Rhythm 1 LMS
Rhythm 1 LMS
This course generally focuses on rhythm, lexical and grammatical words. The rhythm of English
speech is determined by stress as well as weakenings and reductions of vowel and consonants
sounds. Words can either be grammatical or lexical. It is important to note that in English
sentences, lexical words attract stress while grammatical or function words do not carry stress.
Since grammatical words do not carry stress, they can easily weaken into the schwa /ə/. Hence,
What is Rhythm?
Rhythm could be defined as the periodic repetition of an event while speech rhythm is the
periodic repetition of a particular event during speech. Every language in the world is spoken
with either one type of rhythm or the other. Two types of periodic events may happen during
speech and these events dictate the type of speech rhythm which a language has. These events
are the periodic repetition of syllables and the periodic repetition of stressed syllables.
The former occurs in syllable-timed languages and the latter in stress-timed languages. That is,
Syllable-timed languages
Syllable-timed languages are those in which syllables are repeated periodically as all syllables
take approximately the same amount of time during speech. For each syllable, there is a
rhythmic beat which occurs at more or less equal time intervals during each speech event.
Generally, these types of languages have two main characteristics: they have a small variety
of syllable types, and they do not have reduced vowels as each vowel sound is pronounced in
full. These two features are responsible for the fact that syllables in syllable-timed languages
are more similar to each other in duration. Syllable-timed rhythm is also referred to as
machine-gun rhythm. French, Spanish, Italian, and Yoruba are examples of syllable-timed
languages.
Stress-timed languages
Stress-timed languages have stresses occurring at approximately equal intervals. This results in
spending roughly the same amount of time between stresses, even when there are many
intervening unstressed syllables. Stressed syllables tend to occur at roughly equal intervals of
time. This is because the unstressed syllables in between give the impression of being
compressed if there are many, and expanded if there are few. English is a stress-timed language,
thus, sentence stress is the basis of rhythm in English. Stress-timing can be seen at work in the
following example:
w x y z
‘David had ‘seen ‘helicopters at the ‘airport.
Since it is stresses that occur at approximately equal intervals, stress-timed rhythm requires that
more or less the same amount of time be spent in the pronunciation of the three syllables
between w and x, for the one syllable between x and y, for the six syllables between y and z and
for the two syllables between z and the end of the utterance. It is, therefore, necessary to
compress the duration of syllables more in the stretch between y and z: ‘helicopter at the, than in
the one between w and x: ‘David had, whereas the syllable between x and y, ‘seen and the ones
after z ‘airport will be relatively long. In the sentence below, the stressed syllables are given
numbers: 1 and 2 and are not separated by any unstressed syllables, 2 and 3 are separated by one
1 2 3 4 5
´Walk ´down the ´path to the ´end of the ca ´nal
When there is a need to hurry over some word, it is mainly unstressed syllables that get
shortened. Therefore, stress-timing requirements are responsible for many of the phonetic
Words classes are sub-divided into lexical and grammatical words based on their functions. The
lexical words are nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs while grammatical words are pronouns,
Words
Lexical Grammatical
Nouns Pronouns
Verbs Preposition
Adjectives Conjunction
Adverbs Interjection
Lexical and grammatical words have distinct characteristics which distinguish one group from
the other. The lexical words are considered the major parts of speech because they consist of
content words. This means that lexical words have dictionary meanings and each of them has a
specific referent that is, an object person, process, idea or state of affairs to which it refers. For
example: book, house, announcement, sleep, run, slowly, etc. Also, lexical words constitute the
class known as the “open-system” class of words. This means that these groups of words are
open in the sense that the group can be extended by creating additional members. This is why the
list of nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs can never be exhaustive as new words are constantly
Grammatical words belong to the group generally known as the function words because their
main job is to create grammatical structures and express grammatical ideas. Grammatical words
belong to a group known as “closed-class” items because their group cannot be increased.
Therefore, it is possible to list all the pronouns, prepositions, conjunctions and interjections of a
language.
Lexical words can also be referred to as open-class items. In English pronunciation, lexical
words attract stress while grammatical words do not carry stress. Therefore, English is said to be
a stress-timed language. That is, stress is placed on lexical words and the time spent on stressed
syllables occurs at approximately equal intervals. The time spent on a stressed syllable is the
same not taking into consideration the unstressed syllable before the next stressed syllable. For
example:
´Beat her.
the type of speech rhythm which a language has. These events are the periodic
repetition of syllables and the periodic repetition of stressed syllables. The former
Rhythmic Stress
When an English word has more than one syllable and when it is pronounced in isolation, that is,
when it is in its citation form, one of its syllables will be more prominent and audible than the
others. This most audible syllable bears the main lexical stress or accent of the word. When
words are put together into phrases or sentences in connected speech, some words retain their
lexical stress and others lose it. In connected speech, one-syllable words can bear stress. Look at
In this utterance, there are three stressed syllables: fight, cat and dog.
These all happen to one-syllable words. The words of, and and the do not bear stress at all,
Moreover, a word may bear stress when it appears in connected speech on a different syllable
from that which carries the main lexical stress when the word is said in isolation.
The stresses we marked in the phrase above the cat and the dog and in afternoon tea are not
lexical stresses but sentence stresses or rhythmic stresses. As we have seen, the two types of
stress do not always coincide. In this book, when we use the term stress, we mean rhythmic
stress, unless we explicitly say otherwise. We will not use the word accent to refer to syllable
prominence at all.
As we have already said, when words are in isolation, that is, out of context, we use their citation
forms. In that case, all words have at least one stressed syllable. However, some words may not
be stressed in connected speech and there are words which are rarely stressed. These words
which are not usually stressed are words that have little lexical meaning (grammatical or form
words). Propositions, pronouns, auxiliary and modal verbs, conjunction and articles are
grammatical words. In the other hand, words which often keep the stress in connected speech
because they carry considerable semantic weight (lexical or content words) are nouns, main
Phonetic weakenings and reductions mostly affect unstressed syllables. Sounds in unstressed
syllables are frequently weakened, for instance by shortenings, elisions etc. In English, these
syllables, particularly in vowels, and are now the usual pronunciation of words. For instance,
historical /ˈɔ:fʊl/ for awful is no longer the usual pronunciation; its reduced form is much more
likely: /ˈɔ:fəl/. In some grammatical words, on the other hand, both the full and reduced
pronunciations co-exist. The choice between these pronunciations depends largely on whether
Because grammatical words are usually unstressed, their reduced or weak pronunciations are
very frequent, even more so than their full forms. There is, accordingly, a weak, normal
pronunciation and a full, strong one which is used when the word is stressed for some reason and
in some other circumstances which we shall see below. These pronunciations are known as the
Not all grammatical words have an alternation between weak and strong pronunciations. For
instance, only monosyllabic grammatical words have a weak form. Conversely, some words
Changes affecting grammatical words are systematic since they follow general language rules
and properties of sounds. For instance, the more centralized a vowel, the weaker it is. The
Weak form is also known as the ……………… while the strong form is known as …………..
Weak form is also known as the unaccented form while the strong is known as the accented
form.
Vowel changes
Weakening makes all vowels move to the centre of the vowel space. They will first move to the
Vowel changes
STRONG VOWEL weak vowel Example
/ɒ/ /ə/ Of
In the case of grammatical words which have the vowel /ʊ/ in their citation form, such as could,
they can stay unchanged when unstressed since this vowel is already weak, or they may be
By the same token, grammatical words which have the vowel /u:/ in their strong form may go a
further step in weakening, which make the vowel /ʊ/ go to /ə/, for example:
The last pronunciation of these words (which is only possible when the following sound is a
consonant as we shall see below) is considered to be very informal in RP, but is usual in other
varieties of English
8.2.2 Consonant changes
Consonant changes are not an intrinsic part of weak forms but an optional step in a scale of
weakening which depends on the speech register that is used. Many of the consonant changes
that we will mention are not exclusive to weak forms. They can be seen to occur in lexical
words, too, depending on the phonetic context and register. Nevertheless, grammatical words are
favourable environments for these changes. The following changes are very often found in weak
forms:
/h/ may be elided if it is not at the very beginning of the utterance (following a potential
pause):
but not in
/d/ and /t/ may be elided when at the end of a word and preceded by another consonant:
The table below lists the most common words which have strong and weak forms in RP English.
Notice that all these words consist of a single syllable and that they nearly all belong to one of
The symbol /ḷ/ and /ṇ/ in this table represent syllabic consonants.
Use of weak forms in RP
Grammatical words in connected speech are used in their weak form most of the time but take
(I). When the word is stressed because of emphasis or contrast, the strong form is
compulsory:
/ənd/ /’ӕnd/ in the sentence: I didn’t say apples or pears, I said apples and pears.
(II). When prepositions and auxiliary verbs appears in grammatical structures such as the
You were later than I was this morning. /wɒz/ not /wəz/
He can sing well, but I can too. /kӕn/ not /kən/ or /kṇ/
They were being looked for by the police. /fɔ:/ not /fə/
The underlined words above are not likely to bear stress, but nevertheless appear in the strong
form. The reason is that a word which normally follows the underlined preposition or auxiliary
verb has either been deleted or moved to some other position in the sentence, leaving the
auxiliary or preposition behind. The auxiliary or preposition is said to be stranded. Take the first
sentence, for example. The word at, being a preposition, is normally followed by a noun or noun
phrase which it is said to govern. The noun phrase which at governs in this sentence is the
picture. Because of the grammatical structure used, this phrase does not immediately follow the
preposition, therefore, the proposition is stranded. In the second and third sentences, the verb was
and can are not followed by an adjective or a verb, respectively. They have been deleted in order
to avoid repetition. Again, the auxiliaries are stranded. Stranding often takes place at the end of
the sentence, but not always, as you can see from some of the sentences above.
One final detail about stranding is that the auxiliary verb have in structures where it is
immediately preceded by another auxiliary, such as can’t, could, couldn’t, must, mustn’t, should,
shouldn’t, will, won’t, would, wouldn’t, is normally used in its weak form even if it is stranded.
word strong form weak form Word strong form weak form
It is important to note that when we write, we leave gaps between words so it is easy for the
reader to process the text. However, when we speak, it is unnecessary to leave pauses between
words much of the time as it would sound unnatural if we did. This is owing to the fact that
segments affect each other within individual words and this influence also operates across word
boundaries. Therefore, differences often exist between the pronunciation of words in their isolate
form and their pronunciation in connected speech because they are subject to influences from
other surrounding sounds and from larger accentual and rhythmic patterns. The differences may
affect the word as a whole, (for instance, weak forms in an accented situation) or they may
concern a word’s accentual pattern or sounds used at the word boundaries. This variation
between isolate forms and context-influenced forms depends on: (a) casual or formal nature of
speech; (b) the speed of delivery; and (c) the tendency to preserve a form near to that of the
isolate word.
1. Assimilation
2. Elision
3. Liaison
Assimilation
What is assimilation?
In recent years, computer technology has led to big improvements in speech production, but the
inadequacies of ‘mechanical speech’ that is, the machines designed to join words together to
form sentences, has brought about the teaching and learning of pronunciation. This device
mentioned above that produces mechanical speech would contain all words in English, each
having been recorded in isolation. A significant difference in natural connected speech is the way
that sounds belonging to one word can cause changes in sounds belonging to neighbouring
words. Hence, assuming we know how the phonemes of a particular word would be realised
when the word is pronounced in isolation, in cases where we find a phoneme realised differently
as a result of being near some other phoneme, we call this difference an instance of assimilation.
Assimilation is a process whereby a sound segment becomes identical with or more like another
conditioning where one phoneme is replaced by a second under the influence of a third.
Phonetic conditioning is a term used to cover the way in which speech segments are influenced
phonetic context. Technically, assimilation is a process whereby segments become more like the
segments which precede or follow them. Assimilation is something which varies in extent
according to speaking rate and style: it is found in rapid, casual speech and less likely in slow,
careful speech. For instance, the word broadcast, which is pronounced /ˈbrɔ:dkɑ:st/ in careful
speech but which in rapid speech may well become /ˈbrɔ:gkɑ:st/. Here, one phoneme /d/ has
been replaced by a second /g/, under the influence of a third /k/. We can distinguish here two
forms of the word broad: (1) /brɔ:d/ (2) /brɔ:g/ where form 1 can be considered as the ideal
form, which corresponds to the target that the native speakers have in their minds and it is what
is produced in the slowest and more careful style of speech. It bears a close resemblance to the
spelling representation. Form 2 is more typical of connected speech and is termed the
assimilated form. Also, good boy for example, will be pronounced /gʊdˈbɔi/ in the slowest and
careful style. However, in connected speech, it will be produced as /gʊbˈbɔi/. The ideal form of
the word good is /gʊd/ while the assimilated form is /gʊb/. Then, the phoneme /d/ has been
Assimilation can be classified according to direction which is left to right or right to left. This is
referred to as the direction of influence. We have three types of assimilation based on the
direction of influence.
of influence that can be defined as features of an articulation which leads into those of the
following segments. That is, the feature of one sound anticipated in the articulation of
the preceding sound. This is a movement from right to left. For example:
However, there are certain order in which the regressive assimilation occurs in
English:
/t/ changes to /p/ before /p. b, m/ (e.g. that pen /ծӕp ˈpen/)
/d/ changes to /b/ before /p, b, m/ (e.g. good man /gʊb ˈmӕn/)
/d/ changes to /g/ before /k, g/ (e.g. good concert / gʊg ˈkɒnsət/)
/n/ changes to /m/ before /p, b, m/ (e.g. ten players /tem ˈpleɪəz/)
/n/ changes to /ŋ/ before /k, g/ (e.g. ten cups /teŋ ˈkʌps/)
Say these English words and phrases, first following the transcription in
direction of influence that moves from the left to right. The progressive assimilation is
relatively uncommon. Here, the articulation features may be held over from a preceding
segment, so that the articulators lag in their movements. In other words, one sound
Organ /ˈɔ:gŋ/
In the example above, /n/ changes to /ŋ/ after /g/
However, there are certain order in which the progressive assimilation occurs in
English:
/n/ changes to /ŋ/ after /k, g/ (e.g. second chance /sekg ˈʧaɪns/)
3. Coalescence assimilation is when two segments merge into one. It is a process where
phonological features of both X and Y. For example: in connected speech, haven’t you is
likely to be pronounced / hӕvənʧu: / with the /t/ at the end of haven’t and the /j/ at the
Transcribe the word happen in the ideal and connected forms and explain the type of
In many cases, there is a two-way exchange of articulation features which is known as the
reciprocal assimilation. For example, we can have raise your glass as /ˈreɪz jɔ: ˈglɑ:s/ becomes
Assimilation can also be classified based on the types of influence. We can identify assimilation
place of articulation are termed place assimilation. For instance, final alveolar in ideal forms are
often replaced by bilabials (preceding /p b m/) or velars (preceding /k/) or palatals (preceding
/ʃ/), e.g. wet blanket /wet ˈblӕŋkɪt/ becomes /wep ˈblӕŋkɪt/. Place assimilation is very common
in English.
Assimilation may involve a reduction of fortis/lenis contrast which is a type known as the
energy or voicing assimilation. In stressed syllables, energy assimilations are less frequent in
English than in most other languages, but It is important to note that assimilation of different
kinds may occur simultaneously. That is, both place and manner assimilation might occur in a
word or phrases. Also, more than one phoneme may be affected by assimilation. This is known
/bəˈhainʤu:/. Here, both place and manner of assimilation affects /d/ and /j/ of the ideal form.
What is elision?
The nature of elision may be stated quite simply: under certain circumstances sounds disappear.
One might express this in more technical language by saying that in certain circumstances, a
phoneme may be realised as zero, or have zero realisation. Elision may be defined as the
change from ideal form in connected speech which involves the deletion of a phoneme. In rapid
speech, sounds may be left out or elided, especially when they occur as part of a cluster of
consonants or when identical segments occur in a sequence and this happens frequently. Apart
from word-internal elisions and those associated with weak forms, sounds may also be elided in
fast, colloquial speech, especially at or in the vicinity of word boundaries. In connected speech,
using the examples of next day and prime minister, the /t/ at the end of next will be lost before
day and the /m/ at the end of prime will lost before /m/ at the start of minister. Hence, the term
examples of elision or not. The fact that they are regularly represented with special
spelling forms makes them seem rather different from the above examples. The best-
‘had’, ‘would’: spelt ‘d, pronounced /d/ (after vowels), /ed/ (after consonants)
‘is’, ‘has’: spelt ‘s, pronounced /s/ (after fortis consonants), /z/ (after lenis
consonants), except that after /s, z, ʃ, ʧ, ʤ, ʒ/ ‘is’ is pronounced /ɪz/ and ‘has’ is
‘will’: spelt ‘ll, pronounced /l/ (after vowels), /l/ (after consonants)
‘have’: spelt ‘ve, pronounced /v/ (after vowels), /əv/ (after consonants)
‘not’: spelt n’t, pronounced /nt/ (after vowels), /nt/ (after consonants)
‘are’: spelt ‘re, pronounced /ə/ (after vowels) and contracted ‘are’ is also
8.5.3: Liaison
What is liaison?
The converse of elision is liaison. That is, liaison is the insertion of an extra sound in
order to facilitate the articulation of a sequence. Liaison is the term used for the process
The accents of English can be divided into two groups according to /r/ distribution: (a)
rhotic accents where /r/ is pronounced in all contexts (i.e. before a consonant and at the
end of an utterance) and; (b) non-rhotic accents where /r/ is pronounced only when it
The linking /r/ is the pronunciation of the final r in a word that is immediately followed by
another word that begins with a vowel sound. It involves the restoration of orthographic r as
The /r/ at the end of four, father and over is not pronounced when the words are in
isolation but when they are followed by a vowel sound in a string of words of in
The Intrusive /r/: Possibly due to the fact that the use of the linking r is common among
(orthographic r) does not appear in spelling. This type of /r/ is known as the intrusive r.
For example, America has no /r/ at the end, but if America is placed before a word
beginning with a vowel, such as in America and Canada, an intrusive/r/ emerges to give
terminating in /ə/. Instances with other vowels hardly ever occur. For example, /ɛ:/ is
invariably spelt with r and final /ɜ:/ almost always has r in the spelling.
Also, related to liaison is epenthesis, which is the insertion of a segment which was
previously absent. In all varieties of English, speakers often insert a homorganic stop
after a nasal in examples such as the following: length /leŋkθ/. As a result, words such as