Language Differences
Language Differences
net/publication/309290816
CITATIONS READS
4 7,970
2 authors, including:
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Abderrazak Mohammed Chouchane on 20 October 2016.
Abstract
The focus of this research is in the area of Language and gender. It investigates the major
linguistic differences between men and women speech by examining the validity of the
conversational differences claimed by the Deficit and Dominance Theory. The research delves
into the major linguistic features that characterize women’s speech by analyzing a mixed
gender conversation. The findings from the conversation analysis provide evidence of
significant linguistic differences between female and male speech in using lexical hedges and
fillers, intensifiers and the lexical choice and intonation which supports the Deficit and
Dominance claims. However, in other features like the rising pitch and overlapping and
interruptions in turn taking, the conversation analysis does not clearly show a bias to neither
side. Although many of the linguistic features between both genders still exist, some other
features are starting to weaken as they do not show clear bias, which will keep the debate open
to further studies and theories.
1. Introduction:
Gender language differences are one of the controversial issues in linguistics which generated a
substantial debate among linguists and resulted in a massive literature about language usage
between men and women. It would make sense looking at differences between men’s and
women’s language competences from an angle of superiority and inferiority in societies where
women are really suppressed and subordinated, which is thought to be something of the past,
at least for Western societies since a few decades back when women were supposed to stay at
home, take care of family domestic life, listen and obey commands from a man dominated
society. To still interpret language differences in terms of women low and subordinate social
status in the 21st century with the influential women’s rights and liberation movements and the
unlimited communal and equivalent social roles women are playing side by side with men, casts
qualms and spurs desire to investigate the validity of the claimed conversational differences
between both genders.
The study is based on an analysis of a 25 minute casual mixed-sex conversation between three
men and three women English native speakers of equal social status and delves into the major
features that characterize women’s speech suggested by Robin Lakoff and the Deficit,
Dominance Theory.
These features are interpreted and categorized by many linguists from several angles. Holms
(2001) divides them into two categories, “hedging devices” and “boosting devices” were they
linguistic or stylistic features. Hedging devices are features which may be used for hedging or
weakening the “force of an utterance”, like tag questions, lexical hedges and fillers, and rising
intonation on declaratives. Whereas, boosting devices include features that may boost or
intensify a statement like intensifiers and emphatic stress. Lakoff, (1975), states that, in
occasions where hedges reveal the woman’s unassertiveness to her statement, intensifiers add
force to it. She considers that women’s use of hedges and intensifiers in mixed-gender
conversation reflects her lack of power in comparison with men. O’ Barr and Atkins (1980) in
their research on women’s language in courtroom discourse pointed out that women use these
speech styles more than men because it is associated with their lower-status positions. Hence,
it is understandable why many linguists refer to the Deficit and the Dominance models
inseparably and in many occasions as one approach.
2.4. The Dominance Model
Building on the previous claims of the Deficit model explaining reasons why women’s language
is described as deficient, the Dominance model presents the woman’s speech from a negative
traditional attitude evolved from the social, cultural and political subordination of women with
regard to men. Investigations of this theory focused on mixed-sex conversations to explain
men’s tendency to dominate and how women are subordinated. Zimmerman and West (1975)
in their research about speech styles in mixed-sex conversations, found that 95% of
interruptions were made by men, and that men interrupted women more than women did to
men. Freeman and McElhinny (1996) attribute the linguistic conversational inadequacies of
women to the social inequalities between both genders, and similarly men’s conversational
dominance reflects males’ social and political dominance over females. Lakoff (1975) adds that
women’s different conversational performance reflects their subordinate social status, and the
gender-differentiated language is a result of the men-oriented society where men have more
power and influence and where women tend to be careful, more considerate and polite when
they speak.
2.5. The Difference Model
The assumption of relating gender talk to power and social status is challenged by the emerging
dual-culture which attribute sex differences to “…contrasting orientations towards relations”,
(Montgomery, 1995). This model could probably have evolved as a reaction to Lackoff’s
Dominance model. Deborah Tannen (9990) the most prolific linguist of the Difference model
attributes differences between men and women in conversational behavior not to their social
status based on weakness and subordination, but rather they have to be interpreted in the
same way as cultural differences because men and women grow up in two different
subcultures, brought differently and they both fill different roles, (Wardhagh, 2002). In her
book You Just Don’t Understand, Tannen (1990) argues that men dominate conversation over
women does not really mean they intentionally intend to, but this is due to the fact that both of
them behave in accordance with their sex class traits. From their childhood, girls are trained,
encouraged and reinforced to play a feminine role, whereas boys are brought up to play the
masculine role. Thus differences in gender-differentiated forms of language use stem from their
early dual socialization. “Men and women are social beings who have learned to act in certain
ways. Language behavior is largely learned behavior. Men learn to be men and women learn to
be women, linguistically speaking”, (Wardhough 2002). Maltz and Broker (1982) look at gender
differences within “cross-ethnic miscommunication” and relate cross-sex conversation
differences to “cultural differences between men and women” in their way of perceiving,
engaging and interpreting friendly conversation,(cited in Bonvillain, 2008). In conversations,
women tend to ask questions, to encourage responses from their interlocutors, make minimal
responses and allow interruptions, while men usually tend to interrupt, challenge their
interlocutors’ speech and make direct assertion of opinion and control topics. Women tend to
“create and maintain relationships of closeness and equality” and men tend to “assert positions
and dominance, attract and maintain audience”, (Maltz and Broker, 1982). These dual
contrasting sex tendencies are due to what each sex has learned throughout childhood. Tannen
(1986) deepens the dichotomy more than Maltz and Broker by strongly claiming that women
and men grow up in “different worlds” in their childhood and in their adulthood they “travel in
different worlds”. Tannen (1990) interprets women’s and men’s conversations from a
perspective where man sees himself as “an individual in a hierarchical social order in which he
is either one-up or one-down. In this world, conversations are negotiations in which people try
to achieve and maintain the upper hand if they can, and protect themselves from others’
attempts to put them down and push them around.” The woman, on the other side, sees
herself as “…an individual in a network of connections. In this world, conversations are
negotiations for closeness in which people try to seek and give confirmation and support, and
to reach consensus. They try to protect themselves from others’ attempts to push them away.”
Tannen considers that contrary to men who see life as “a contest, a struggle to preserve
independence and avoid failure”, women, see it as “a community, a struggle to preserve
intimacy and avoid isolation. Though there are hierarchies…they are…more of friendship than
of power and accomplishment.” Tannen looks at sex conversations from a motivational
perspective according to the objectives and agendas each sex has behind conversation. She
conversely interprets the underestimations of the Dominance and Deficit theories of woman’s
speech from a positive perspective. Tannen (1990) makes distinction between men’s “report”
talk agenda and women’s “rapport-building” talk agenda. Her interpretations can be
summarized in six contrastive points: Status vs. support, independence vs. intimacy, advice vs.
understanding, information vs. feelings, orders vs. proposals, and conflict vs. compromise. To
justify her interpretations, Tannan suggests the following differences between women’s and
men’s conversational behavior:
Women: Men:
Talk too much Get more air time
Speak in private contexts Speak in public
Build relations Negotiate status
Overlap Speak one at a time
Speak symmetrically Speak asymmetrically
3. Conversation Analysis
This study is based on a case study of a casual mixed-sex conversation between English native
speakers. For the sake of balance, variety of topics and talk continuity, six persons are
approached to take part in the conversation, three males and three females. The participants
are all English teachers in the same university, and their relationship is merely a work
friendship. The conversation was video-taped and then transcribed as per Jefferson’s
transcription system, (1972).
While analyzing this conversation, the lexical fillers such as “well”, “like”, and “I mean” are easy
to distinguish. However, some others are subject to different interpretations. The vocal fillers
“mm” and “eh” and “yeah” have different functions. When they occur in the beginning of a
speech turn or individually in minimal responses, they usually mean a “yes” answer or they
show the listener’s agreement and follow of the speaker. When they occur in between the
same speech, they are usually considered as fillers. Hence, the count is done on this basis. In
the conversation, the number of lexical hedges and fillers used by women is nearly double that
used by men. Out of a total of 92 hedges and fillers, 57 are used by women. Females use much
more vocal pauses “eh” and “mm” than males. The most frequent lexical feature here “like” is
used 15 times by women and 8 times by men. Wendi alone used it 6 times in her only two
“extended” speech turns, which may indicate that the use of these words are a matter of
personal and individual conversational habits far from being generalized. The two sample
speeches of Wendi and Cathy below illustrate clearly the frequency of these devices.
Cathy: Some people eat in their kitchen, from here TV, bedroom, eating, like
that. So I need the bedroom to be a kind of, it’s like when I go to mm, you
know to Baskin-Robins…
Wendi: Because we I have a friend and her daughter, their daughter is like
twelve I guess, and she’s too embarrassed to go with her parents... It was
awful, we had to stand there for like two and a half hours waiting for him to
come on stage and it was only standing and it’s just like packed in with ten
year old sobbing girls …
Fishman (1998) argues that when the woman uses more hedges than men does not necessarily
show lack of confidence or insecurity, rather it is a conversational strategy to draw her
listener’s attention and engage him/her more in the conversation when her partner becomes
careless or loses concentration. Maltz and Broker (1982) claim that women use these fillers to
indicate they are listening while men use them to express agreement. This supports Tannen’s
claim that women are rapport-builders and during their conversation they tend to “preserve
intimacy and avoid isolation”, (Tannen, 1990).
Apart from the interpretation dichotomy that lies behind the dual perspective of the
Dominance theory and the Difference theory, it is evident from this conversation that women
use more hedges and fillers than men do.
3.1.3. Intensifiers
“Intensifiers are words like very, rather, absolutely, that modify adjectives, adverbs, and verbs
by heightening or lowering their intensity”, (Thornbury, 2006). Jespersen (1922) and Lackoff
(1975) point out that women tend to use intensifiers such as “so”, “very”, and “extremely”
more than men do to exaggerate and superficially reinforce their statement. The Dominance
theory attributes this to the negative view subordinating and lowering the woman’s social
status. So she includes intensifiers to support and empower her speech by using boosting
devices.
There is a number of 38 intensifiers used in this conversation, 25 of them are used by the
female participants, which proves the claim that intensifiers occur more in women’s speech
than men’s. The conversation analysis reveals that females use “so” and “very” more often than
other intensifiers, while men tend to use “really” more frequently than “so” or “very”, which is
a point raised by Ito and Tagliamonte (2003) when they studied the use of “very” and “really”
by different age groups in York. In the same sense of boosting devices, the quantifier “all” is
used 8 times by females and only once by males, which again shows that women tend to
intensify and exaggerate their utterances. This can be achieved by either using lexical features
or even other ways like metaphors or intonation. In the following example, Cathy, not only uses
“very” and “so” to boost her statement, but also adds a vocal booster “ooh” with emphatic
form.
Exaggeration and boosting reach a peak in the metaphor used by Cathy to describe Andrew’s
musical ability.
Cathy: Well, he knows more about music than I ever dreamed [to know
in my life].
Addressee-oriented tags are “affective tags” with double function either facilitative to engage
the addressee in the conversation or as an indication of politeness to mitigate and soften “the
force of a command of criticism”.
In the conversation, 5 question tags occurred; three are used by men and 2 by women. 2 of the
question tags used by men are speaker-oriented, in which the speakers just need a
confirmation of the statement to continue the talk, whereas the 2 used by women are
“facilitative” and have an “affective” function inviting interlocutor to contribute and take turn.
Sherlyn: Yeah, I have a drawer full of pink spoons. Thank you, Cathy.
Apparently the number does not support the dominance theory claim that women use question
tags more than men. However there are certain utterances which can be interpreted as having
the same function as question tags.
Wendi: Because we I have a friend and her daughter, their daughter is …….
3.3. Intonation
According to Lakoff, the fact that women tend to exaggerate and amplify utterances, was it by
means of lexical devices or speech intonation, reflects a lack of confidence and state of
devaluation due to her early socialization.
3.4.1. Overlapping
West and Zimmerman define overlapping speech as “simultaneous speech where a speaker
other than the current speaker begins to speak at or very close to a possible transition place in
a current speaker’s utterance (i.e., within the boundaries of the word)”, (cited in Bonvillain,
2008). Both Hutchby and Wooffitt (1998) and Bonvillain (2008) contrast overlapping and
interruptions. They describe overlapping as the failure of the next speaker to recognize the end
of the current speaker’s talk and that most overlapping speeches occur during talk turn
transitions, they are merely a wrong estimation of the start time of the next turn, whereas,
interruptions are violations of the current speaker’s rights. Fishman (1983) in his study of talk
between couples, found out that women exert more effort to gain the listener’s attention and
respond to his/her talk, and to show they are interested and listening, they tend to put their
minimal responses like “yeah”, “em”, “ok“, “that’s right” within the speaker’s turn as a strategy
of encouraging and maintaining conversation. This strategy occurred frequently in this
conversation. Among the 32 overlapping speeches 22 are women’s and 18 of these 22 are
speeches within the interlocutor’s speech. On the other hand, male speakers have 10
overlapping speeches only 6 of them are speeches at the end of the interlocutor’s talk and end
in turn taking. This proves that while men’s overlapping speech is a signal of taking
conversation turn, women’s simultaneous speeches appear mostly in the middle of the
speaker’s turn to show interest and support. Tannen interprets this in terms of the woman’s
agenda behind conversation, which is her tendency to maintain conversation and be intimate,
collaborative and a rapport-builder.
3.4.2. Interruptions
In their study about interruptions, West and Zimmerman (1983, 1975) noted that interruption
occurs when an interlocutor violates a speaker’s turn and thus the speaker cannot finish his
talk. It is when the co-participant intrudes into the current speaker’s talk. They found out that
men frequently interrupt women more than the other way round. Interruptions are interpreted
from different angles by different linguists. West and Zimmerman (1983) and Clark (1993) see
them as means for dominating and controlling interaction, whereas, Tannen (1993) interprets it
as a way of maintaining conversation and supporting speakers. Research shows that women in
conversation tend to be cooperative and try to maintain talk and build rapport whereas men
tend to be competitive and try to dominate the talk and prove themselves as superior, (Holmes,
2001). In mixed-sex conversation, women are usually more interrupted by men, and men do
not allow to be interrupted and often succeed either in usurping the conversation turn from
women or in keeping their turns in case of being interrupted, (Romaine 2000 and Bonvillain
2008). Holmes (2001) and Bonvillain (2008) ascribe this difference to the woman’s social status
being subordinated to men from their early childhood socialization.
There are 31 interruptions in the conversation; 16 by men and 15 by women. What is significant
here is that 14 out of the 16 of men’s interruptions were towards women while women
interrupted men only 9 times. The claim that men do not allow interruption is clearly illustrated
when Cathy tries to interrupt Andrew, but he “rebuttals” and does not give up the floor and
insists to continue his talk and consequently speaker switch fails and Cathy’s attempt of
interruption was “unsuccessful”.
Andrew: … that type of harmonica is diatonic which means you need the
twelve different key harmonicas, Ok, you can’t play them all on the one
instrument. So [so, if you got twelve major keys], that’s one harmonica, as far
as I am concerned.
Notice that the overlapping speech starts just after the phrase “so if” and Cathy poses the
question [“So, if you are going to do a sound]” thinking that they are going for a “common
goal”. Surprisingly that does not happen and Andrew ignores her and continues his talk. Cathy
surrenders and loses the “conversation struggle” and does not even bother to pose her
question when Andrew finishes his talk. This supports the claim that boys are trained to
dominate talk from their early childhood and girls in parallel are socialized to expect being
interrupted and lose the talk without any protest. Consequently, men usually end up
dominating the conversation in spite of all the woman’s attempts of maintaining conversation
and building equal opportunities of interaction, (Romaine, 2000).
On the other hand, Cathy’s intervention in the following example, which may be considered
neither an interruption nor an overlap, is an attempt to facilitate, support and encourage
Andrew to express his thought.
Andrew: That’s fun. There’s a lot of them. I don’t know, I won’t discuss the
opening scene because ((looking at the camera )) we’re being recorded but
mm but, but but there’s an opening scene on it which is is [which, which
would]=
Cathy: = [a lot of porn?]
Andrew: would send most Omanis into hospital with heart palpitations if
they saw it.
Andrew wants to speak about an open porn scene but he is hesitant least his listeners should
feel offended as some of them are not very close friends. Cathy, recognizing his hesitation by
making repeated gap fillers and vocal pauses (but em eh, but but) and (which is is which which),
intervenes to facilitates and maintain the conversation. The latching of their speech turn taking
shows Andrew released and assured to continue. Holmes (2001) and James and Clarke (1993)
point out that women use interruptions and overlaps not as “disruptive”, but for “cooperative
and rapport building” goals.
4. Conclusion
Overall, this case study of a mixed-sex conversation aimed at evaluating the different features
that characterize women’s speech as claimed by the Deficit and Dominance theory. Many of
the features, in particular the lexical ones, show compatible evidences to those claims, whereas
others do not clearly figure out a pure bias to either of the genders such as rising intonation on
declaratives and question tags. It is apparent that gender language differences has been one of
the controversial issues in language change and variation among other variables within the
social dimensions of change variation, like age, ethnicity, regional and social dialects; and as
Wardhaugh (2002) stated, it “generated considerable amount of thought and discussion … and
many are still unresolved”. The issue will still spawn debate and research as women’s “social
status” does not seem to be stagnant and is changing rapidly with regard to the call for equality
in society, politics, and work place. The woman does not seem to still have that subordinate
status she had half a century ago. In several societies, particularly Western countries, the
woman reached a high social status not that very underestimated than men, which might put in
question the whole Dominance theory and gives way to other approaches to prevail.
5. Table of figures:
8 18
fillers
hedges and
Lexical
eh & mm,
well 3 6
You know 5 6
I mean 3
I guess/ I think/ I 3 7
don’t know/ I hear/
they say 35 57
That thing/ 3 4
something etc…
so 1 6
intensifiers
very 1 5
too 3 2
really 6 2
used
Fantastic (1) Beautiful(2)
Awful (2)
Hysterical (2)
Fantastic
green / pink
Tag questions 3 2
Interruptions To women 14 To 6
women
15
To men 2 To men 9
16
Overlapping 10 22
Rising intonation on 4 6
declaratives
6. References
Bonvillain, N. (2008) Language Culture, and Communication, 5th ed. London: Pearson
Cameron, D. et al. (1989) Lackoff in Context: The Social and Linguistic Function of Tag
Questions In Coates, J. and Cameron. Women in their Speech Community London: Longman
Cameron, D. (1998), Performing Gender Identity: Young Men’s Talk and the Construction of
Heterosexual Masculinity. In Coates (1998), London: Blackwell
Coates, J. (1998) “Thank God I'm a Woman: The Construction of Differing Feminities”, In
Cameron, D. (ed), The Feminist Critique of Language. USA & Canada: Routledge.
Freeman, R. D. and McElhinny, B. (1996). Language and Gender, In Mckay, S. & Hornberger N.
(eds.), Sociolinguistics and Language Teaching, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
James, D. and Clarke S. (1993), Women, Men, and Interruptions: A critical Review. In Tannen, D.
Gender and Conversational Interaction. New York: Oxford University Press.
Kendall, S. and Tannen, D. (2001), Discourse and Gender. In Schiffrin, D, Tannen, D. and
Hamilton H.E (eds) The Handbook of Discourse Analysis. USA. UK. Australia: Blackwell
Lakoff, R. (1979). Woman’s Language. In Butturff, D. and Epstein, E. Women’s Language and
style, (eds). Akron: University of Akron.
Lakoff, R. (1975), Language and Women’s Place. New York: Harper & Row
O'Connor, James D. & G. K Arnold. (1973) Intonation of Colloquial English. London: Longman.
Tannen, D. (1990) You Just Don’t Understand Women and Men in Conversation. New York:
Harper
Tannen, D. (1993) Gender and Conversational Interaction (ed). New York and Oxford: Oxford
University Press
Thornbury, S. (2006) An A-Z of ELT A Dictionary of Terms and Concepts. Oxford: McMillan.
Zimmerman, D. H. and West, C. (1975). Sex Roles, Interruptions and Silences in Conversation. In
Torne and Henley (1975).