals that enrolling in early
morning classes lowers students' course grades and the likelihood of future STEM course enrollment. There is a
79% reduction in pursuing the corresponding major and a 26% rise in choosing a lower-earning major,
predominantly influenced by early morning STEM classes. To understand the mechanism, I conducted a survey
of undergraduate students enrolled in an introductory course, some of whom were assigned to a 7:30 AM
section. I find evidence of a decrease in human capital accumulation and learning quality for early morning
sections.
Suggested citation: Yim, Anthony. (2024). How Early Morning Classes Change Academic Trajectories: Evidence from a Natural
Experiment. (EdWorkingPaper: 24-937). Retrieved from Annenberg Institute at Brown University:
https://doi.org/10.26300/7dwy-cq90
How Early Morning Classes Change
Academic Trajectories: Evidence from a
Natural Experiment∗
Anthony LokTing Yim†
Abstract
Using a natural experiment which randomized class times to students, this study re-
veals that enrolling in early morning classes lowers students’ course grades and the
likelihood of future STEM course enrollment. There is a 79% reduction in pursuing
the corresponding major and a 26% rise in choosing a lower-earning major, predom-
inantly influenced by early morning STEM classes. To understand the mechanism, I
conducted a survey of undergraduate students enrolled in an introductory course, some
of whom were assigned to a 7:30 AM section. I find evidence of a decrease in human
capital accumulation and learning quality for early morning sections.
Keywords: Higher Education, Human Capital, STEM, College Major
∗
I am grateful to my advisors Timothy Bond and Victoria Prowse for their support and encouragement
at all stages of this project. I need to particularly thank Kevin Mumford for directing me to find the
data and providing feedback in this project. I also thank Jillian Carr, Colin Sullivan, Brigham Frandsen,
Jeff Denning, Rich Patterson, Kendall Kennedy, seminar participants at the Department of Economics from
Brigham Young University and Purdue University, and participants at the AEFP, MEA, and BYU Graduate
Student Conferences for their helpful comments. This study has been approved by the Purdue IRB (IRB-
2021-1013 and IRB-2022-874) and has been registered (RCT ID: AEARCTR-0010038) at the AEA RCT
Registry. All remaining errors are my own.
†
Department of Economics, Brigham Young University, 2146 West View Building, Provo, UT 84602,
USA; email: anthony [email protected].
1 Introduction
A substantial body of research has shown early morning work schedules have negative effects
on health and performance outcomes. Individuals who work in the early morning suffer
from a higher rate of vehicle accidents and work-related injuries (Horne & Reyner, 1999;
Nakata et al., 2005; Barnes & Wagner, 2009), are more likely to have cardiovascular disease
(Kecklund & Axelsson, 2016), experience slower reaction times (Van Den Berg & Neely,
2006), and have lower work productivity (Barnes & Wagner, 2009; Jagnani, 2018). Due
to differences in circadian rhythm, young people are potentially even more at risk in early
mornings (Garcı́a et al., 2012; Hasler et al., 2014; Cosgrave et al., 2018). Despite this, a
substantial amount of education is conducted before 8:30 AM (Wolfson & Carskadon, 2005).
The extent to which this educational scheduling decision detrimentally affects students’
outcomes is still not well understood.
In this paper, I investigate the impact of early morning (7:30 AM) classes on some of the
most important outcomes for post-secondary students: introductory grades, future STEM
enrollment, persistence in challenging majors, and whether students graduate from higher-
earning majors. To do this, I exploit a natural experiment at a large land-grant university
which effectively randomized the course time for students. By using an instrumental variable
approach and administrative data from the university, I find that being assigned to an
early morning class causes a decrease in students’ course grades by 0.06 GPA points, a
23% reduction in the probability of future STEM course enrollment, a 79% decline in the
likelihood of studying in the corresponding major, and a 26% reduction in the probability
to choose a major from the same college. In addition, having early morning classes raises
the likelihood of graduating from a lower-earning major by 26%. The primary influence of
graduating from a lower-earning major stems from being placed in early morning STEM
classes.
While I find negative effects on academic performance, the magnitude is not strong
enough to fully explain the change in enrollment and major selection behavior. To further
1
investigate the mechanisms behind these changes, I conduct a survey of students in a large
introductory economics course. My survey covers 343 students across both an early morning
and mid morning section. I find students become less motivated and have lower participation
in the early morning section. This suggests both academic and non-academic factors play
an important role fueling the detrimental effect of early morning classes.
To the best of my knowledge, this is the first study to examine the impact of early
morning classes on students’ choice of majors with differing earning potentials. For college
students, which majors and courses to select is an important decision since it directly influ-
ences students’ academic trajectories and future labor market outcomes. Arcidiacono (2004)
and Webber (2014a) document large earning gaps across majors due to ability sorting. An-
drews et al. (2017) find long-lasting effects of major selection on earnings. Bleemer & Mehta
(2022) provide strong evidence of huge earning premiums when students were selected into
economics majors. Given the size of my estimates, my results suggest early morning class
assignment can have durable detrimental effects on labor market outcomes.
Previous studies on the impact of early morning classes on college students’ outcomes
have been limited to military academies (Carrell et al., 2011; Williams & Shapiro, 2018;
Haggag et al., 2021). While the unique environment of these institutions allows for clean
identification, they are not representative of the typical university experience.1 For instance,
class attendance is mandatory in both military academies. Students who are not attentive
in class face discipline from their commanding officers. In addition, students are required
to participate in military drills before 7:30 AM on a typical day. Both academies set daily
curfews and limit the ability of students to leave campus. Students are committed to five
years of active-duty military service immediately after graduation, limiting the effects of
major choice on career trajectories.
I find a similar effect on grades to the military academy studies, but much stronger
1
According to the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES), the enrollment statistics at the
university I study are similar to the enrollment statistics from other U.S. land-grant universities, such as
gender ratio, racial composition, age group, SAT scores, etc.
2
effects on the choice of a major. This likely reflects the different institutional environment,
as non-military students have more flexibility to respond to adverse conditions in their early
educational experience. This study also opens up a potential channel where students may
actually skip early morning classes since attendance is not mandatory in this land-grant
university. Unlike my study, Haggag et al. (2021) are unable to investigate the effect of
early morning classes on STEM persistence as there is limited course choice at the military
academies and unable to address students’ major selection associated with different earning
potential, which may directly affect their labor market outcomes. In addition, Haggag et al.
(2021) suggest attribution bias as a mechanism of their findings. In my paper, it means that
students may misattribute the negative effects of early morning classes to the course subject,
and I find suggestive evidence of attribution bias.
The rest of the paper proceeds as follows: Section Two discusses the biological background
and the connection between sleep and educational outcomes, Section Three describes insti-
tutional setting and assignment algorithm, and Section Four explains the data and sample;
Section Five discusses empirical strategy, and Section Six contains the results and mecha-
nisms. Section Seven concludes.
2 Background
To understand how early morning classes affect students’ decision making and educational
outcomes, we first need to have a basic understanding of the circadian rhythm and the link
between sleep and academic achievements.
The circadian rhythm, a hard-wired “clock” in the brain that controls the production of
the sleep-inducing hormone melatonin, is the biological rhythm that governs our sleep-wake
cycles. During adolescence, teenagers experience significant changes of the circadian rhythm.
3
Hence, they experience more daytime sleepiness while preferring later bedtimes and wake-up
times (Carskadon et al., 1993; Wolfson & Carskadon, 1998; Crowley et al., 2007). In fact,
the average adolescent body starts producing melatonin at around 11 PM and continues in
peak production until 7 AM, then stops its production at around 8 AM. In comparison, the
highest production of melatonin for adults is at around 4 AM. Hence, if we ask teenagers
to attentively participate in class activities at 7 AM , it is equivalent of asking adults to
attend work meetings at 4 AM (Carrell et al., 2011). In short, teenagers are more awake in
the late morning and early evening, but they experience low levels of alertness in the early
morning and mid-afternoon (Cardinali, 2008). A number of studies show that depending on
the circadian rhythm of individuals, their ability to learn and receive information fluctuates
throughout the day (Goldstein et al., 2007; Schmidt et al., 2007; Pope, 2016).
Standard university class schedules are incompatible with young students’ circadian
rhythms by requiring students to attend early morning classes when the melatonin gen-
erated by their bodies is at the peak levels.2 With the current class schedules among many
universities, students get up early to attend morning classes when they should be asleep.3
I acknowledge other factors that contribute to later bedtimes, but studies show that young
people stay awake mostly for biological reasons instead of social reasons (Carskadon et al.,
1993; Crowley et al., 2007). The university class schedules, therefore, create an environment
in which students learn less and make erroneous educational decisions, especially for students
who are assigned to attend early morning classes.
2
Policymakers and school administrators suggest that high schools and universities should start classes
later. In fact, back in 2009, the House of Representatives introduced House Concurrent Resolution 176, also
as known as the Zzz’s to A’s Resolution, which calls for secondary schools to begin school no earlier than 9
AM. However, this resolution is strictly voluntary, so schools and universities can still determine students’
class schedules. In this study, I extend this issue to the university level because the majority of university
freshmen are still in their late teen years when they first enroll in university courses.
3
Students may potentially go to bed earlier on the night before the early morning classes, but it is difficult
for them to do so because students’ bodies do not start producing melatonin until late into the night.
4
2.2 The Relationship between Sleep and Academic Achievements
Mental states and sleep-wake cycles matter for learning (Persson et al., 2007; Schmidt et al.,
2007; Williams & Shapiro, 2018). Early morning classes imply that students’ biological
sleep-wake cycle is disrupted (Carskadon et al., 1993; Wolfson & Carskadon, 1998; Crowley
et al., 2007). Research finds that students who attend early morning classes receive lower
test scores and course grades than other students from latter sections at the post-secondary
level (Carrell et al., 2011; Williams & Shapiro, 2018).4
In this study, I also explore the impact of early morning STEM courses on the probability
of future enrollment in STEM courses and subsequent selection of majors. I am particularly
interested in these outcomes because university administrators have increasingly focused
on guiding students toward majors with higher earning potentials, particularly in STEM-
related fields (Bleemer & Mehta, 2021; Sjoquist & Winters, 2015; Denning & Turley, 2017).
In addition, the courses students undertake and their eventual majors significantly impact
their long-term earnings and career paths (Chevalier, 2011; Altonji et al., 2012; Hastings
et al., 2013; Altonji et al., 2014; Kirkeboen et al., 2016; Webber, 2014b; Patnaik et al., 2020;
Bleemer & Mehta, 2022). This paper demonstrates that early morning classes adversely
affect students’ future enrollment in STEM fields and reduce the likelihood of them selecting
a corresponding major. Thus, it suggests that university course scheduling plays a significant
role in shaping students’ academic and labor market outcomes.
Consequently, students make important educational decisions, such as types of courses to
take and majors to study, based on their perceived academic achievements within courses,
such as test scores and final grades (Haggag et al., 2021). Students may make improper
academic decisions that have huge future labor market implications if they are subject to
4
In K-12 literature, students earn higher grades with later school start times. For example, Edwards
(2012) identifies a two percentile point gain in math test scores if students have later school start times due
to variation in bus schedules from all middle schools in Wake County, North Carolina from 1999 to 2006.
Additionally, Groen & Pabilonia (2019) show that when high schools start school days an hour later, female
students have higher reading test scores, but they find no evidence for higher test scores from male students.
Pope (2016) further investigates how the time of day affects students’ productivity and concludes that having
a morning instead of afternoon math or English class increases students’ GPAs.
5
a disrupted mental state and fatigue while being exposed to academic subjects early in the
morning.
Purdue is a large public university in the U.S. with a 2021 total enrollment of almost 50,000
students, including more than 37,000 undergraduate students. Purdue offers over 200 majors
in agriculture, business management, education, engineering, science, social science, human-
ities, pharmacy, and veterinary medicine that students can freely choose from throughout
their college career. In fact, more than 65% of undergraduate students studied in STEM-
related fields in the academic year of 2021.5 Like many public universities in the U.S.,
freshman students can declare a major in their first year at Purdue or also enroll in courses
without a declared major.6
Courses and sections are two different concepts in this paper. While courses refer to a
series of lectures or lessons in particular subjects, sections refer to specific times and locations
that students are assigned to attend their particular courses.7 Purdue offers multiple sections
for various types of lower-level introductory courses throughout the day.
5
31% of undergraduate students are under the College of Engineering; 12% of them are in the College of
Health and Human Science; 14% and 11% of undergraduate students are under the College of Science and
Polytechnic Institute respectively. These four colleges are some of the most popular colleges in the university.
6
Freshman and transfer students who have not chosen a major are assigned to exploratory studies, a
2-academic-year program to help students discover the major that best suits their interests. Nonetheless,
students are free to leave the program within two years after they choose their major.
7
For example, there is a course called ECON 101 with two different sections : section 1 at 7:30 AM and
section 2 at 11:30 AM, so sections are the subsets of a course.
6
3.2 Course Assignment Algorithm
The Purdue class assignment algorithm is called the batch registration, which was reintro-
duced in 2018.8 The university randomly assigns class schedules to undergraduate students
through this algorithm conditional on their course request preferences.9 The algorithm in-
corporates the individual course preference rankings as inputs to produce schedules for all
students.10 The algorithm assigns students in random order based on the number of available
sections of each course students submit before assigning sections to students. Afterwards,
those without a complete course schedule would be put in random order, and the algorithm
assigns sections to them again(Müller & Murray, 2010).
Under the batch registration process, students request their preferred schedules as if they
are entering one huge scheduling competition. Since students do not usually know how other
students make their respective course requests and do not know the most optimal course
request strategies, students submit their course requests in the hope that they receive their
most preferred schedule.11 Course characteristics (e.g., time, date, teacher’s race, teacher’s
gender, etc.) are available on the university web page, and students can review them before
8
Purdue University had been assigning class schedules to students via the batch registration for a long
period of time, but it was discontinued in 2008. Between Fall 2008 and Spring 2018, students self registered
for their courses as long as they met course prerequisites.
9
The batch registration only applies to students enrolled in fall and spring terms. Even though Purdue
provides summer sessions from May to early August, the batch registration process is not used, so class
schedules are not randomly assigned to students. Instead, students just need to register for their preferred
courses. Summer sessions usually start in the middle of May and end at the beginning of August. Like many
other universities, the enrollment at Purdue is relatively lower in summer terms than the enrollment in fall
and spring terms. The university offers fewer in-person courses and more online courses during summer.
10
The batch registration optimizes the objective to satisfy students’ course request preferences subject to
the number of courses and sections the university offers, classroom capacity, and physical distances between
classes. Students with similar course requests are grouped together. Then, the algorithm works in 6 phases:
1) The algorithm orders students based on the number of sections available for the courses they requested
and assigns course sections to them. 2) Students without a complete schedule are taken in random order
and are assigned sections. 3) The algorithm randomly selects and assigns an unassigned section to students.
4) The algorithm improves the overall schedules by using backtracking technique. 5) Students are selected
randomly and try to fill in any available sections at that point if all their requests are unassigned. 6) The
algorithm goes back to step 1 and starts over again. More discussion can be found under Appendix - Batch
Registration.
11
Even though students may potentially game the course assignment algorithm, they still need to compete
with other students who also game the algorithm.
7
submitting their course request.12 However, most freshman students do not state their
preferred class times because they are unaware of this feature in the course request form. I
will discuss more about course and section compliance rates in a later section.
Data for this study comes from Purdue University in West Lafayette, Indiana, and includes
9,030 student-by-course-by-term observations from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020.13 I
focus on domestic non-athlete students who are assigned to general education or introductory
courses with multiple sections including at least one early morning class.
I split the data in two ways.14 Hence, I can estimate students’ likelihood to take STEM
courses within the subsequent two terms for 5,118 student-course observations from Fall
2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020.15 In terms of students’ choice of major, I mainly focus
on the domestic freshman students from Fall 2018, with 3,912 student-course observations,
because they are going to be the first graduating class since the introduction of the class
schedule randomization policy at Purdue University.16
Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics for my regression sample. Among all three panels,
there are roughly 20% of student-course observations assigned to early morning classes.
12
For time and date-related studies, please see Dills & Hernandez-Julian (2008), Carrell et al. (2011),
Edwards (2012), Pope (2016), Diette & Raghav (2017), Williams & Shapiro (2018), Groen & Pabilonia
(2019), and Haggag et al. (2021). For teacher’s gender and race-related studies, please see Canes-Wrone
& Rosen (1994), Robst et al. (1998), Robb & Robb (1999), Carrell et al. (2009), Hoffmann & Oreopoulos
(2009), Ehrenberg & Brewer (1995), Ehrenberg et al. (1995), Rask & Bailey (2002), Dee (2004), Dee (2005),
Klopfenstein (2005), and Price (2010). Even though Diette & Raghav (2017) investigate the impact of
different class times on students’ test scores at a private liberal art college, they do not test if the class
assignments are random, so it casts doubt on the validation of the identification strategy.
13
There are 9,020 student-course observations for estimating the effect on course grades. This number is
lower than the reported observations of 9,030 since 10 of the student-course observations were not rewarded
with a letter grade.
14
Since I can observe more students’ course taking information in multiple terms, I define this set of
sample as “STEM Sample” that includes domestic non-athlete freshman students between the age of 18 and
21 years old from Fall 2018 to Spring 2020. Because I can only observe students’ whole university career
from the Fall 2018 cohort, I define “Major Sample” as domestic non-athlete freshman students between the
age of 18 and 21 years old from Fall 2018.
15
The data of Spring 2019 are excluded because Purdue did not randomly assign students class schedules.
16
Samples in this study are all domestic non-athlete freshman students from the age of 18 to 21 years old.
8
There are more female students assigned in early morning sections than male students.
Black, Hispanic, and “Other” students make up about 12% of the regression sample, with
the majority of students being white.17 In this study, I only include observations of domestic
students in my main analysis because I can make the closest comparison with the findings
of Carrell et al. (2011), Williams & Shapiro (2018), and Haggag et al. (2021).18
My primary sources for students’ academic schedules are the course request data from Fall
2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020 terms.19 In this data set, I have course request information
that students submit to the university before they receive their random class schedules
including, the number of requested courses, order of requested courses, alternative courses
(if requested courses are not granted to students), indicators of preferred class times, and
indicators of required class times.20 Figure A.1 in the appendix illustrates the course request
form each undergraduate student needs to fill out.
After submitting course requests to the university, students receive their initial course
assignments from the university on Batch Day. Students may not get the requested courses
and times they prefer. Hence, the university allows them to adjust their course schedules
by adding, dropping, and switching into different courses or class times before the add/drop
deadline, which is four weeks into a term. After the add/drop deadline, the schedules
become finalized, but students can still withdraw from courses with withdrawal records on
their transcripts. The final grade of each course is recorded after all final exams and projects
17
I identify Native Americans or undisclosed race as “Other” and include Pacific Islanders in the “Asians”
category.
18
Observations of Carrell et al. (2011), Williams & Shapiro (2018), and Haggag et al. (2021) are on
students from the the USMA and USAFA who are all U.S. citizens.
19
I exclude student observations from Fall 2020 and Spring 2021 because the university moved most of
the classes to hybrid or online models to encounter the global Pandemic of COVID-19. Under the hybrid
and online models, professors recorded lectures and allowed students to review them online, so students did
not have to attend classes during the designated class time.
20
The major difference between preferred and required class times is that students can list their preferred
class times, but students can only state their required class times after approval from their academic advisors.
9
are concluded.21
To summarize course registration activities at Purdue, Figure 1 and the following bullet
points illustrate the chronological order of course registration in a term:
• Batch Day: Students receive their initial course assignments on that day, and it usually
happens a month before the start of a term.
• A/D Deadline: Students can freely add, drop, or change their class schedules before
that day. This date is usually a week after the beginning of a term.
• Period 2 : Students can freely add, drop, or switch their classes without additional fee
or marks on their transcripts.
• Period 3 : Students can withdraw courses, but the withdrawals are shown in students’
transcripts.
With the course request data available, I am able to estimate the causal effect of early
morning classes on students’ education outcomes by linking it with the course request data
and registrar data.
10
request data, the registrar data also include information on students’ finalized class schedules,
students’ credit hours earned, age, gender, race, SAT scores, and first-generation student
status. In addition, I linked the university-provided earning data for each college major with
the respective majors students graduated from. This earning information for each college
major was provided from Purdue’s Center for Career Opportunities (CCO), where university
alumni completed career surveys detailing their post-graduation employment status and
income. I, then, calculated the average and median earnings for each major.
5 Empirical Strategy
I exploit a natural experiment at Purdue University. The identification strategy comes from
the random class assignments conditional on student’s course request preferences. This allows
me to compare the effects of early morning classes on students’ academic outcomes when
students take the same course with the same instructor but in two different class times (i.e.
7:30 AM and 11:30 AM).22 I focus on whether students are assigned to attend 7:30 AM classes
as my instrument since 7:30 AM classes are the earliest lecture classes offered at Purdue.
Even though instructor assignments to teach classes are not random, I include course-by-
instructor-by-term level in my main specifications because the identification strategy is to
compare students who take the same course with the same instructor but in either a 7:30
AM class or a non-7:30 AM class.
I will further discuss the empirical strategy in three subsections: course compliance,
randomization, and instrumental variable.
22
The course-by-instructor observations provide a way to control the endogeneity of instructor effect since
assignments of instructors’ teaching schedules are not exogenous. Senior or more popular faculty may teach
classes in the late morning or in the afternoon, while junior faculty are likely to give lectures in the early
morning.
11
5.1 Course Compliance
Figure 3 displays descriptive comparisons between early morning and non-early morning
assignments. 1,749 student-course-term observations were assigned to early morning sections.
188 early morning assignments were dropped while 1,397 of them were kept to the early
morning assignments. In addition, 164 student-course-term observations were switched into
non-early morning sections. On the other hand, of those 9,795 observations who got assigned
to non-early morning sections, 1,035 of them were dropped, and 1,452 observations were
switched into early morning sections. Figure 3 shows that most students complied with the
assignments they received with roughly 90% compliance rate.
Students can freely change their class schedules after the Batch Day, so there could be
differential attrition between the treatment (7:30 AM classes) and control (non-7:30 AM
classes) groups that may raise bias in this study. Table A.1 summarizes course compliance
rates of the sample.23 In Table A.1, the upper panel called “STEM Sample” displays the
compliance rates for domestic non-athlete freshman students between the ages of 18 and
21 years old from Fall 2018 to Spring 2020, while the lower panel called “Major Sample”
shows the compliance rates from Fall 2018.24 In the upper panel, compliance rates of early
morning and non-early morning sections show small mean differences. Even though the mean
difference of “STEM” is 1.90% significant at the 10% level. Similarly, the mean difference
of the lower panel is 0.419% and not statistically significant, so the differential attrition of
this study is small and should not cause major issues.
5.2 Randomization
I examine how early morning classes affect students’ educational trajectories by adopting
instrumental variables (IV) estimation. Broadly speaking, ordinary least squares regressions
23
The observations are in course-by-instructor-by-term level because the identification strategy is to com-
pare students who take the same course with the same instructor but in either a 7:30 AM class or a non-7:30
AM class.
24
In the following analyses, I will conduct different regression analyses by using observations from “STEM
Sample” and “Major Sample.”
12
of education outcomes on finalized class assignments are likely to yield biased estimates
because of self-selection of class times. Purdue’s conditionally random class assignments
provide me with an approach to solve the endogeneity problem.
One key assumption is that assignments to early morning classes are random conditional
on students’ course request information. To test this, I regress an indicating variable for
whether students are assigned into early morning classes on a vector of students’ observable
characteristics, including gender, race, standardized SAT scores, and first generation status
conditional on course-by-term fixed effect and course request preferences.25
Table 2 shows the results from the regressions. In column 1, I regress indicators of as-
signed 7:30 AM classes on students’ observable characteristics without course request controls
and course-by-instructor-by-term fixed effect. Even though 3/8 of the characteristics differ
at the 5% and 10% levels of significance, the observable characteristics of students are bal-
anced across early morning and other period classes with the F-test p-value of 0.129. When
I include course-by-instructor-by-term fixed effect in column 2, the students’ observables are
also balanced with no characteristics varying between early morning and non-early morning
classes at the 5% level of significance. The joint F-test p-value is 0.134. After including both
course-by-instructor-by-term fixed effect, course preference controls, and number of courses
requested fixed effect in column 3, the joint F-test p-value of students’ observable covariates
increases to 0.23.26 Altogether, columns 1, 2, and 3 of Table 2 suggest that students’ assign-
ments to early morning classes are conditionally random with F-test p-values greater than
0.1.
The course request preferences include information such as the number of requested
courses, rank of requested courses, alternative courses (if requested courses are not granted
to students), indicators of preferred class times (that are 7:30 AM or non-7:30 AM), and
25
This approach is similar to the methods used by Carrell et al. (2011) and Haggag et al. (2021).
26
Since my specifications of the balance test should be as close to the treatment level as possible, I use
course-by-instructor-by-term fixed effect instead of course-by-term fixed effect even though the randomization
occurs at the course level across terms. The results of these alternative specifications are shown in Table
A.2, and the results are well-balanced.
13
indicators of required class times (that are 7:30 AM or non-7:30 AM).27
If I estimate the effect of early morning classes by simply regressing my outcomes of inter-
est on an indicator for whether students actually enroll in early morning classes shown in
equation (1), the estimated results would be biased due to students’ self-selection in or out
of early morning classes. Therefore, I use an indicator for whether students get randomly
assigned into early morning classes as the instrument.28 Then, I estimate the following equa-
tion to identify the causal effects of early morning classes on educational outcomes by using
the 2-stage-least square (2SLS) approach:
where Yicpt indicates the education outcomes of students. Finalized Earlyicpt is an en-
dogenous regressor and an indicator for whether student i in course c with instructor p at
term t enrolls in an early morning class. Si is a vector of students’ observable character-
istics, including gender, race, standardized SAT scores, first generation status, and college
athlete status. Cict is a vector of course request information students submit to the univer-
sity before they receive their random class schedules including: number of requested courses,
rank of requested courses, alternative courses if requested courses are not granted to stu-
dents, indicators of preferred class times, and indicators of required class times. σcpt is the
course-by-instructor-by-term fixed effect, which allows me to compare the effects of early
morning classes and non-early morning classes taught by the same instructor in the same
term. An indicating variable for whether students get randomly assigned to early morning
classes Assigned Earlyicpt is the instrumental variable in this study.
27
All course request controls are indicator variables. I also explicitly state that what class times students
request and create different interaction terms between each course request control variable. Estimated results
of both specifications are similar.
28
I discuss the identification assumptions of this instrumental variable and the first stage regression model
in Appendix III Regression Models.
14
Although students are not all compliers of the early morning class treatment, over 84%
of course-by-instructor-by-term course assignments remain unchanged when I compare the
compliance rates. For this purpose, I run a reduced form (or direct) regression model:
where Assigned Earlyicpt , the instrument, indicates whether students get randomly as-
signed to early morning classes. The subscript notations and definitions of other variables
are consistent with the descriptions from equation (1).
As noted in equation (1), Yicpt is students’ education outcomes defined as follows:29
Students receive their final course letter grades at the end of the term. The letter
grades are on a 4.0 GPA scale. This variable of interest is a starting point to inves-
tigate the effect of early morning classes on students’ human capital accumulation.30
This indication of aptitude could also serve as a mechanism of students’ course-taking
behavior and major choice.
2. Indicators for whether students are going to take corresponding STEM classes within
the next two terms
STEM classes are defined as courses that are offered by departments with STEM af-
filiation. Departments with STEM affiliation refer to departments that offer STEM
majors to undergraduate students. The U.S. Department of Homeland Security (DHS)
STEM Designated Degree Program List is a complete list of fields of study that are
STEM-verified by DHS for purposes of the 24-month STEM optional practical train-
ing (OPT) extension. University administrators and faculty then decide if majors
29
Table ?? shows the lower-level division courses with multiple sections including at least one early
morning section in each sample. Those courses are diversely offered by School of Science, Business School,
School of Liberal Arts, and so on.
30
Instructors may adjust final course grades for students and therefore creates measurement error. Yet,
it is still the most relevant variable to infer their learning.
15
offered by different colleges are perceived as STEM respectively based on the intensity
of science, technology, engineering, or mathematics courses and academic credits that
each field of study requires.31 Estimating whether students are likely to take corre-
sponding STEM classes in subsequent terms projects their human capital development
during their university career. It also infers how persistent students are in acquir-
ing STEM-related skills. Since university graduates with strong STEM training have
higher earnings than students with fewer STEM skills do, this estimation may shed
more light on students’ labor market outcomes in the future.
3. Indicators for whether students are going to study in a major directly corresponding to
the assigned early morning classes at 7:30 AM
The third outcome variable of interest is an indicator of whether students would study
in a corresponding major and choose a major from the same college. I do so by creating
a mapping between courses with multiple class times including at least one 7:30 AM
classes and their most direct major.32 For example, if a student gets assigned into a
100-level Principles of Economics course at 7:30 AM, I am interested in understanding
if she will major in economics in the near future conditional on course request controls,
student’s observable characteristics, and course-by-instructor-by-term fixed effect.33
Followed by Haggag et al. (2021), I also propose a broader level of mapping between
courses and colleges in Table A.5.
4. Indicators for whether students are going to graduate from a lower-earning major
16
lower-earning major, and zero otherwise. I categorize majors with earnings in the first
quartile (Q1) as lower-earning majors. Initially, I link major earning data with the
specific majors from which students graduate provided by Purdue’s CCO. Following
that, I rank these earnings and divide them into different quartiles: first (Q1), second
(Q2), and third (Q3).
First, I estimate whether students’ final course grades are affected by assigned early morning
classes in Table 4 by 2SLS and reduced form estimations of equation 1.34 I find that students
receive lower course grades if early morning classes are assigned to them by both 2SLS and
reduced form estimations. In column (1), with the course-by-instructor-by-term fixed effect,
assignments to early morning classes reduce performance by 0.0806 GPA points.35
The course-by-instructor-by-term fixed effect is the most credible identification strategy
to explore the effects of early morning classes because it enables me to compare the effects
on students from early morning and non-early morning sections within the same courses
taught by the same instructors. When I include course request preferences such as the
number of requested courses, ranks of requested courses, indicators of preferred class times,
and indicators of requested class times in column (2), the estimates are still negative and
34
I estimate the regression models by using only domestic undergraduate non-athlete students between the
age of 18 and 21 years old because I am interested in studying how early morning classes affect traditional
domestic freshman students who are in their late teen years. Also, the estimates can have more direct
comparisons with the estimated results of Carrell et al. (2011), Williams & Shapiro (2018), and Haggag et al.
(2021). Robust standard errors are clustered at the individual and section-by-term levels. Conventionally,
clustering a higher level such as course-by-term level is more ideal. However, I do not have sufficient number
course-by-term clustering with only 15.
35
I only interpret estimated results from the 2SLS estimations for simplicity.
17
close with the p-value at 0.106. In column (3), the precision of my estimates increases
after including demographic characteristics of students such as gender, race, first generation
status, and standardized SAT scores. The estimated result in column (3) is 0.0598 significant
at the 5% level with the magnitude slightly higher than the findings of Carrell et al. (2011),
Williams & Shapiro (2018), and Haggag et al. (2021).36 Altogether, my results in Table 4
provide evidence that early morning classes reduce students’ academic performance.
Some STEM majors require a minimum GPA or course grade threshold, so students with
lower course grades may shy away from taking more STEM classes and choosing a related
major. Table 5 demonstrates a breakdown of the effect of early morning classes on each
letter grade. I find suggestive evidence that early morning classes decrease the probability
of getting higher grades especially with A-, Bs, and C+ from columns (2) to (6). In order
to get admitted into the selective programs, students must reach a minimum GPA (3.0 or
above) of required introductory courses. Assignments to early morning classes, therefore,
inadvertently lower the chance for the marginal students to get into selective programs.
To understand the mechanisms of the findings, I conducted an online field survey by
asking students about their in-class experiences from both early morning and non-early
morning sections. The survey was distributed to students from a lower-level introductory
economics course with two sections (7:30 AM and 9:30 AM) taught by the same instructor
at Purdue University in Fall 2022.37 The response rate was 0.409 with 343 respondents. 38%
of students who responded to the survey came from the early morning section while 45% of
other respondents came from the non-early morning section. Among the respondents, 0.51
of them were assigned to the 7:30 AM section, while 0.49 of them got assigned to the later
section.38 The response rates of students who got assigned to both sections are balanced, so
36
I regress standardized course GPA on assigned early morning classes with the same specifications for
the purpose of making direct comparisons with the findings of Carrell et al. (2011), Williams & Shapiro
(2018), and Haggag et al. (2021).
37
The survey has been approved by the Purdue IRB (IRB-2022-874). The survey is anonymous and
strictly voluntary and does not affect any grades in this course. Upon completion of the survey, students
can choose to enter a raffle to win a $5 Amazon gift card.
38
Figure 2 shows compliance comparisons among two sections.
18
differential attrition is not a concern. In my survey, I listed 11 statements and asked them to
choose one of the following responses: 1. Strongly Disagree, 2. Disagree, 3. Neither Agree
or Disagree, 4. Agree, and 5. Strongly Agree.39 Then, I regressed the survey outcomes of
interest on assigned early morning classes in the following model:
where Fi is an indicator for answering “Strongly Agree” or “Agree” in student i since the
outcome variables are ordinally measured and are hard to interpret estimated results for if
only using response values. τi is the class year rank fixed effect, and the definitions of other
variables are consistent with previous equations. From columns (1) to (5) of Panel 1 in Table
6, the estimated results are related to students’ in-class learning. I find suggestive evidence
that students participate less in class discussions and do not think that early morning classes
motivate learning. Students also conclude that they would learn more from a later section
with the same instructor. The survey results are consistent with the findings of Tables 4 and
5, which suggest that early morning classes lower academic performance.40
Teaching quality may serve as another possible explanation. Instructors who teach mul-
tiple sections within the same course may teach with greater clarity in non-early morning
classes because they have already given the same lecture in the morning and know in advance
if the lectures are well-received by students. Then, instructors can adjust the lectures accord-
ingly. However, in Table 7, I regress final course grades on an indicator for whether students
are randomly assigned into subsequent classes taught by the same instructors within the
same courses. It means that courses with multiple sections offered in the non-early morning
period, late in the morning or in the afternoon would be included in this analysis because
I want to test if students actually performed better academically in the subsequent section.
The results of Table 7 show no evidence that being assigned into subsequent classes may
39
Please see Appendix II for the entire student survey and Appendix IV for the summary statistics.
40
Table A.9 shows the consistent results for only freshman students.
19
increase students’ course grades.41 Additionally, survey results from columns (1) to (4) of
Panel 2 in Table 6 show no evidence about teaching quality in subsequent classes by same
instructors.
Both estimated and survey results suggest that students’ human capital accumulation is
negatively affected by early morning classes. These findings may affect students’ educational
decisions in the future.
Exclusion restriction would be violated if taking an early morning class affects students’
performance in other courses. To test this, I first calculate the leave-one-out term GPA
excluding the course grade from the assigned early morning classes. Then, I regress the
leave-one-out term GPA on an indicator for whether students were assigned to an early
morning class with the same specification in equation (2). I find no evidence that attending
early morning classes affect academic performances in other courses in Table 8.42
In Table 9, I estimate equations (1) and (2) on the impact of early morning classes on
students’ propensity to take corresponding STEM courses within the next two terms. Both
the upper and lower panels of Table 9 present the results of the 2SLS and reduced form
estimations, respectively.
In Table 9, column (1) is the standard regression with the course-by-instructor-by-term
fixed effect because this enables me to estimate the effect of early morning sections within
the same course taught by the same instructor. The estimates of column (1) are negative
and statistically significant at the 5% level.43 When I control for course request preferences
41
I need to acknowledge that the way instructors curve students’ final grades may affect my findings.
Instructors may adjust final grades by each section or by course, and some departments may even adjust
final grades for all sections even taught by different instructors. Hence, final course grades may provide a
less precise measure of students’ academic performance.
42
In this study,
43
For simplicity, I only interpret the 2SLS estimates. Estimates of 2SLS and reduced form are similar.
20
in column (2), the estimated results are now statistically significant at the 1% levels. After
including students’ demographic controls in column (3), the results indicate that students
are less likely to enroll in the corresponding STEM courses by 5.9 percentage points (or 23%)
within the next two terms at the 1% level.44 Furthermore, I investigate the heterogeneous
effects on the likelihood of taking corresponding STEM courses in Table 10. However, I
do not see consistent heterogeneous effects on students’ observable characteristics including
gender, race, first generation status, and SAT scores.
Table 11 discusses the propensity for students to study in a major directly corresponding
to the assigned early morning classes. Similarly, I perform the 2SLS and the reduced form
estimations of equations (1) and (2). I first regress an indicator for whether students study
in a corresponding major by including the course-by-instructor fixed effect in column (1).
I find that there are negative effects of attending early morning classes, but the estimates
are imprecise in both columns (1). I further include course request preferences in column
(2) and including demographic characteristics in column (3), early morning classes decrease
the probability of choosing a corresponding major by 1.2 percentage points (or 68%) and
1.4 percentage points (or 76%). In Table 12, I further explore the heterogeneous effects
of early morning classes on student’s choice of major but find no evidence across students’
demographic characteristics. In Figure 4, I illustrate the effects of different class times on
major choice and find that the negative effect of early morning classes gradually fade away
after 9:30 AM and show null effect later in the morning.45
In addition, I find that students who are assigned into early morning STEM classes are
1.6 percentage points (or 98%) less likely to study in a corresponding STEM major in column
44
I also explore the likelihood of students taking additional corresponding STEM courses in the very next
first term alone in Table A.10. Estimated results are robust.
45
Since each estimation in Table 4 refers to different treatment groups, so readers should not make direct
comparison with the same sample.
21
(2) of Table 13.46 On the other hand, I find no evidence from being enrolled in early morning
non-STEM classes.
In Table 14, I estimate the impact of early morning classes on whether students study
in a major from the same college. I find that early morning classes reduces students’ proba-
bility to choose a major from the corresponding college by 2.7 percentage points (or 26%).47
Consistent with the approach of Table 13, I find that assignments to early morning STEM
classes decrease the probability of studying in the major within the corresponding college by
3.4 percentage points (or 39%) in column (2) of Table 16.
I further examine the effect of early morning classes on students’ likelihood to graduate from
a lower-earning major. The outcome variable of interest is an indicator of whether a student
graduates from a lower-earning major. I define majors with lower earnings as major earning
in the first quartile (Q1).
In column (1) of Table 17, I discovered that assignments to early morning classes increase
the likelihood of graduating with a lower-earning major by 6.6 percentage points (or 26%).
The central reason for graduating from a lower-earning major is enrollment in early morning
STEM classes; in column (2) attendance in these classes results in an 8.1 percentage point (or
29.2%) increase in the likelihood of selecting a lower-earning major. Conversely, I found no
indication that students graduating from lower-earning majors were influenced by enrollment
in early morning non-STEM classes as shown in column (3). Results from columns (4) to
(9) show no evidence that being assigned into early morning classes affects the probability
to graduate from median-earning and higher-earning majors.
46
This analysis includes students with a bachelor degree and students with their latest majors in their
senior year.
47
In Table 15, I do not find strong evidence of heterogeneous effects on major choice at college level.
22
6.2.4 Mechanisms of the Effects on Educational Decisions
This paper analyzes the effect of early morning classes on students’ human capital accumu-
lation and academic trajectories: the likelihood of taking corresponding STEM courses and
the propensity to study a corresponding major. Through a random class assignment algo-
rithm, I document the causal effects that students receive lower academic performance, are
less likely to take corresponding STEM courses in future terms, become less likely to study
in a corresponding major, and increase the probability to graduate from a lower-learning
major.
This study provides more representative findings and has a higher relevance to future
labor market outcomes than prior studies by using a new administrative dataset at a large
public university in the U.S. The institutional setting of Purdue University is more consistent
with the settings of other public universities than the military and educational institutions
like the USMA and USAFA (Carrell et al., 2011; Williams & Shapiro, 2018; Haggag et al.,
2021). I also provide field survey evidence to support my empirical findings.
48
Haggag et al. (2019) and (Haggag et al., 2021) also discuss attribution bias.
23
From the field survey, I find that students become less motivated and participate less
in early morning class. However, the result is not driven by instructor’s teaching quality
documented from both empirical and survey results. In my survey data, I find suggestive
evidence of attribution bias, which is consistent with the findings of Haggag et al. (2021).
As policymakers strive to encourage students, they may want to make adjustments to
university course schedules. Universities may schedule more introductory STEM courses
(i.e., Chemistry 1 and Calculus 1) later in the morning and more humanity and other non-
STEM courses in the early morning. Universities should consider having later class start
times. University students at the age of 18 and 19 are still experiencing the changes of
their circadian rhythm. It therefore becomes difficult for them to stay attentive during early
morning classes (Crowley et al., 2007; Pandi-Perumal et al., 2008; Carrell et al., 2011).
24
8 Figures and Tables
25
Figure 3: Selection Diagram
Dropped (n = 188)
Stayed (n = 1,397)
Assigned Early
Completed Early
(n = 1,749)
(n = 1,452)
Dropped (n = 1,035)
Notes: The observations are in course-by-student-by-term level from the Grade sample.
26
Figure 4: Effects of Different Class Times on Major Choice
-.005
-.01 Coefficient
95% CI
-.015
-.02
-.025
rly
rly
ly
ly
ly
m
r
ea
ea
ea
ea
ea
a
30
or
or
or
or
or
7:
m
0a
0a
0a
0a
0p
3
:3
:3
:3
8:
9:
10
11
12
Semester
Notes: Each estimation refers to different sets of sample since there are students from courses that may be
in the control group at 7:30 AM specification but may be in the treatment group at 8:30 am specification.
27
Table 1: Summary Statistics
Course-Instructor-Term FE N Y Y
Course Preference Controls N Y Y
Number of Courses Requested FE N N Y
Notes: Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the individual
and course-by-term levels (6,595 clusters at the individual level and 197 clus-
ters for course-by-instructor-by-term level). The outcome in this regression
is an indicator for whether students were assigned to an early morning class.
SAT combined is a standardized variable with mean of zero and standard
deviation of one. Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort be-
tween the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020. *
p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
29
Table 3: First Stage Estimates
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
F-Statistics 995.271 947.936 948.967
N 5,118 5,118 5,118
R2 0.861 0.861 0.862
Panel 2: Major Sample
(1) (2) (3)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section 0.870*** 0.864*** 0.864***
(0.0300) (0.0310) (0.0280)
Course-Instructor FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
F-Statistics 838.096 779.575 782.017
N 3,963 3,963 3,963
R2 0.901 0.901 0.902
Notes: Panel 1 shows the first stage estimates. The observations of Panel
1 are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the age of 18 and 21
from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020 who registered to lower-level
(100 or 200 level) STEM classes either in an early morning (7:30 AM) sec-
tion or a non-early morning section. These observations are included into
my STEM-course analyses. There are 61 instructors who teach an early-
morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same course in the
same semester. Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the
individual and section-by-term levels (4,088 clusters at the individual level
and 166 clusters for section-by-term level). Similarly, Panel 2 shows the
first stage estimates. The observations are domestic freshman cohort be-
tween the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018 who registered to lower-level
(100 or 200 level) either in an early morning (7:30 AM) class or a non-early
morning class. These observations are included into my choice-of-a-major
analyses. There are 40 instructors who teach an early-morning course and
also teach multiple sections of the same course in the same semester. Ro-
bust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the individual and
section-by-term levels (2,844 clusters at the individual level and 237 clus-
ters for section-by-term level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
30
Table 4: Effect on Course Grades
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 9,020 9,020 9,020
Dependent Variable Mean 3.00 3.00 3.00
Panel 2: Reduced Form Estimates
(1) (2) (3)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section -0.0687 -0.0475 -0.0509**
(0.0438) (0.0353) (0.0230)
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 9,020 9,020 9,020
R2 0.119 0.130 0.240
Dependent Variable Mean 3.00 3.00 3.00
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman students between the
age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020. There are 78
instructors who teach an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections
of the same course in the same semester. Robust standard errors in parentheses
are clustered at the individual and course-by-term levels (5,029 clusters at the
individual level and 311 clusters for section-by-term level). * p < 0.10, **
p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
31
Table 5: Effect on Getting Different Grades
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Course Request Controls Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Demographic Controls Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
N 9,020 9,020 9,020 9,020 9,020 9,020 9,020 9,020 9,020 9,020 9,020
R2 0.191 0.199 0.217 0.190 0.174 0.169 0.105 0.097 0.094 0.094 0.049
Dependent Variable Mean 0.306 0.384 0.464 0.680 0.723 0.783 0.888 0.913 0.931 0.965 0.971
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman students between the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020. There are
78 instructors who teach an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same course in the same semester. Robust standard errors in
parentheses are clustered at the individual and course-by-term levels (5,029 clusters at the individual level and 311 clusters for section-by-term level).
* p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
32
Table 6: Effect of Assigned Early Morning Classes on Students’ Class Experiences
Panel 1
Class Participation Classmate Participation Increase Critical Thinking Class Motivate Learning Learn More in Diff. Section
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section -0.124** -0.00835 -0.0220 -0.0966* 0.247***
(0.0518) (0.0635) (0.0622) (0.0560) (0.0548)
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y Y
Course-Time FE Y Y Y Y
Course Request Controls Y Y Y Y
Demographic Controls Y Y Y Y
N 34,783 34,783 34,783 34,783
R2 0.272 0.213 0.309 0.110
Dependent Variable Mean 3.21 0.770 0.506 0.0823
Notes: Observations are from domestic non-athlete freshman cohort from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring
2020. Observations are higher than my regression sample in my previous results because in this regression, I
estimate the effect of attending subsequent classes on all undergraduate-level courses with multiple sections
taught by the same instructors from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020. It means that courses with
multiple sections offered in the non-early morning period, late in the morning or in the afternoon would be
included in this analysis. Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the individual and section-
by-term levels (9,664 clusters at the individual level and 1701 clusters for section-by-term level). * p < 0.10,
** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
34
Table 8: Effect on Leave-One-Out Term GPA
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 8,981 8,981 8,981
Dependent Variable Mean 3.21 3.21 3.21
Panel 2: Reduced Form Estimates
(1) (2) (3)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section 0.0115 0.0235 0.0191
(0.0331) (0.0286) (0.0250)
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 8,981 8,981 8,981
R2 0.057 0.072 0.147
Dependent Variable Mean 3.21 3.21 3.21
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman students between the
age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020. The number of
observation is 8,981 that is lower than the number of observation (9,020) in
Table 5 because 39 students only took one course. There are 78 instructors
who teach an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections of the
same course in the same semester. Robust standard errors in parentheses are
clustered at the individual and course-by-term levels (5,029 clusters at the
individual level and 311 clusters for section-by-term level). * p < 0.10, **
p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
35
Table 9: Effect on STEM Courses within the Next 2 Terms
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 5,118 5,118 5,118
Dependent Variable Mean 0.260 0.260 0.260
Panel 2: Reduced Form Estimates
(1) (2) (3)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section -0.0414** -0.0491*** -0.0535***
(0.0190) (0.0179) (0.0172)
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 5,118 5,118 5,118
R2 0.070 0.093 0.089
Dependent Variable Mean 0.260 0.260 0.260
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the age
of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020. There are 61 instructors
who teach an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same
course in the same semester. Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered
at the individual and section-by-term levels (4,088 clusters at the individual level
and 166 clusters for section-by-term level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
36
Table 10: Heterogeneous Effect on Taking Corresponding STEM Courses within the Next Two Term
Course-Instructor-Term-term FE Y Y Y Y Y
Course Request Controls Y Y Y Y Y
Demographic Controls Y Y Y Y Y
N 5,118 5,118 5,118 5,118 5,118
R2 0.089 0.090 0.090 0.090 0.089
Dependent Variable Mean 0.260 0.260 0.260 0.260 0.260
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring
2020. There are 61 instructors who teach an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same course in the same
semester. Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the individual and course-by-term levels (4,088 clusters at the
individual level and 166 clusters for section-by-term level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
Table 11: Effect on Choice of Major
Course-Instructor FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 3,912 3,912 3,912
Dependent Variable Mean 0.0179 0.0179 0.0179
Panel 2: Reduced Form Estimates
(1) (2) (3)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section -0.00973 -0.0109* -0.0124*
(0.00669) (0.00652) (0.00656)
Course-Instructor FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 3,912 3,912 3,912
R2 0.281 0.287 0.292
Dependent Variable Mean 0.0179 0.0179 0.0179
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the
age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018. There are 40 instructors who teach an early-
morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same course in the
same semester. Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the
individual and section levels (2,812 clusters at the individual level and 237
clusters for section level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
38
Table 12: Heterogeneous Effect on Choice of Major
Course-Instructor FE Y Y Y Y Y
Course Request Controls Y Y Y Y Y
Demographic Controls Y Y Y Y Y
N 3,912 3,912 3,912 3,912 3,912
R2 0.291 0.311 0.311 0.303 0.303
Dependent Variable Mean 0.0179 0.0179 0.0179 0.0179 0.292
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018. There are 40
instructors who teach an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same course in the same semester. In
column (4), I use white students as the reference group, and Other refers to Native American students and students with
non-disclosure ethnicity. Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the individual and course-by-term levels (2,812
clusters at the individual level and 237 clusters for section-by-term level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
Table 13: Effect on Choice of STEM Major
Course-Instructor FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls Y Y Y
Demographic Controls Y Y Y
N 3,912 2,555 1,357
R2 0.292 0.448 0.095
Dependent Variable Mean 0.0177 0.0164 0.0209
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between
the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018. There are 40 instructors who teach
an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same
course in the same semester. Robust standard errors in parentheses are
clustered at the individual and section levels (2,812, 1,995, and 1,293
clusters at the individual level and 237, 90, and 147 clusters for section
level in columns 1, 2, and 3 respectively). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, ***
p < 0.01
40
Table 14: Effect on Choice of Major (College Level)
Course-Instructor FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 3,912 3,912 3,912
Dependent Variable Mean 0.104 0.104 0.104
Panel 2: Reduced Form Estimates
(1) (2) (3)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section -0.0207* -0.0210* -0.0216*
(0.0112) (0.0122) (0.0123)
Course-Instructor FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 3,912 3,912 3,912
R2 0.198 0.209 0.225
Dependent Variable Mean 0.104 0.104 0.104
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the
age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018. There are 40 instructors who teach an early-
morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same course in the
same semester. Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the
individual and section levels (2,844 clusters at the individual level and 237
clusters for section level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
41
Table 15: Heterogeneous Effect on Choice of Major (Department Level)
Course-Instructor FE Y Y Y Y Y
Course Request Controls Y Y Y Y Y
Demographic Controls Y Y Y Y Y
N 3,912 3,912 3,912 3,912 3,912
R2 0.225 0.225 0.225 0.225 0.225
Dependent Variable Mean 0.104 0.104 0.104 0.104 0.104
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018. There are 40
instructors who teach an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same course in the same semester.
In column (4), I use white students as the reference group, and Other refers to Native American students and students
with non-disclosure ethnicity. Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the individual and course-by-term
levels (2,844 clusters at the individual level and 237 clusters for section-by-term level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, ***
p < 0.01
Table 16: Effect on Choice of STEM Major (College Level)
Course-Instructor FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls Y Y Y
Demographic Controls Y Y Y
N 3,912 2,555 1,357
R2 0.225 0.260 0.234
Dependent Variable Mean 0.104 0.0881 0.135
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between
the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018. There are 40 instructors who teach
an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same
course in the same semester. Robust standard errors in parentheses are
clustered at the individual and section levels (2,812, 1,995, and 1,293
clusters at the individual level and 237, 90, and 147 clusters for section
level in columns 1, 2, and 3 respectively). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, ***
p < 0.01
43
Table 17: Effect on Major Earnings
Q1 Q2 Q3
Combined STEM Non-STEM Combined STEM Non-STEM Combined STEM Non-STEM
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section 0.0655*** 0.0805*** -0.0127 0.0138 0.0168 0.0520 0.00910 0.0133 0.0458
(0.0204) (0.0208) (0.0765) (0.0283) (0.0325) (0.0682) (0.0168) (0.0138) (0.0440)
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Course Request Controls Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
Demographic Controls Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y Y
N 3,911 2,555 1,357 3,911 2,555 1,357 3,911 2,555 1,357
R2 0.145 0.119 0.228 0.286 0.239 0.373 0.318 0.230 0.447
Dependent Variable Mean 0.252 0.276 0.206 0.500 0.551 0.405 0.743 0.807 0.620
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018. There are 40 instructors who teach an
early-morning course and also teach multiple sections of the same course in the same semester. Q1 refers to the college-major earnings that fall into
the 25th percentile major-earning distribution. In columns (1), (2), and (3), the outcome variable is an indicator for whether students graduated from
44
a college major that is under the 1st quartile major earnings. Q2 refers to the college-major earnings that fall into the 50th percentile major-earning
distribution. In columns (4), (5), and (6), the outcome variable is an indicator for whether students graduated from a college major that is under
the 2nd quartile major earnings. Q3 refers to the college-major earnings that fall into the 75th percentile major-earning distribution. In columns
(7), (8), and (9), the outcome variable is an indicator for whether students graduated from a college major that is under the 3rd quartile major
earnings. Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the individual and section levels (2,812 at the individual level in columns (1), (4) &
(7) respectively, 1,995 at the individual level in columns (2), (5) & (8) respectively, and 1,293 clusters at the individual level in column (3), (6), & (9)
and 237 clusters for section level in columns (1), (4) & (7) respectively, 90 clusters for section level in columns (2), (5) & (8) respectively, and 147
clusters for section level in columns (3), (6), & (9) respectively). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
References
Altonji, Joseph G, Blom, Erica, & Meghir, Costas. 2012. Heterogeneity in human capital
investments: High school curriculum, college major, and careers. Annu. Rev. Econ., 4(1),
185–223.
Altonji, Joseph G, Kahn, Lisa B, & Speer, Jamin D. 2014. Trends in earnings differentials
across college majors and the changing task composition of jobs. American Economic
Review, 104(5), 387–393.
Andrews, Rodney J, Imberman, Scott A, & Lovenheim, Michael F. 2017. Risky business?
The effect of majoring in business on earnings and educational attainment. Tech. rept.
National Bureau of Economic Research.
Arcidiacono, Peter. 2004. Ability sorting and the returns to college major. Journal of
Econometrics, 121(1-2), 343–375.
Barnes, Christopher M, & Wagner, David T. 2009. Changing to daylight saving time cuts
into sleep and increases workplace injuries. Journal of applied psychology, 94(5), 1305.
Bleemer, Zachary, & Mehta, Aashish. 2021. College major restrictions and student stratifi-
cation. Available at SSRN 3981921.
Bleemer, Zachary, & Mehta, Aashish. 2022. Will studying economics make you rich? A
regression discontinuity analysis of the returns to college major. American Economic
Journal: Applied Economics, 14(2), 1–22.
Canes-Wrone, Brandice, & Rosen, Harvey S. 1994. Following in her footsteps? Women’s
choices of college majors and faculty gender composition.
Cardinali, Daniel P. 2008. Chronoeducation: How the biological clock influences the learning
process. The educated brain: Essays in neuroeducation, 110–26.
Carrell, Scott E, Fullerton, Richard L, & West, James E. 2009. Does your cohort matter?
Measuring peer effects in college achievement. Journal of Labor Economics, 27(3), 439–
464.
Carrell, Scott E, Maghakian, Teny, & West, James E. 2011. A’s from Zzzz’s? The causal
45
effect of school start time on the academic achievement of adolescents. American Economic
Journal: Economic Policy, 3(3), 62–81.
Carskadon, Mary A, Vieira, Cecilia, & Acebo, Christine. 1993. Association between puberty
and delayed phase preference. Sleep, 16(3), 258–262.
Chevalier, Arnaud. 2011. Subject choice and earnings of UK graduates. Economics of
Education Review, 30(6), 1187–1201.
Cosgrave, Jan, Wulff, Katharina, & Gehrman, Philip. 2018. Sleep, circadian rhythms, and
schizophrenia: where we are and where we need to go. Current opinion in psychiatry,
31(3), 176–182.
Crowley, Stephanie J, Acebo, Christine, & Carskadon, Mary A. 2007. Sleep, circadian
rhythms, and delayed phase in adolescence. Sleep medicine, 8(6), 602–612.
Dee, Thomas S. 2004. Teachers, race, and student achievement in a randomized experiment.
Review of economics and statistics, 86(1), 195–210.
Dee, Thomas S. 2005. A teacher like me: Does race, ethnicity, or gender matter? American
Economic Review, 95(2), 158–165.
Denning, Jeffrey T, & Turley, Patrick. 2017. Was that SMART? Institutional financial
incentives and field of study. Journal of Human Resources, 52(1), 152–186.
Diette, Timothy M, & Raghav, Manu. 2017. Does early bird catch the worm or a lower
GPA? Evidence from a liberal arts college. Applied Economics, 49(33), 3341–3350.
Dills, Angela K, & Hernandez-Julian, Rey. 2008. Course scheduling and academic perfor-
mance. Economics of Education Review, 27(6), 646–654.
Edwards, Finley. 2012. Early to rise? The effect of daily start times on academic performance.
Economics of Education Review, 31(6), 970–983.
Ehrenberg, Ronald G, & Brewer, Dominic J. 1995. Did teachers’ verbal ability and race
matter in the 1960s? Coleman revisited. Economics of Education Review, 14(1), 1–21.
Ehrenberg, Ronald G, Goldhaber, Daniel D, & Brewer, Dominic J. 1995. Do teachers’
race, gender, and ethnicity matter? Evidence from the National Educational Longitudinal
46
Study of 1988. ILR Review, 48(3), 547–561.
Garcı́a, Aı́da, Ramı́rez, Candelaria, Martı́nez, Benito, & Valdez, Pablo. 2012. Circadian
rhythms in two components of executive functions: cognitive inhibition and flexibility.
Biological Rhythm Research, 43(1), 49–63.
Goldstein, David, Hahn, Constanze S, Hasher, Lynn, Wiprzycka, Ursula J, & Zelazo,
Philip David. 2007. Time of day, intellectual performance, and behavioral problems in
morning versus evening type adolescents: Is there a synchrony effect? Personality and
individual Differences, 42(3), 431–440.
Groen, Jeffrey A, & Pabilonia, Sabrina Wulff. 2019. Snooze or lose: High school start times
and academic achievement. Economics of Education Review, 72, 204–218.
Haggag, Kareem, Pope, Devin G, Bryant-Lees, Kinsey B, & Bos, Maarten W. 2019. Attri-
bution bias in consumer choice. The Review of Economic Studies, 86(5), 2136–2183.
Haggag, Kareem, Patterson, Richard W, Pope, Nolan G, & Feudo, Aaron. 2021. Attribution
bias in major decisions: Evidence from the United States Military Academy. Journal of
Public Economics, 200, 104445.
Hasler, Brant P, Soehner, Adriane M, & Clark, Duncan B. 2014. Circadian rhythms and
risk for substance use disorders in adolescence. Current opinion in psychiatry, 27(6), 460.
Hastings, Justine S, Neilson, Christopher A, & Zimmerman, Seth D. 2013. Are some degrees
worth more than others? Evidence from college admission cutoffs in Chile. Tech. rept.
National Bureau of Economic Research.
Hoffmann, Florian, & Oreopoulos, Philip. 2009. A professor like me the influence of instructor
gender on college achievement. Journal of human resources, 44(2), 479–494.
Horne, Jim, & Reyner, Louise. 1999. Vehicle accidents related to sleep: a review. Occupa-
tional and environmental medicine, 56(5), 289–294.
Jagnani, Maulik. 2018. Poor sleep: Sunset time and human capital production. Mimeo.
Kecklund, Göran, & Axelsson, John. 2016. Health consequences of shift work and insufficient
sleep. Bmj, 355.
47
Kirkeboen, Lars J, Leuven, Edwin, & Mogstad, Magne. 2016. Field of study, earnings, and
self-selection. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 131(3), 1057–1111.
Klopfenstein, Kristin. 2005. Beyond test scores: The impact of Black teacher role models on
rigorous math taking. Contemporary Economic Policy, 23(3), 416–428.
Müller, Tomáš, & Murray, Keith. 2010. Comprehensive approach to student sectioning.
Annals of Operations Research, 181(1), 249–269.
Nakata, Akinori, Ikeda, Tomoko, Takahashi, Masaya, Haratani, Takashi, Fujioka, Yosei,
Fukui, Satoe, Swanson, Naomi G, Hojou, Minoru, & Araki, Shunichi. 2005. Sleep-related
risk of occupational injuries in Japanese small and medium-scale enterprises. Industrial
health, 43(1), 89–97.
Pandi-Perumal, Seithikurippu R, Trakht, Ilya, Srinivasan, Venkataramanujan, Spence,
D Warren, Maestroni, Georges JM, Zisapel, Nava, & Cardinali, Daniel P. 2008. Physio-
logical effects of melatonin: role of melatonin receptors and signal transduction pathways.
Progress in neurobiology, 85(3), 335–353.
Patnaik, Arpita, Venator, Joanna, Wiswall, Matthew, & Zafar, Basit. 2020. The Role of
Heterogeneous Risk Preferences, Discount Rates, and Earnings Expectations in College
Major Choice. Tech. rept. National Bureau of Economic Research.
Persson, Jonas, Welsh, Kathryn M, Jonides, John, & Reuter-Lorenz, Patricia A. 2007. Cog-
nitive fatigue of executive processes: Interaction between interference resolution tasks.
Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 1571–1579.
Pope, Nolan G. 2016. How the time of day affects productivity: Evidence from school
schedules. Review of Economics and Statistics, 98(1), 1–11.
Price, Joshua. 2010. The effect of instructor race and gender on student persistence in STEM
fields. Economics of Education Review, 29(6), 901–910.
Rask, Kevin N, & Bailey, Elizabeth M. 2002. Are faculty role models? Evidence from
major choice in an undergraduate institution. The Journal of Economic Education, 33(2),
99–124.
48
Robb, Roberta Edgecombe, & Robb, A Leslie. 1999. Gender and the study of economics:
The role of gender of the instructor. The Journal of Economic Education, 30(1), 3–19.
Robst, John, Keil, Jack, & Russo, Dean. 1998. The effect of gender composition of faculty
on student retention. Economics of Education Review, 17(4), 429–439.
Schmidt, Christina, Collette, Fabienne, Cajochen, Christian, & Peigneux, Philippe. 2007. A
time to think: circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive neuropsychology, 24(7),
755–789.
Sjoquist, David L, & Winters, John V. 2015. State merit aid programs and college major:
A focus on STEM. Journal of Labor Economics, 33(4), 973–1006.
Van Den Berg, Johannes, & Neely, Gregory. 2006. Performance on a simple reaction time
task while sleep deprived. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102(2), 589–599.
Webber, Douglas A. 2014a. The lifetime earnings premia of different majors: Correcting for
selection based on cognitive, noncognitive, and unobserved factors. Labour economics, 28,
14–23.
Webber, Douglas A. 2014b. The lifetime earnings premia of different majors: Correcting for
selection based on cognitive, noncognitive, and unobserved factors. Labour economics, 28,
14–23.
Williams, Kevin M, & Shapiro, Teny Maghakian. 2018. Academic achievement across the
day: Evidence from randomized class schedules. Economics of Education Review, 67,
158–170.
Wolfson, Amy R, & Carskadon, Mary A. 1998. Sleep schedules and daytime functioning in
adolescents. Child development, 69(4), 875–887.
Wolfson, Amy R, & Carskadon, Mary A. 2005. A survey of factors influencing high school
starttimes. NASSP Bulletin, 89(642), 47–66.
49
Appendix I Batch Registration
The objective of the batch registration is to maximize satisfy students’ course request prefer-
ences subject to the number of courses and sections the university offers, classroom capacity,
and physical distances between classes. Students with similar course requests are grouped
together. Then, the algorithm works in 6 phases:
1. The algorithm orders students based on the number of sections available for the courses
they requested and assigns course sections to them.
2. Students without a complete schedule are taken in random order and are assigned
sections.
5. Students are selected randomly and try to fill in any available sections at that point if
all their requests are unassigned.
Interested readers can refer to (Müller & Murray, 2010) for more information about the
batch registration at Purdue University. The figure below also illustrates what information
students need to fill out for their course submission.
50
Figure A.1: Course Request Form
51
Appendix II Student Survey
I conduct a field survey to Purdue students about their in-class experiences from an early
morning and a non-early morning classes in Fall 2022. The email message about the survey
and the online survey via Qualtrics are attached in the following pages. This activity has
been approved (RCT ID: AEARCTR-0010038) by the AEA RCT Registry.
52
Dear Students from ECON 252,
I am Anthony Yim, a Purdue PhD student in economics. Dr. Victoria Prowse and I would like to invite you
to participate in a research survey (IRB-2022-874).
The purpose of this survey is to help researchers understand student experiences from ECON 252
Macroeconomics. It will take 2-3 minutes to complete the survey. After completing the survey, you can
choose to opt in to a raffle with the chance (up to 50% chance) to win gift cards.
The survey is anonymous and strictly voluntary and would not affect your course grades. Professor
Vargas will not know who will participate in the survey and survey responses because collected data will
not be shared with him. Dr. Prowse and I are the only people to receive and review the responses and
associated Purdue emails if students choose to participate in a lottery to win a prize. Responses are not
associated with emails.
If you are interested to participate in this survey, please click on the following link:
We would also advice interested participants to take the survey outside of class time.
Best wishes,
Anthony
9/19/22, 10:37 AM Qualtrics Survey Software
Select the option that best describes how you feel about each statement.
Notice that the statements only focus on your experiences during
class.
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
https://purdue.yul1.qualtrics.com/Q/EditSection/Blocks/Ajax/GetSurveyPrintPreview?ContextSurveyID=SV_9oVzdz4R5eOZNSm&ContextLibraryID=U… 1/6
9/19/22, 10:37 AM Qualtrics Survey Software
Strongly Disagree
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
6. I would learn more from a different section of ECON 252 with the same
instructor.
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
https://purdue.yul1.qualtrics.com/Q/EditSection/Blocks/Ajax/GetSurveyPrintPreview?ContextSurveyID=SV_9oVzdz4R5eOZNSm&ContextLibraryID=U… 2/6
9/19/22, 10:37 AM Qualtrics Survey Software
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
Strongly Agree
https://purdue.yul1.qualtrics.com/Q/EditSection/Blocks/Ajax/GetSurveyPrintPreview?ContextSurveyID=SV_9oVzdz4R5eOZNSm&ContextLibraryID=U… 3/6
9/19/22, 10:37 AM Qualtrics Survey Software
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
Strongly Agree
Agree
Neither Agree or Disagree
Disagree
Strongly Disagree
BASIC INFORMATION
Freshman
Sophomore
Junior
Senior
Graduate
Other
Male
Female
Prefer not to answer
https://purdue.yul1.qualtrics.com/Q/EditSection/Blocks/Ajax/GetSurveyPrintPreview?ContextSurveyID=SV_9oVzdz4R5eOZNSm&ContextLibraryID=U… 4/6
9/19/22, 10:37 AM Qualtrics Survey Software
Yes
No
Prefer not to answer
major
minor
general education
Economics
Business major (excluding economics)
Other
7:30 am - 8:20 am
9:30 am - 10:20 am
Neither. I joined the course after the start of the semester.
https://purdue.yul1.qualtrics.com/Q/EditSection/Blocks/Ajax/GetSurveyPrintPreview?ContextSurveyID=SV_9oVzdz4R5eOZNSm&ContextLibraryID=U… 5/6
9/19/22, 10:37 AM Qualtrics Survey Software
7:30 am - 8:20 am
9:30 am - 10:20 am
Neither of them
Both
20. How often do you usually attend classes of ECON 252 each week?
0 times
1 time
2 times
3 times
More than 3 times
Block 1
Would you like to enter the raffle to win a prize? Your response will still remain
anonymous.
Yes
No
Powered by Qualtrics
https://purdue.yul1.qualtrics.com/Q/EditSection/Blocks/Ajax/GetSurveyPrintPreview?ContextSurveyID=SV_9oVzdz4R5eOZNSm&ContextLibraryID=U… 6/6
Appendix III Regression Models
After verifying that assignments to first period 7:30 AM classes are random, I can move
forward with the IV estimation models to estimate the impact of early morning classes on
students’ education outcomes. Previously, I discussed the course registration process at
Purdue University in which students can still make changes after they receive initial class
schedules. Since all students are not compliers into the treatment shown in Table A.1, I test
the strength of the instrument by estimating the first-stage regression with the following
equation:
where Finalizedicpt is an indicator for whether student i in class c with instructor p at term
t enroll in an early morning class. σ is course-by-instructor-by-term fixed effect, addressing
a potential concern that assignments of instructors’ teaching schedules are endogenous since
they are not random.
For Assigned Earlyicpt to be a valid instrument under the Local Average Treatment Effect
(LATE) framework, there are four identification assumptions:
1. Relevance: Assigned early morning sections has a causal effect on finalized early morn-
ing sections conditional on course request preferences.
3. Exclusion Restriction: Assigned early morning sections only affect the outcome vari-
ables of interest through finalized early morning sections.
4. Monotonicity: Getting assigned to early morning classes either increases the likelihood
of actually enrolling in early morning classes or does nothing, but it does not decrease
the likelihood of actually enrolling in early morning classes. That also means that the
students are all always takers, never takers or compliers with no defiers.
60
Appendix IV Tables
Course by Instructor
Overall Early Non-Early N Mean Diff. P-value
STEM Sample
0.857 0.873 0.854 6,551 0.0190* 0.071
Major Sample
0.845 0.842 0.846 5,094 -0.00419 0.753
Notes: The observations in both upper and lower panels are in course-by-instructor-by-term
level. The upper panel displays the compliance rates from domestic freshman cohort from Fall
2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020 in my STEM-course analyses. I call those observations as
“STEM Sample”. The lower panel displays the compliance rates from domestic freshman co-
hort from Fall 2018 in my major-choice analyses. I call those observations as “Major Sample”.
* p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01.
61
Table A.2: Student-by-Course-by-Term Level Balance Test (Alternative Specifications)
Course-Term FE N Y Y
Course Preference Controls N Y Y
Number of Courses Requested FE N N Y
Notes: Robust standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the individ-
ual and course-by-term levels (6,595 clusters at the individual level and 41
clusters for course-by-term level). SAT combined is a standardized variable
with mean of zero and standard deviation of one. In this specification, I
use the course-by-term fixed effect in this specification instead of course-by-
instructor-by-term fixed effect in Table 2. Observations are domestic non-
athlete freshman cohort between the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall
2019, and Spring 2020. * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
62
Table A.3: Courses in the Three Samples
Course Grade Sample STEM Course Sample Major Choice Sample
Course Title Enrollment Early AM Course Title Enrollment Early AM Course Title Enrollment Early AM
Basic Aircraft Science 82 59 Basic Aircraft Science 45 33 Basic Aircraft Science 38 27
Fundamentals Of Biology I 1911 575 Fundamentals Of Biology I 1343 392 Fundamentals Of Biology I 570 183
Fundamentals Of Biology 6 2 Fundamentals Of Biology 6 2 Human Anatomy and Physiology 264 129
Human Anatomy And Physiology 777 334 Human Anatomy And Physiology 513 205 Fundamentals Of Speech Communication 608 65
Fundamentals Of Speech Communication 608 65 Macroeconomics 77 76 Macroeconomics 75 72
63
Macroeconomics 152 148 Functions And Trigonometry 947 115 Exploring Teaching As A Career 43 18
Exploring Teaching As A Career 43 18 Applied Calculus I 2056 162 Multiculturalism And Education 31 9
Multiculturalism And Education 31 9 Applied Calculus II 64 15 First-Year Composition 496 25
First-Year Composition 496 25 Statistics And Society 138 106 French Level III 8 2
French Level III 8 2 Functions And Trigonometry 434 48 Applied Calculus I 1019 71
Functions And Trigonometry 1376 161 Applied Calculus I 3073 233 Applied Calculus II 58 14
Applied Calculus I 3073 233 Applied Calculus II 122 29 Spanish Level III 161 5
Applied Calculus II 122 29 Spanish Level III 161 5 Spanish Level IV 25 5
Spanish Level III 161 5 Statistics And Society 133 98
Statistics And Society 271 204
Table A.4: Mapping Between Courses and Majors
64
Table A.5: Mapping Between Courses and Colleges
65
Table A.6: Effect on Standardized Course Grades
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 9,020 9,020 9,020
Panel 2: OLS Estimates
(1) (2) (3)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section -0.0667 -0.0503 -0.0533**
(0.0425) (0.0349) (0.0225)
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 9,020 9,020 9,020
R2 0.119 0.123 0.234
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between
the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020. There are
78 instructors who teach an early-morning course and also teach multiple
sections of the same course in the same semester. Robust standard errors
in parentheses are clustered at the individual and course-by-term levels
(5,029 clusters at the individual level and 311 clusters for section-by-term
level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
66
Table A.7: Descriptive Statistics of the Field Survey
67
Table A.8: Effect of Assigned Early Morning Classes on Students’ Class Experiences
About Instructor
Clear Lecture -0.0327 0.0178 0.0224 -0.00889
(0.0617) (0.0335) (0.0176) (0.00909)
Engaging Lecture -0.0270 -0.0531 -0.00129 -0.00634
(0.0588) (0.0541) (0.0323) (0.0205)
Instructor Help Learning -0.0647 0.00482 0.00379 -0.00891
(0.0578) (0.0478) (0.0185) (0.00911)
Instructor Enthusiastic -0.104* -0.0404 -0.00338 -0.00889
(0.0616) (0.0403) (0.0164) (0.00909)
68
Table A.9: Effect of Assigned Early Morning Classes on Freshman Students’ Class Experiences
Panel 1
Class Participation Classmate Participation Increase Critical Thinking Class Motivate Learning Learn More in Diff. Section
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section -0.554*** -0.0603 -0.214 -0.0898 -0.00463
(0.182) (0.185) (0.167) (0.111) (0.160)
N 88 88 88 88 88 88
R2 0.184 0.175 0.276 0.210 0.197 0.277
Dependent Variable Mean 0.932 0.761 0.864 0.864 0.716 0.557
Notes: Observations are freshman students from ECON 25200 course in Fall 2022. There are two sections (7:30 AM and 9:30 AM) taught by the
same instructor. The dependent variables are indicators for whether students answer “Strongly Agree” and “Agree.” Columns (1), (2), (3), (4), and
(5) of Panel 1 are outcomes related to student’s learning; columns (1), (2), (3), and (4) of Panel 2 are outcomes related to the course instructor, and
columns (5) and (6) of Panel 2 are outcomes related to student’s interest on the course subject. ECON 25200 is an introductory macroeconomics
course offered by the Krannert School of Management at Purdue University. * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
Table A.10: Effect on Corresponding STEM Courses in the Next Term
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 5,118 5,118 5,118
Dependent Variable Mean 0.205 0.205 0.205
Panel 2: Reduced Form Estimates
(1) (2) (3)
Assigned 7:30 AM Section -0.0271 -0.0357** -0.0353**
(0.0185) (0.0178) (0.0177)
Course-Instructor-Term FE Y Y Y
Course Request Controls N Y Y
Demographic Controls N N Y
N 5,118 5,118 5,118
R2 0.044 0.055 0.057
Dependent Variable Mean 0.205 0.205 0.205
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the age
of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall 2019, and Spring 2020 in my STEM-course
analyses. There are 61 instructors who teach an early-morning course and
also teach multiple sections of the same course in the same semester. Robust
standard errors in parentheses are clustered at the individual and section-by-
term levels (4,088 clusters at the individual level and 166 clusters for section-
by-term level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
70
Table A.11: Heterogeneous Effect on Taking Corresponding STEM Courses in the Next Term
(0.0157)
1st Gen Student × 7:30 AM Section 0.0182
(0286)
Course-Instructor-Term-term FE Y Y Y Y Y
Course Request Controls Y Y Y Y Y
Demographic Controls Y Y Y Y Y
N 5,118 5,118 5,118 5,118 5,118
R2 0.057 0.058 0.058 0.057 0.057
Dependent Variable Mean 0.205 0.205 0.205 0.205 0.205
Notes: Observations are domestic non-athlete freshman cohort between the age of 18 and 21 from Fall 2018, Fall
2019, and Spring 2020. There are 61 instructors who teach an early-morning course and also teach multiple sections
of the same course in the same semester. In column (4), I use white students as the reference group, and Other refers
to Native American students and students with non-disclosure ethnicity. Robust standard errors in parentheses
are clustered at the individual and course-by-term levels (4,088 clusters at the individual level and 166 clusters for
section-by-term level). * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01