Tan - 2011 - Towards A Culturally Sensitive and Deeper Understanding of "Rote Learning" and Memorisation of Adult
Tan - 2011 - Towards A Culturally Sensitive and Deeper Understanding of "Rote Learning" and Memorisation of Adult
Po-Li,Tan1
Abstract
This article aims to provide evidence that “rote learning” or “memorisation” is a
complex construct and is deeply embedded in the East Asian culture. An in-depth
understanding of this learning approach is increasingly crucial considering the complex
demography of contemporary higher education nowadays. Not only is there a rise
in the number of matured adult learners but also there is an increase in the number
of international students (in particular students from the East). Despite the widening
participation, there is an appalling lack of awareness of cultural differences in learning
and teaching in Western higher education. International students from the Eastern
cultures are often frowned at as ineffective rote learners. The current study, which
adopted a culturally sensitive framework, provides verification that when adopting
a memorisation approach, learners from the Eastern cultures could learn beyond
“rote.” About 1,000 Malaysian Malay and Chinese adult learners who were engaging
with professional development were involved in the current study. The data in the
current study provides insightful evidence that when memorisation is a culturally
ingrained approach, it can lead to deep understanding, even for nontraditional
students like the adult learners.
Keywords
deep culture, memorisation, rote learning, middle-way philosophy, Confucianism, adult
learners, professional development, East Asian learners, Malaysia
First, this article describes the recent demographic scenario of higher education (HE)
and argues that the existing learning theory for HE is limiting in understanding the
1
King’s Learning Institute, King’s College London, 5.19, Franklin-Wilkins Building, Waterloo Bridge Wing,
London SE1 8WA
Tan 125
current changes. It then argues that a deeper understanding on one of the learning
constructs—“memorisation” from a culturally sensitive perspective could help
bridge the “gap” in the current theory. The article then provides concrete evidence
and discusses the findings in relationships to deep cultural East Asian values and
philosophy.
changes in HE raised the critical issue of whether current theory on teaching and
learning models adopted by the U.K. HE is adequate for improving the quality of
teaching and learning in the United Kingdom.
Table 1. Scale, Example of Item, Number of Items, Cronbach’s Alpha, SRMS, CFI
Scales No. of items Cronbach’s alpha
R-SPQ-2FM
DA 12 .84
Example I find that at times studying gives me a feeling of deep personal satisfaction (Item 4)
I find most new topics interesting and often spend extra time trying to obtain
more information about them (Item 8)
CM/AM 5 .80
Example I am at the university because I feel that I will be able to obtain a better job if I
have a higher academic qualification (Item 1)
I see further education is for me and my family, a good way to get a better job
(Item 5)
SA 8 .75
Example I do not find my study very interesting so I keep my effort to the minimum (Item 10)
I see no point in learning material, which is not likely to be in the examination
(Item 22)
U&M 4 .74
Example I repeat many times so that I can understand (Item 30)
Repetition helps memorising by creating a deep impression (Item 23)
I learn something by rote, going over and over them until I know them by heart
even if I do not understand them (Item 9)
Repetition plus “attentive effort” can lead to new meaning (Item 29)
current context when there is a widening demography in HE, where the majority of the
students come from the diverse “minority” cultures. These “minority cultures” (interna-
tional students or adult learners in the current context) are being measured up with the
“elite” culture (in the U.K. context, this means the Western Aristotelian—inquiring
and critiquing learning culture) expected by the HE. The “elite” goals and values of
academics are for students in HE to engage with DA to learning—displaying deep
motives and strategies. However, it is argued that the majority of students continue to
engage with surface learning and that changing approaches is complicated and devel-
opmental. Research currently has constructed approaches to learning model which is
based on a set of elite values, attitudes, and epistemologies that make more sense to
HE’s “gatekeepers” than they do to many of its students. Learners who do not
“comply” with the deep-learning idealism are diagnosed as “problematic” and need
to change, to improve to meet the grand HE agenda. Haggis (2003), for example, has
argued that deep learning is a set of highly complex cognitive operations and takes
many years to learn. The small number of deep learners who succeed in performing
according to the “elitist” HE agenda either have acquired the skills and ability to per-
form in the way that is desired, or at least are compliant in shaping towards this
direction. The majority of students, in particular nontraditional adult learners and
students who come from minority cultural backgrounds, are often sidelined and their
voices and “learning approaches” are not considered in this learning and teaching
model. Very often, the richness of their previous experiences and cultural learning
approaches are downplayed or even disregarded. The approaches to learning model
has also not been explicit in describing the intensity required and that “academically
unsuccessful students do not just exhibit ‘poorer’ approaches to studying but fail to
exhibit any coherent approaches at all . . . poor academic performance appear to be
associated with a disintegration or fragmentation of the normal patterns of studying”
(p. 99).
In a similar vein, Case (2007) suggested the theme of alienation and engagement
as an alternative perspective for characterising the student experience of learning in
the HE. Congruent with Haggis (2003) who argues that students’ diverse voices are
not considered in the existing model of learning, Case suggests that the preexisting
discourse of HE (in particular Western universities who have been receiving massive
number of international and adult students) places many students in “fixed” ways,
and these ways constraint their dominant learning behaviour they have internalised
from their previous experiences and learning culture. The above set ways have further
subjugated and disempowered these students when they are placed in subservient
power relationships to lecturers, and there is barely any freedom for negotiation or
empowerment. Case (2007) has characterised learning in the HE as the acquisition of
specialist knowledge and argued that the discourse that university learning require
students to acquire will often be in conflict with more experiential discourses that
students have acquired in their previous work or social cultural community. Very
often, gaining of the specialist knowledge involves some personal loss, leading to the
consequence of these students not only losing some of the implicit experiential
knowledge but also not fully engaged with the new HE learning environment. Echoing
Tan 129
the disempowerment and losing identity of students is the work by Leach, Neutze,
and Zepke (2001), who argued that the current dogmatic traditional assessment prac-
tices has disempowered learners in the HE as a result of subtle ways of exerting
power over students on what knowledge is essential to be assessed or how it should
be assessed. “To impose a unitary view of near-objectivity on the assessment process,
is to required the learner to conform to the reality of the assessor” (p. 296). Thus, if
assessment in HE values specialist discourse and set the students against the expe-
riential or personal discourse, they argue that the students may eventually choose
alienation as a strategy for self-preservation and may simply not engage at all (Case,
2007). One of the evidence of students not engaging is student dropout rate. The
recent statistics released by HESA, on “percentage of mature entrants to full-time first
degree courses in 2005/06 who are no longer in HE in 2006/07” revealed that 20.2%
of mature students without previous qualification failed to complete the degree
course (HESA, 2007).
This article argues that one learning approach which is culturally embedded but con-
tradicting to the current “elitist” deep meaningful critiquing approach is “memorisation.”
(see Bempechat & Drago-Severson, 1999; Biggs, 1990; Kember, 1991; I. Mok et al.,
2001; M. C. Mok et al., 2008; Niles, 1996; Watkins & Ismail, 1994; Watkins & Reghi,
1991). These studies argued that Asian learners may not be rote learners. Rather,
some of these studies even established that Asian learners use deep and achieving
approaches more than their Western counterparts. For instance, Volet et al. (1994)
found East Asian learners keener to adopt achievement strategies than Australian
learners and performed better in academic examinations. Even though the data from
these SPQ-based studies have initiated doubts about the learning processes and ability
of Asian learners (Kember et al., 1999), they did not provide explanations to the puz-
zling issue of how these Asian learners can rote learn and yet use deep and achieving
approaches concurrently.
likely to adopt similar learning approach to enhance deep understanding despite their
age. In addition, much of our understanding of adult learners indicates that they are
mostly motivated by internal factors and hence are more likely to adopt DA (see
Biggs, 1987; Richardson, 1995; Zeegers, 2001, 2002). If deep understanding can
be achieved by repetition and memorisation in some cultures, then adult learners
from these cultures may prefer to adopt culturally embedded approach despite being
slowed down by deteriorating age and memory ability as suggested by literature. If
that is the case, it implies that cultural factors may be a fundamental influential mod-
erating variable than age factors in influencing a learner to adopt a “repetition and
memorisation” strategy.
In short, globalisation has driven changes at HE. The complexities in HE has led to
the current debate on learning theory underpinning the teaching and learning in HE and
its limitation in conceptualising and addressing the changes in HE. This article provides
evidence that “memorisation” learning approach which has often been downplayed by
the Western HE could “bridge” some of the gaps of the current learning debate.
and content validity. Second, new “emic” items, which form the CM (this scale was
included based on theoretical underpinnings of adult learning theory and Asian learners)
and U&M subscales, were added in the instrument development process guided by the
literature (see Dahlin & Watkins, 2000; Kember et al., 1999; Watkins & Biggs, 1996)
and adult learning contexts in Malaysia. The 43 items were initially grouped into
7 subscales—Deep Strategy, Deep Motive, Understanding & Memorising, Achieve
Motives, Career Motives, Surface Strategy, and Surface Motive (see Kember et al.,
1999 and Table 1).
R-SPQ-2FM (the English version) was not translated as they are direct, behavioural
information questions (Behling & Law, 2000). Most of the graduate programmes in
Malaysia use English as a medium of instruction, and hence the adult participants in
this research are proficient in the English language. However, many words and idio-
matic expressions were changed and adapted to local colloquialism, with the aim of
increasing familiarity (Hinkin, 1998).
Sample
Stage 1—The Pilot Study
One hundred and one adult learners from the Klang Valley in Malaysia participated
in the preliminary Stage 1 study. These were adult learners who were engaging with
professional development programmes either full-time or part-time in formal settings
(i.e., university) and hence are representative of the population of interest. The sample
comprised 52.5% Malays, 47.5% Chinese, and 73% females. Participants ranged in
age from 21 to 51 and above and majority fell into the age range of 21 to 40. Sixty-
one percent participated in full time study, and 72% were studying in a master’s
programme.
Results
To address Research Question 1, EFA followed by CFA were carried out to see if
U&M scale was rigorous and valid. EFA (performed with Varimax rotation and factor
loading of >0.55) shows that the factors extracted which concerned the purpose of this
study are valid and reliable. Table 1 shows that DA has 12 items, CM/AM has 5 items,
SA has 8 items, and U&M has 4 items. The reliabilities found are a = .84, a = .8, a = .75
and a = .74, respectively.
DA in the current study combines items of Deep Motive and Deep Strategy scales of
SPQ and R-SPQ-2F, congruent with findings on other Malaysian secondary students
(Watkins & Ismail, 1994). More important, the U&M scale which was significantly
extracted in this study supported Kember et al.’s (1999) proposal to include cultural
sensitive scale such as memorisation, in particular when East Asian learners with
Confucian background are involved.
DA e1
.73(.92)
ME
.49 (.54)
.50 (.47)
RE .30 (.16)
U&M e3
.49 (.61)
SA e4
higher mean for this item, Malay, M = 3.13, SD = 1.01; Chinese, M = 2.61, SD = 1.16;
t(858) = 6.97, p < .001. This reaffirms the relational pattern CM/AM→U&M→SA,
suggesting that Malay and Chinese adult learners may engage with memorisation
approaches differently. In this case, the memorisation approach adopted by the Malay
adult learners may be less helpful in enhancing deep learning as advocated by Biggs
(2003), whereas the memorisation approach adopted by the Chinese adult learners is
more likely to be a DA.
Even as Pearson product–moment correlation analysis is valuable in describing the
strength and direction of the linear relationship between two variables (Pallant, 2001),
it is limiting in establishing the strength and causal relationships of a series of multiple
relationships simultaneously. However, having established that (a) there are correla-
tions between U&M with both DA and SA and (b) there are significant differences in
the correlation coefficients between the Malay and Chinese cultural groups, SEM car-
ried out based on theoretical justification may be a more rigorous and holistic tool to
address the complicated causal relationships (Hair, Anderson, Tatham, & Black, 1995).
Please see Figure 1 for the following discussion.
One of the most significant observation of the model in Figure 1 is the paths,
RE→U&M and ME→DA. For the Malay cultural group, the RE latent variable has a
stronger causal effect on U&M, with a higher β value (RE→U&M, Malay b = .30,
p < .01; Chinese, b = .16, p<.01). However, the Chinese adult learners who adopt ME
display a stronger positive causal effect on DA (ME→DA, Chinese b = .92, p < .01;
Malay, b =.73, p < .01). The path reiterates the notion that Malay learners who are
reproductive orientated are more likely to adopt memorisation approach. It is plausible
that the Malay adult learners, when adopting memorising approach, is more likely to
be SAs to learning, that is, rote memorisation with no understanding.
MANOVA analysis was carried out to explore whether there were statistically
significant differences between the Malay and Chinese cultural groups (independent
variable) caused by the seven moderating variables (e.g., gender, age, work experi-
ence, and level of courses studied) and the effect size of such moderating effect on
the four factors (dependent variables): DA, SA, CM/AM, and U&M approach in a
combined score. However in this article, only the effects of “level of courses studied”
(ranging from PhD, master’s, to short/long continuous professional development/
studies such as ACCA, diploma in education) were reported as they are pertinent
analysis related to U&M (see Table 4).
The “level of courses studied” moderating variable shows some interesting results
when considering U&M approaches to learning. For Malay adult learners, there is a
significant difference on the effect of this moderating variable on the U&M approach,
but such a result is not evident for the Chinese adult learners, Malay: F(3, 532) =
12.49, p < .001, h2 = .06; Chinese: F(3, 326) = 3.37, p > .05, h2 = .03. The Malay
adult learners who undertook a PhD adopted the least U&M approach (M = 2.95)
while those who engaged with short continuous professional development adopted
the most U&M approach (M = 3.75). It is interesting to note that Chinese adult learn-
ers across all courses adopted U&M approach but with no significant differences in
Tan 137
Level of courses
Malay .23 12 10.75 .076 <.001a
Chinese .19 12 5.24 .88 <.001a
Note: MANOVA = multivariate analysis of variance; DA = Deep Approach; SA = Surface Approach;
CM/AM = Career/Achieve Motives; U&M = Understand & Memorising.
a. Significant results.
the mean scores regardless of the level of courses they undertook, unlike their Malay
counterparts.
Discussion
Current debates on the changes in HE has undoubtedly questioned the appropriateness
of the learning theories developed from the Western perspectives. Though “memorisa-
tion as a learning approach” has been put in contempt by many Western educationists,
this study reveals that indigenous conceptualisation of the construct “memorisation”
from the perspective of the East Asian adult learners differs from the perspective of the
Westerners. Such evidence adds credence to the debates by authors like Watkins and
Biggs (1996; I. Mok et al., 2001) on Chinese learners—who have persistently argued
that Chinese learners have been “misconstrued” when perceived from the Western
educational theory and philosophy.
First, the narrow conceptualisation of memorisation, that is, rote learning which
leads to nonlearning, does not help to fully appreciate the current learning complexities
of HE. Memorisation perceived from the East Asian culture is more than just rote learn-
ing. Memorisation can transcend to the level of understanding and meaningful learning,
even in the context of adult learners. The cognitive processes of memorising seem more
intricate than what has been understood. This approach is one of the key approaches
favoured by Malaysian adult learners, in particularly the Chinese learners, regardless of
the level of study they engaged with. It may be plausible to suggest that despite their
age, these Chinese adult learners generally may be more likely to adopt a culturally
innate approach to enhance learning. In this respects, the Chinese-educated Malaysian
Chinese would have a stronger inclination to memorise to understand; as one of the
effective means of learning the Chinese language characters is to practice repeatedly
and memorise the Chinese characters and the four-character Chinese idioms.
The finding in this study that memorisation, which can be a deep-learning approach,
concurs with the argument by Tang (1993) on deep memorisation adopted by her
Hong Kong Chinese students. She argues that deep memorisation is a concept which
138 Journal of Studies in International Education 15(2)
occurs intuitively and naturally by many Chinese students but not for Western learners.
Dahlin and Watkins (2000) also pointed out that whereas their Hong Kong secondary
students appreciate a memorisation approach to learning, their German counterpart
tended to downplay “memorisation approach” and perceived this learning approach
with a negative light. Similarly, Kember (1996) argued that there are four levels of
memorisation adopted by Hong Kong Chinese students.
The question raised is “how could one memorise and understand simultaneously?”
Perhaps an appreciation of deep cultural values could provide powerful insights to
understand memorisation as an effective learning approach.
Conclusion
This article has considered the changes in HE and argued that the limitation of current
learning theory may benefit from understanding learning from a culturally sensitive
perspective. The current study presents evidence that memorisation, which has been
misconstrued from the Western perspective, is more than rote-learning.
As culture is one of the most complex concepts to study, unpacking the deeper
cultural values, such the cultural philosophy and language, can enlighten some of the
puzzling issues in memorisation as a learning approach. Empowering educators in
HE with deep cultural knowledge on educational concepts and approaches would
certainly benefit in addressing the complexity of pedagogy as a result of changing
demography at HE, in particular at the Western universities.
Although this study acknowledges its limitation—(a) with the focus on Malaysian’s
perspective and (b) using solely quantitative method—it is argued that its focus on
Malaysian Malays and Chinese adult learners has ironically become its strength. This
is because the literature on memorisation has targeted on mostly Hong Kong Chinese
traditional school leavers and university students. Hence, the specific target on Malaysian
adult learners can hopefully expand the scope of the current literature.
Funding
The author declared no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article.
Notes
1. These are adult students who left university for a while and have enrolled in the university
but have work experience. Hence they are generally more matured than, for example the
traditional A-levels applicants.
2. For full validation of the instrument, please see Tan (2006).
References
Alexander, I. D. (2007). Multicultural teaching and learning resources for preparing future
faculty in teaching in higher education. New Directions for Teaching and Learning,
111(Fall), 27-33.
Barbarous scratchings or universal system? The Chinese puzzle on paper. (2008, February 7).
Times Higher Education. Retrieved February 19, 2009, from http://www.timeshighereduca-
tion.co.uk/story.asp?sectioncode=26&storycode=400474
Barnett, R. (2000). Supercomplexities and the curriculum. Studies in Education, 25, 255-265.
Behling, O., & Law, K. S. (2000). Translating questionnaires and other research instruments:
Problems and solutions (Sage University Papers Series on Qualitative Applications in Social
Sciences, 07-131). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bempechat, J., & Drago-Severson, E. (1999). Cross-cultural differences in academic achievement:
Beyond etic conceptions of children’s understanding. Review of Educational Research,
69, 287-314.
Biggs, J. (1987). Student approaches to learning and studying. Melbourne: Australian Council
for Educational Research.
Biggs, J. (1990, July). Asian students’ approaches to learning: Implications for teaching overseas
students. Paper presented at the Eighth Australasian Learning and Language Conference,
Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane, Australia.
Biggs, J. (1996). Western misperceptions of the Confucian heritage learning culture. In D. Watkins
& J. Biggs (Eds.), The Chinese learners: Cultural and psychological and contextual influence
(pp. 45-67). Melbourne, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research.
Biggs, J. (2003). Teaching for quality at university (2nd ed.). Berkshire, UK: Open University Press.
Biggs, J., Kember, D., & Leung, D. Y. P. (2001). The revised two-factor Study Process
Questionnaire: R-SPQ-2F. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 133-149.
Biggs, J., & Watkins, D. (1996). The Chinese learner in retrospect. In D. Watkins & J. Biggs
(Eds.), The Chinese learner: Cultural, psychological and contextual influences. (pp. 260-285).
Melbourne, Australia: Australian Council for Educational Research.
Case, J. M. (2007). Alienation and engagement: Development of an alternative theoretical
framework for understanding student learning. Higher Education, 55, 321-332.
Chen, M. J. (2002). Transcending paradox: The Chinese “middle way” perspective. Asia Pacific
Journal of Management, 19, 179-199.
142 Journal of Studies in International Education 15(2)
Cohen, F. G. (1996, August 15). Confucianism: An excerpt from a synopsis of the ethics of
Confucianism. Retrieved September 18, 2002, from http://www.thespiritualsanctuary.org/
Confucianism/Confucianism.html
Dahlin, B., & Watkins, D. (2000). The role of repetition in the processes of memorising and
understanding: A comparison of the views of German and Chinese secondary school students
in Hong Kong. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 65-84.
Don: Confucianism still relevant. (2004, November 4). The Star, p. 12.
Durkin, K. (2007). The middle way: East Asian Master’s students’ perceptions of critical
argumentation in U.K. universities. Journal of Studies in International Education, 12(1), 38-55.
Echter, T., Kim, U., James Kau, C.-V., Li, H.-C., Simmons, C., & Ward, C. (1998). A comparative
study in the levels of human values: People’s Republic of China, Singapore, Taiwan, and the
United States. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 1, 271-288.
Entwistle, N., & Entwistle, D. (2003). Preparing for examination: The interplay of memorising
and understanding, and the development of knowledge object. Higher Education Research
and Development, 22(1), 19-41.
Entwistle, N., & Ramsden, P. (1983). Understanding student learning. London: Croom Helm.
Floyd, F. J., & Widaman, K. F. (1995). Factor analysis in the development and refinement of
clinical assessment instruments. Psychological Assessment, 7, 286-299.
Haggis, T. (2003). Constructing images of ourselves? A critical investigation into “Approaches
to learning” research in higher education. British Educational Research Journal,
29(1), 89-104.
Hair, J. F. J., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1995). Multivariate data analysis
(4th ed.). New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
Helmke, A., & Tuyet, V. T. A. (1999). Do Asian and Western students learn in different way? An
empirical study on motivation, study time, and learning strategies of German and Vietnamese
university students. Asia Pacific Journal of Education, 19(2), 30-44.
Higher Education Statistics Agency. (2007). Percentage of mature entrants to full-time first
degree courses in 2005/06 who are no longer in HE in 2007/08. Retrieved December 7, 2009,
from http://www.hesa.ac.uk/dox/performanceIndicators/0607/sn2_0607.xls
Hinkin, T. R. (1998). A brief tutorial on the development of measures for use in survey
questionnaires. Organizational Research Methods, 1(1), 104-121.
Hofstede, G. (1991). Management in a multicultural society. Malaysian Management Review,
26(1), 3-12.
Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indexes in covariances structure modeling: Sensitivity to
underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Method, 3, 424-453.
Jiang, X. (2002). Zhang Dongsun: Pluralist epistemology and Chinese philosophy. In C.-Y.
Cheng & N. Bunnin (Eds.), Contemporary Chinese Philosophy (pp. 57-81). Oxford, UK:
Blackwell.
Kember, D. (1991). A challenge to the anecdotal stereotype of the Asian student. Studies in
Higher Education, 16(2), 117-127.
Kember, D., Biggs, J., & Leung, Y. P. (2004). Examining the multidimensionality of approaches to
learning through the development of a revised version of the Learning Process Questionnaire.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 261-279.
Tan 143
Kember, D. (1996). The intention to both memorise and understand: Another approach to learning?
Higher Education, 31, 341-354.
Kember, D., Wong, A., & Leung, D. Y. P. (1999). Reconsidering the dimensions of approaches
to learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 323-343.
Kingston, E., & Forland, H. (2004, September 20-21). Bridging the gap in expectations
between international students and academic staff—“At home the teachers feed me with
knowledge, but in the UK they help me pick up the spoon and learn to feed myself!” Paper
presented at the European Conference on Educational Research Post Graduate and New
Researcher Pre-Conference, University of Crete. Retrieved December 7, 2009, from http://
www.leeds.ac.uk/educol/documents/00003751.htm
Knowles, M. (1990). The adult learner: Neglected species. Houston, TX: Gulf Publishing.
Leach, L., Neutze, G., & Zepke, N. (2001). Assessment and empowerment: Some critical
questions. Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 26, 293-305.
Leung, Y. P. D., Ginns, P., & Kember, D. (2008). Examining the cultural specificity of approaches
to learning in universities in Hong Kong and Sydney. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
39, 251-266.
Marton, F., Dall’Alba, G., & Kun, T. L. (1996). Memorising and understanding: The key to
the paradox. In D. Watkins & J. Biggs (Eds.), The Chinese learners: Cultural, psychologi-
cal and contextual influences (pp. 69-83). Melbourne: Australian Council for Educational
Research.
Marton, F., & Saljo, R. (1976). On qualitative differences in learning—I: Outcome and process.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, 4-11.
Marton, F., Watkins, D., & Tang, C. (1997). Discontinuities and continuities in the experience of
learning: An interview study of high-school students in Hong Kong. Learning and Instruction,
7(1), 21-48.
Merriam, S. B., & Caffarella, R. S. (1999). Learning in adulthood: A comprehensive guide
(2nd ed.). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Mok, I., Chik, P. M., Ko, P. Y., Kwan, T., Lo, M. L., Marton, F., et al. (2001). Solving the paradox
of the Chinese teacher? In D. Watkins & J. Biggs (Eds.), Teaching the Chinese learners: Psy-
chological and pedagogical perspectives (pp. 161-181). Melbourne: Australian Council for
Educational Research.
Mok, M. C., Kennedy, K. J., Moore, P. J., Shan, P. W. J., & Leung, S. O. (2008). The use of
helping-seeking by Chinese secondary school students: Challenging the myth of “the Chinese
learner.” Evaluation and Research in Education, 21, 188-213.
Niles, S. (1996). Cultural variation in learning strategies: A comparison of Australian and
Sri Lankan university students. Journal of Psychology, 130(1), 83-94.
Pallant, J. (2001). SPSS survival manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS for
Windows (Version 10). Sydney, Australia: Allen & Unwin.
The Prime Minister’s Initiatives. (n.d.). Retrieved February 23, 2009, from http://www.british-
council.org/eumd-pmi2-about.htm
Purdie, N., & Hattie, J. (2002). Assessing students’ conceptions of learning. Australian Journal
of Developmental and Educational Psychology, 2, 17-32.
Ramsden, P. (2003). Learning to teach in higher education (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.
144 Journal of Studies in International Education 15(2)
Renshaw, P. D., & Volet, S. E. (1995). South-east Asian students at Australian universities:
Reappraisal of their tutorial participation and approaches to study. Australia Educational
Researcher, 22(2), 85-106.
Richardson, J. T. E. (1995). Mature students in higher education: An investigation of approaches
to studying and academic performance. Studies in Higher Education, 20(1), 5-14.
Russell, M. (2008, March 20-26). The age of uncertainty. The Times Higher Education, p. 24.
Shaules, J. (2007). Deep culture: The hidden challenges of global living. Toronto, Ontario,
Canada: Multilingual Matters.
Tan, P.-L. (2006). Approaches to learning and learning values: An investigation of adult learners
in Malaysia. Unpublished doctoral thesis, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane,
Australia.
Tan, P.-L., Pillay, H., & Fiona, S. (2004). Adult learners in Malaysia: Some Issues to consider.
In E. Mcwilliam, S. Danby, & K. Knight (Eds.), Performing educational research (pp. 303-317).
Brisbane: Queensland, Post Pressed National Library of Australia.
Tang, K. C. C. (1993). Spontaneous collaborative learning: A new dimension in student learning
experience? Higher Education Research and Development, 12(2), 115-130.
Tse, S. K., Marton, F., Ki, W. W., & Loh, K. Y. E. (2007). An integrative perceptual approach for
teaching Chinese characters. Instructional Science, 35, 375-406.
United Kingdom Council for International Student Affair. (2006/7). Higher education
statistics. Retrieved February 24, 2009, from http://www.ukcosa.org.uk/about/statistics_
he.php#table3
Vijver, V. D., & Leung, K. (2000). Methodological issues in psychological research on culture.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 31(1), 33-51.
Volet, S. E., Renshaw, P. D., & Tietzel, K. (1994). A short-term longitudinal investigation of cross-
cultural differences in study approaches using Biggs’ SPQ questionnaire. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 64, 301-318.
Watkins, D. (2001). Correlates of approaches to learning: A cross-cultural meta-analysis.
In R. J. Sternberg & L. F. Zhang (Eds.), Perspectives on thinking, learning, and cognitive
styles (pp. 165-195). London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Watkins, D., & Biggs, J. (Eds.). (1996). The Chinese learners: Cultural, psychological and
contextual influences. Melbourne: Australian Council for Educational Research.
Watkins, D., & Ismail, M. (1994). Brief research report: Is the Asian learner a rote learner?
A Malaysian perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 19, 483-488.
Watkins, D., & Reghi, M. (1991). The Asian-learner-as a rote-learner stereotype: Myth or reality?
Educational Psychology, 11(1), 21-35.
Zeegers, P. (2001). Approaches to learning in science: A longitudinal study. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 71, 115-132.
Zeegers, P. (2002). A revision of the Biggs’ Study Process Questionnaire (R-SPQ). Higher
Education Research & Development, 21(1), 73-92.
Zhang, L.-F. (2000). University students’ learning approaches in three cultures: An investigation
of Bigg’s 3P Model. Journal of Psychology, 134, 35-55.
Zhang, L.-F., & Sternberg, R. J. (2000). Are learning approaches and thinking styles related?
A study in two Chinese populations. Journal of Psychology, 134, 469-489.
Tan 145
Bio
Po-Li, Tan is a lecturer in higher education in King’s Learning Institute, King’s College
London. Her current research interests are intercultural awareness, intercultural pedagogy,
cross-cultural coaching, adult learning, cross-cultural methodology, and internationalisation of
higher education. She teaches sessions such as “Intercultural Pedagogy” and “Intercultural PhD
Supervision” for lecturers and “Intercultural Awareness Development” for students.