research
research
Hunston &
D. Oakey (Eds.), Introducing applied linguistics: Concepts and skills (pp. 74-83). London: Routledge.
CHAPTER/
CONTENTS Language teachers frequently use the term 'motivation' when they describe successful
The starting point: or unsuccessful learners. This reflects our intuitive belief that during the lengthy and
'Integrativeness'as a
motivational
often tedious process of mastering a foreign/second language (L2), the learner's
factor 74 enthusiasm, commitment and persistence are key determinants of success or failure.
Towards the'L2 Indeed, in the vast majority of cases, learners with sufficient motivation can achieve a
Motivational Self
System' 75
working knowledge of an L2, regardless o/their language aptitude, whereas without suf-
Validating the L2
ficient motivation even the brightest learners are unlikely to persist long enough to
Motivational Self attain any really useful language ('you can lead a horse to water, but you can't make it
System 80
drink').
Conclusion 82
Because of the central importance attached to it by practitioners and researchers
References 82
alike, L2 motivation has been the target of a great deal of research in Applied Linguistics
during the past decades. In this chapter I describe a major theoretical shift that has
recently been transforming the landscape of motivation research: the move from the
traditional conceptualization of motivation in terms of an integrative/instrumental
dichotomy to the recent conceptualization of motivation as being part of the learner's
self system, with the motivation to learn an L2 being closely associated with the learner's
'ideal L2 self. For space limitations I cannot provide a detailed review of the relevant
literature (for recent summaries, see Dornyei 2005; Dornyei and Ushioda 2009); instead,
my focus will be on illustrating how such a major paradigm shift has emerged through
a combination of theoretical considerations and empirical research findings.
The starting There has been a long-lived (and inaccurate) understanding in the L2 profession that
point: language learning motivation can be divided into two main dimensions: integrative
Mntegrativeness' motivation and instrumental motivation. The former refers to the desire to learn an L2
as a of a valued community so that one can communicate with members of the community
motivational and sometimes even to become like them. Instrumental motivation, on the other hand,
factor is related to the concrete benefits that language proficiency might bring about (e.g.
career opportunities, increased salary). Thus, broadly speaking, it was thought that we
learn a language either because we like it and its speakers or because we think it will be
useful for us. . : .
The integrative/instrumental distinction has been attributed (again somewhat inac-
curately) to the influential work of Canadian social psychologist Robert Gardner (1985,
2001), who did indeed introduce these terms but whose theoretical motivation
construct was much more elaborate than this simplistic duality. Furthermore, Gardner
hardly ever discussed the nature and impact of instrumental motivation, because he was
Researching motivation 75
Integrativeness reflects a genuine interest in learning the second language in order to come
closer to the other language community. At one level, this implies an openness to, and respect
: '- ' for other cultural groups and ways of life. In the extreme, this might involve complete iden-
-; tification with the community (and possibly even withdrawal from one's original group),
but more commonly it might well involve integration within both communities.
(Gardner 2001:5)
Towards the In 2005,1 proposed a new motivation construct (Dornyei 2005) - the 'L2 Motivational
*L2 Self System' - that builds upon the foundations laid by Gardner (1985) but which at the
Motivational same time broadens the scope of the theory to make it applicable in diverse language
Self System* learning environments in our globalized world. The proposed model, which attempts
to synthesize a number of influential new approaches in the field (e.g. Ushioda 2001;
, Noels 2003), has grown out of a combination of empirical research findings and the-
oretical considerations (for a detailed description, see Dornyei, 2009). Let us look at
these more closely, starting with the former.
1993 1999 1993 1999 1993 1999 1993 1999 1993 1999 1993 1999
Integrativeness .43* .33* .43* .33* .47* .43* .42* .44* .43* .43* .25* .32*
Instrumentality .28* .25* .28* .25* .30* .30* .27* .30* .29* .31* .20* .21*
Attitudes towards L2 ,23* .16* .17* .16* .33* .30* .31* .33* .32* .31* .12* .21*
speakers/community
Vitality of the .12* .09* .12* .09* .11* .12* .13* .16* .16* .18* .07* .10*
community
Cultural interest .14* .09* .12* .10* .20* .17* .20* .21* .26* .23* .12* .17*
Milieu .12* .12* .12* .12* .01 -.00 .03 .04 .01 -.00 -.05* -.10*
Linguistic .07* .06* .07* .06* -.00 .01 .03 - .02 -.01 -.02 -.02 -.04
self-confidence
Multiple
correlations .44* .34* .44* .34* .49* .45* .44* .46* .45* .45* .27* .34*
p < .001
Table 7. i Correlations between the attitudinal/motivational scales and Language choice in the Dornyei and
: Csizer (2002) study
Researching motivation 77
negative correlation an inverse relationship (Dornyei 2007). Thus, for example, learners'
IQ is expected to have a high positive correlation with their mathematics grades and
zero correlation with, say, the love of chocolate. Table 7.1 presents the results.
As can be seen in Table 7.1, three variables stand out consistently across the
languages and the data points: Integrativeness, Instrumentality and Attitudes towards L2
speakers/community. This was, actually, to be expected given our previous under-
standing of L2 attitudes and motivation, but what surprised us was that when we
computed multiple correlations (i.e. correlations between language choice and all the
motivational variables together), the joint correlation was hardly higher than the cor-
relation associated only with Integrativeness. For example, the correlation of the choice
of English (UK) in 1993 was .43 with Integrativeness and .44 with all the altitudinal
variables together. This suggested that Integrativeness played a principal role in
determining the extent of a learner's overall motivational disposition.
To test the prominent position of Integrativeness, Dornyei et al. (2006) submitted
the data from all the three waves of the survey to a more complex statistical procedure,
structural equation modelling (SEM). SEM is very useful to interpret the relationship
among several variables within a single framework. Its strength is that we can specify
directional paths (i.e. cause-effect relationships) amongst the variables and SEM then
produces various goodness-of-fit indices to evaluate the feasibility of the whole model.
In conducting the analysis, we took each language and each year separately (so we
computed separate models for, say, German in 1993 and French in 2004), but the
various models converged and with minor variations produced the same overall result.
Figure 7.1 presents the schematic representation of the final construct.
Figure 7.1
Schematic
representation
of the structural
equation model
in Dornyei et al.'s
(2006) study
Attitudes toward
L2 speakers
/8 Zoltan Dornyei
The final model that emerged from our study, presented in Figure 7.1, confirms our
earlier observation based on correlation analysis that Integrativeness plays a key role in
L2 motivation, mediating the effects of all the other attitudinal/motivational variables
on the criterion measure Language choice (and we obtained exactly the same results with
another criterion measure, Intended effort to study the L2). Curiously, the immediate
antecedents of this latent variable were Attitudes towards L2 speakers/community and
Instrumentality, thus, the three variables that the correlations in Table 7.1 highlighted
emerged as the central motivational components in the SEM model as well, and the
model also gave us an indication about how these variables related to each other and
to the criterion measure. What is more, this was a very consistent finding because it
applied to all the different target languages and all the three waves of our survey. The
only problem was that what we found did not make much theoretical sense:
'Integrativeness' turned out to be the principal motivation factor in an environment
where 'integrating' was not very meaningful (since there was nothing really to integrate
into) and, furthermore, integrativeness was closely associated with two very different
variables: faceless pragmatic incentives and personal attitudes towards members of the
L2 community. It was clear that we needed a new theory to accommodate these
findings.
Theoretical considerations
Parallel to conducting the empirical research outlined above, I became familiar with an
intriguing new theoretical approach in psychology that looked particularly promising
with regard to applying it to L2 motivation: the conceptualization of possible selves. First
introduced by Markus and Nurius (1986), the concept of the possible self represents
an individual's ideas of what they might become, what they would like to become and
what they are afraid ofbecoming. That is, possible selves are specific representations of
one's self in future states, involving thoughts, images and senses, and are in many ways
the manifestations of one's goals and aspiration. From a motivational point of view, two
types of possible selves - the ideal self and the ought self - seemed particularly relevant
(Higgins 1987). The former refers to the representation of the attributes that someone
would ideally like to possess (i.e. representation of hopes, aspirations or wishes),
whereas the latter refers to the attributes that one believes one ought to possess (i.e. a
representation of someone's sense of duty, obligations or responsibilities) and which
therefore may bear little resemblance to desires or wishes. The motivational aspect of
these self-guides was explained by Higgins's (1987, 1998) self-discrepancy theory, pos-
tulating that motivation involves the desire for people to reduce the discrepancy
between their actual and ideal/ought selves.
This self framework not only made intuitive sense to me but it also seemed to
offer a good explanation of our Hungarian findings. Looking at 'integrativeness' from
the self perspective, the concept can be conceived of as the L2-specific facet of one's
ideal self. If our ideal self is associated with the mastery of an L2, that is, if the person
that we would like to become is proficient in the L2, we can be described in Gardner's
(1985) terminology as having an integrative disposition. Thus, the central theme of
the emerging new theory was the equation of the motivational dimension that has
Researching motivation 79
Ideal L2 Self, which is the L2-specific facet of one's 'ideal self. If the person we would
like to become speaks an L2, the 'ideal L2 self is a powerful motivator to learn the L2.
8o Zoltan Dornyei
Ought-to L2 Self, which concerns the attributes that one believes one ought to possess
to meet expectations and to avoid possible negative outcomes.
L2 Learning Experience, which concerns situated motives related to the immediate
learning environment and experience (e.g. the impact of the teacher, the curriculum,
the peer group, the experience of success).
Validating the Over the last two years my research students - Stephen Ryan, Tatsuya Taguchi and
L2 Michael Magid - and I have been conducting large-scale survey research in Japan and
Motivational China to validate the L2 Motivational Self System (for details of the surveys, see Ryan
Self System 2009; Taguchi et al. 2009). We believed that if we can support the main tenets of the
theory by data coming from foreign language contexts that are very different from the
Hungarian learning environment that the L2 self approach originated from, this would
be a powerful validity argument of the construct. The studies did indeed confirm that
our assumptions were correct; in the following I present some of the key findings: (a)
correlations between traditional Integrativeness and the Ideal L2 Self to check whether
the two constructs can indeed be equated; (b) correlations of Integrativeness and the
Ideal L2 Se/fwith criterion measures to see which variable does a better job at explaining
motivated behaviour; (c) correlations between aspects of Instrumentality and the
Ought-to 12 Self to check whether traditional instrumentality can indeed be divided
into two distinct types.
University University
students students Secondary
Total (non-English (English school Adult
sample majors) majors) pupils learners
1 ,586 Japanese learners Ideal L2 Self .71 .71 .61 ' ' • • ;• ' . ', -j
(Taguchi et al. forthcoming) Integrativeness .63 .64 .49 ' - • • •;• - •:;
1 ,328 Chinese learners Ideal L2 Self .55 .52 .51 .69 .51
(Taguchi et al. forthcoming) Integrativeness .52 .47 .53 .63 .44
Table 7.3 Correlations of Integrativeness and the ideal L2 Se//with Intended effort
to the Ought-to L2 Self. These two types are only moderately related to each other anc
show a distinct correlation pattern with the two self dimensions.
Conclusion This chapter discussed a major theoretical shift that has been taking place within the
field of L2 motivation research. I described how a new paradigm has emerged from
both theoretical considerations and research results, and then presented the main com-
ponents of the newly proposed 'L2 Motivational Self System'. In the second part of the
chapter I provided empirical data from three different surveys involving over 5,300 par-
ticipants to validate the new construct. The correlational results clearly indicated that:
(a) Integmtiveness and the Ideal L2 Self lap into the same construct, but the Ideal L2 Self
does a better job at explaining variance in the criterion measures; (b) the traditionally
conceived concept of Instrumentality mixes up two types of pragmatic motives (with
a promotion vs. a prevention focus) that show a rather different relationship pattern
with the Ideal and the Ought-to L2 Selves. These results are all in accordance with the
proposed theory and thus provide a strong validity argument for it. We should reiterate
here that in the current study the third main component of the 'L2 Motivational Self
System1, the L2 Learning Experience, was not measured.
References Coetzee-Van Rooy, S. (2006) 'Integrativeness: Untenable for world Englishes learners?' World
Englishes, 25: 437-50.
Dornyei, Z. (2001) Teaching and researching motivation, Harlow: Longman.
Dornyei, Z. (2005) The psychology of the language learner: Individual differences in second
language acquisition, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Dornyei, Z. (2007) Research methods in applied linguistics: Quantitative, qualitative and
mixed methodologies, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Dornyei, Z. (2009) 'The L2 Motivational Self System', in Z. Dornyei and E. Ushioda (eds),
Motivation, language identity and the 12 self, Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Dornyei, Z. and Csizer, K. (2002) 'Some dynamics of language attitudes and motivation:
Results of a longitudinal nationwide survey', Applied Linguistics, 23: 421-62.
Dornyei, Z. and Ushioda, E. (eds) (2009) Motivation, language identity and the L2 self,
Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Researching motivation 83
Dornyei, Z., Csizer, K. and Nemeth, N. (2006) Motivation, language attitudes and global-
isation: A Hungarian perspective, Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Gardner, R.C. (1985) Social psychology and second language learning: The role of attitudes and
motivation, London: Edward Arnold.
Gardner, R.C. (2001) 'Integrative motivation and second language acquisition', in Z. Dornyei
and R. Schmidt (eds), Motivation and second language acquisition, Honolulu, HI:
University of Hawai'i Press.
Higgins, E.T. (1987) 'Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect', Psychological Review,
94:319-40.
Higgins, E.T. (1998) 'Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational
principle1, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30: 1-46.
Lamb, M. (2004) 'Integrative motivation in a globalizing world', System, 32: 3-19.
Manolopoulou-Sergi, E. (2004) 'Motivation within the information processing model of
foreign language learning', System, 32:427-41. . • .,•'",=.
Markus,H. andNurius, P. (1986) 'Possible selves' American Psychologist, 41: 954-69.
Noels, K.A. (2003) 'Learning Spanish as a second language: Learners' orientations and per-
ceptions of their teachers' communication style' in Z. Dornyei (ed.), Attitudes,
orientations and motivations in language learning, Oxford: Blackwell.
Ryan, S. (2009) 'Self and identity in L2 motivation in Japan: The ideal L2 self and Japanese
learners of English', in Z. Dornyei and E. Ushioda (eds), Motivation, language identity
and the L2 self, Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
Taguchi, T, Magid, M. and Papi, M. (2009) 'The L2 motivational self system amongst
Chinese, Japanese and Iranian learners of English: A comparative study', in Z. Dornyei
and E. Ushioda (eds), Motivation, language identity and the L2 self, Clevedon:
Multilingual Matters.
Ushioda, E. (2001) 'Language learning at university: Exploring the role of motivational
thinking', in Z. Dornyei and R. Schmidt (eds), Motivation and second language
acquisition, Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.
Yashima, T. (2000) 'Orientations and motivations in foreign language learning: A study of
Japanese college students', JACETBulletin, 31: 121-33.