Giving Children A Voice Investigation of
Giving Children A Voice Investigation of
A Voice
Investigation of children’s
experiences of participation in
consultation and decision-making
in Irish hospitals
Dr Imelda Coyne
Ms Eilis Hayes
Dr Pamela Gallagher
Ms Geraldine Regan
School of Nursing, Dublin City University
September 2006
ii
CONTENTS
Acknowledgements v
Executive Summary 1
Findings 2
Conclusions 4
Recommendations 6
Chapter 1: Introduction 7
Background to the study 8
Chapter 4: Findings 27
Children’s experiences of hospitalisation 28
Children’s experiences of consultation 29
Factors that enhanced the consultation process 30
Parents’ role in the consultation process 32
Children’s experiences of non-consultation 34
Factors that inhibited the consultation process 40
Views on decision-making process 40
Factors influencing decision-making process for children 42
iii
Chapter 5: Discussion and Conclusions 45
Children’s experiences of being in hospital 46
Children’s experiences of consultation 46
Factors that enhanced the consultation process 47
Parents’ role as advocates in the consultation process 48
Children’s experiences of non-consultation 49
Factors that inhibited the consultation process 52
Children’s views on decision-making process 53
Frameworks of consultation 55
Conclusions 56
Bibliography 59
Appendices
Appendix 1a: Hart’s Ladder of Participation 70
Appendix 1b: Hart’s Degrees of Participation 71
Appendix 2: Parent information letter 72
Appendix 3: Information sheet for older child 73
Appendix 4: Parental consent – Permission to approach child 74
Appendix 5: Parental consent – Permission for child participation 75
Appendix 6: Older child consent 76
Appendix 7: Younger child consent 77
Appendix 8: Protocol 1 — Child displaying anxiety during interview 78
Appendix 9: Protocol 2 — Signs of illness during interview 79
Appendix 10: Protocol 3 — Disclosure of sensitive information 80
Appendix 11: Treseder’s Degrees of Participation 81
Appendix 12: Children’s Levels of Participation 82
iv
Acknowledgements
On behalf of the research team at Dublin City
University, I wish to make a number of
acknowledgements. I would like to thank the
Office of the Minister for Children for providing
the funding for this project and for making it
possible to hear children’s voices. I would like to
thank the children who so willingly allowed us
to hear their voices in relation to their
experiences of consultation and involvement in
their healthcare. The children generously gave
us their time and without their input this study
could not have been conducted.
I wish to acknowledge and express appreciation
to the Directors of Nursing, Geraldine O’Regan,
Rita O’Shea and Mary Duff, for facilitating the
project. I would also like to thank my research
assistant, Eilis Hayes, for her contribution. I
would like to thank the parents who assisted
with the consent process and supported the
project. Finally, I would like to thank the nurses,
doctors and other healthcare professionals who
contributed to the success of the project.
I hope that the results of this study will
contribute towards our understanding of
children’s views on consultation in the
healthcare setting. I hope that the findings will
advance the realisation of Goal 1 of the National
Children’s Strategy and the process of
recognising children’s views in matters that
affect their health and their lives. All errors and
omissions are the author's, whose views do not
necessarily reflect those of the Office of the
Minister for Children.
Dr Imelda Coyne
Principal Investigator/Project Director
School of Nursing, Faculty of Science and Health
Dublin City University, Dublin 9
v
vi
EXECUTIVE
SUMMARY
1
The focus of this research was to investigate sick children’s experiences of participation in consultation
and decision-making within the healthcare setting. This aim led to the following objectives:
• To describe children’s experiences of consultation in the healthcare setting.
• To identify the factors that enhance children’s involvement in consultation and the
decision-making process.
• To identify the factors that hinder children from involvement in consultation and the
decision-making process.
• To explore strategies that will empower children to participate in their own healthcare
decisions.
The research was comprised of the following elements:
• A literature review and information gathering.
• Focus groups and single interviews with acute and chronically sick children in hospital. The
sample was comprised of children aged 7-18 (n = 55), drawn from the population of children
in three hospitals.
Findings
Experience of consultation
The children had varying experiences of being involved in consultations and decision-making,
which was consistent with previous research. Some children had positive experiences of being
involved, consulted and heard in relation to their information needs. The children liked receiving
information because it helped prepare them for what to expect and thus ameliorated their worries
and provided reassurance. Receiving information and having their views respected enhanced
children’s understanding and adaptation to the hospitalisation. The overall effect of being involved
in the consultation process was that the children reported feeling valued, involved, comfortable
and less anxious.
The significant factors that enhanced the consultation process for the children were (1) familiarity
with hospital and procedures; (2) age level; (3) relationship with health professionals; and (4)
time. The age and maturity of the children were important factors in the extent to which health
professionals respected their wishes and needs. It appears that health professionals tended to
consult with mainly older children, which implies that younger children could be left with
unanswered questions and insufficient information. It was also evident that knowing health
professionals contributed to a trusting relationship, which enabled children to feel that they could
ask questions freely and express their concerns.
Children need time to absorb information. Thus, having sufficient time with health professionals
clearly enhanced the children’s ability to participate because they had time for discussion and time
for clarification of information. However, some children reported difficulty in getting to know
health professionals due to lack of time, lack of frequent contact, many patients, busy workload
and lack of continuity due to shift work. It is not surprising then that only children with chronic
illness reported forming trusting relationships with health professionals. The development of a
trusting relationship and good communication between health professionals and children should
be fundamental aspects of good practice.
2
content to share the consultation with their parents so that they could then help explain and
clarify information received from health professionals. These children were of different ages, which
indicates that the relationship between children’s ages and preferences for communication is not
as straightforward as assumed. Most of the children reported difficulty participating due to health
professionals’ style of communication and therefore were very reliant on their parents to be their
advocate/interpreter in the consultation process.
Although most children appeared to value their parents’ role in the consultation process, some
expressed unease with the constraining aspects of the role. The parents inhibited some children’s
attempts to participate in the consultation process by answering questions on their behalf, by
telling them to stay quiet, by reprimanding them for interrupting the discussion and by withholding
information. It may be argued that the lack of support from some parents in relation to children’s
active participation sends a clear message to children that they should be seen and not heard. The
parents’ actions may have reflected their need to protect their children’s well-being and thus were
well intentioned. Alternatively, parents may hold the belief that children should be seen and not
heard, and thus discourage children’s active participation. Parents may experience difficulty
balancing their need to protect their children with encouraging children’s participation. Therefore,
parents may need help to consider how children’s interests might best be served and encouragement
from health professionals to facilitate children’s participation in the communication process.
Lack of consultation
Consultations were largely carried out between parents and health professionals, which seemed to
leave children without a voice. Most of the children in this study appeared to occupy a marginal
position in consultations, without direct access to information about their healthcare. The children
expressed a range of fears about the hospitalisation, particularly fear of the unknown, but also
inadequate information and communication led to them feeling unprepared and anxious. Some
children had to undergo procedures without adequate information or preparation, which created
stress and unnecessary anxiety.
Avoidance of children’s questions and children’s suspicion of health professionals withholding
information from them potentially undermined their trust in health professionals’ good intentions
and added to anxiety levels. Some children felt that their contributions were not given due weight
and consideration, and the feeling of being disbelieved caused them stress and potentially
undermined their confidence. The children expressed strong feelings about being excluded from the
consultation process, such as feeling disappointed, sad, confused, angry, ‘rotten’, worried, misled,
shocked, betrayed, lonely and ignored. The range and depth of these feelings clearly illustrate the
impact of non-consultation on many children.
The children actively sought involvement in consultations with health professionals, but were
hindered by factors that included (1) fear of causing ‘trouble’ by asking questions; (2) lacking time
with health professionals; (3) difficulty contacting health professionals; (4) being ignored;
(5) being disbelieved; (6) health professionals not listening; (7) not knowing health professionals;
(8) difficulty understanding medical terminology; (9) being sick; and (10) parents’ actions.
Although there were many factors that appeared to obstruct children’s participation, the key
factors were the communication styles, attitudes and behaviours of the health professionals. The
children generally reported that health professionals tended to ‘do things’ to them with very brief
or no explanations. Many children reported being unable to ask questions because of being rushed
during their interactions with health professionals. It was obvious that children’s ability to
participate in discussions about their treatment was hindered by health professionals allowing
insufficient time for discussions.
Views on decision-making
Some children wanted to be involved in the decision-making process, while others were happy for
the decisions to be left to parents and health professionals. Leaving decisions to parents/health
professionals does not indicate a lack of competence; rather, it demonstrates that children have
the ability to choose. Children who may be competent to make their own decisions may,
3
nevertheless, choose to allow parents to make decisions on their behalf. Participation rights do not
necessarily confer the right to be the main decider, nor do children want to exclude their parents
from the process. It appears that the issue of decision-making should be seen as existing on a
continuum rather than an ‘all or nothing’ approach. The view of ‘all or nothing’ obscures the fact
that there are multiple levels and ways to involve children in decision-making. There are also
different types of decisions ranging from minor to major and the decision-making process can
change depending on the situational context.
Children of all age groups (from 7-18) indicated a willingness to participate in decision-making
and therefore their age was not the deciding factor. There was also variability in the children’s
beliefs about the ages at which children could be expected to be competent. This indicates that
health professionals should assess each child’s ability and willingness to participate, rather than
making assumptions about involvement based on a child’s age. Children’s ability to participate may
be frequently underestimated since health professionals tended to communicate with mainly older
children, although there were exceptions.
The findings indicate that decision-making for children is a complex process that evolves over time
and that may be shared or contested with parents and health professionals depending on the type
of decision. It was evident that parents play an influential role in children’s ability to participate,
which indicates that an awareness of the family’s usual pattern of decision-making can be helpful.
It suggests a pragmatic approach, recognising that children need protection while at the same
time allowing flexibility for the child’s emerging knowledge and self-determination.
Conclusions
The children in this study expressed strong desires to have their voices heard in relation to everyday
decisions about their care and treatment. Many children felt that they had a right to be involved
because it was about matters that affected them and their bodies. This is in keeping with Goal 1 of
the National Children’s Strategy, which states that ‘children will have a voice in matters which affect
them and their views will be given due weight in accordance with their age and maturity’.
It is clear, however, from the children’s accounts that many of them were not included in everyday
decisions about matters that affected them. The fact that the experiences of children recruited
from different locations were essentially similar suggests that difficulties of communication
between children and health professionals are widespread. The findings from this study suggest,
therefore, that there is a need for a greater awareness by all stakeholders that a failure to include
children and young people in the decision-making process can have adverse effects on their
physical and emotional welfare. Children are important users of the health service, yet their views
appear to be given insufficient priority or respect by health professionals.
The findings from this study show that children wanted to be kept informed about their care,
wanted to be adequately prepared for specific procedures and, in particular, wanted to be involved
in the ‘small’ decisions about everyday care. Children and young people said they worried when
their parents were taken aside to talk with health professionals. Parents themselves were reported
as playing a critical role in facilitating and encouraging children’s participation, but sometimes in
obstructing it. Parents need to be supported in enabling their children to participate and their
advice should be sought by health professionals on how best to involve individual children.
Three areas of critical importance were identified by children and young people in respect of the
decision-making process.
• First, they reported that information provided should be tailored so that it is accessible and
appropriate according to the individual’s level of understanding. Where children cannot be
provided with their first choice, they strongly suggested they should be given an explanation
of why their preference cannot be met and offered alternatives where possible.
• Second, children and young people reported that sufficient time needs to be given to them
to consider the information provided and also to be able to talk to health professionals and
others so that clarifications can be sought. This can be greatly assisted by the allocation of
a primary carer (nurse) so that a relationship can develop.
4
• Third, assumptions about the ability or willingness of individual children to participate should
be based on the needs of the child. Health professionals should be aware that some children
could be reluctant to challenge aspects of their care, while others use an information-limiting
strategy as a mechanism for coping. For that reason, health professionals, while actively
encouraging participation, should also be sensitive to the needs of individual children so that
they can be best supported to make decisions that they themselves feel able to make.
A commitment from hospital senior management to the principle of consultation with children was
identified as important in this study and it was suggested that this commitment could take a
number of different forms. It could, for example, include:
• an education and awareness campaign for parents, children and health professionals;
• initial and on-going training commitment for staff in communicating with children and
ensuring their right to participate is asserted;
• the development of codes of conduct in this regard that would be subject to evaluation;
• the provision of a forum that includes children and young people to address specific needs
identified by them (such as those relating to space, recreation, play facilities, play therapists,
equipment, the need for separate units for adolescents and appropriately designed information
materials — all of which emerged throughout this study);
• the development of materials to promote children’s right to participate in healthcare decisions
that affect them (e.g. art activities, stories, poems, role-playing, graffiti boards and web-based
notice boards).
The findings from this study suggest that it is important that all stakeholders, including senior
managers, health professionals and parents, develop a greater awareness of the need to
communicate with children in a way that facilitates their participation in issues that affect them.
Health professionals’ attitudes and assumptions about children need to be challenged and
addressed, and health professionals need to re-evaluate their practices to ensure that they embrace
a more flexible approach to children’s participation in consultations and decisions. Health
professionals have a duty and responsibility to ensure that children of all ages and abilities have
a voice in decisions about their health and to exercise control as much as possible about what is
done to their bodies.
The facilitation of children’s participation may be seen by some as time-consuming and labour-
intensive, but it should be recognised that all professionals have an ethical and clinical obligation
to support children’s involvement in consultations and decision-making. The benefits for children
include:
• better provision of information;
• having an opportunity to express feelings;
• developing confidence;
• increasing skills in decision-making;
• enhancing self-esteem;
• overall coping with hospitalisation.
It is imperative that efforts are directed towards facilitating and supporting children’s involvement
because it will improve the quality of care provided for children and their families. Furthermore, it
is an important investment, with immense future potential benefits for children, staff and the
health service.
5
Recommendations
There are a number of specific recommendations arising from the conclusions drawn above:
Recommendation 1: Steps should be taken to increase awareness of all stakeholders — including
children and young people, parents, healthcare professionals and managers — of children’s right to
be consulted in matters that affect them.
Recommendation 2: Initial and on-going training programmes focusing on children’s participation
should be developed and implemented for all healthcare staff working with children.
Recommendation 3: Appropriate mechanisms at an organisational level to ensure children’s
participation within healthcare settings should be developed. These may include formal structures
where children are represented, facilitated to participate and have their views taken into account
in the decision-making process.
Recommendation 4: Special attention should be paid to ensuring children in hospital are aware
of their right to participate and that they develop the necessary skills to do so in a meaningful way.
Recommendation 5: Written information provided for children should be designed and presented
in a way that ensures it is accessible and relevant to children.
Recommendation 6: Research in the area of children’s participation in Ireland is at a relatively
early stage and additional research should be undertaken, particularly in the area of how parents
and health professionals can best facilitate children to participate.
6
1 INTRODUCTION
7
Background to the study
The past decade has seen a growing recognition of the importance of children’s rights and
listening to and consulting with children, both at a national (Department of Health and Children,
2000a and 2000b) and international level (United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child,
1989; Mayall, 1995; Lowes, 1996; Roche, 1999; Shevlin and Rose, 2003).
The United Nations Convention on the Rights of the Child (1989) states that the best interests of
the child must be the primary consideration and that the child’s views must be given due weight
in accordance with the child’s age and maturity. Although children’s rights have increasingly been
acknowledged in the last decade, there are still certain characteristics attributed to children that
can reduce the relevance of attempts to listen to their views (Flatman, 2002). In recent years,
changes in Irish society have attempted to dispel this by the creation of a National Children’s
Office, a Children’s Ombudsman and a National Children’s Strategy, whose vision aims to create an
Ireland where ‘children are respected as young citizens with a valued contribution to make and a
voice of their own’ (Department of Health and Children, 2000b). Although the National Children’s
Strategy is not a rights-based document, Goal 1 of the Strategy clearly establishes the right of
children to be heard as a matter of national public policy: ‘Children will have a voice in matters
which affect them and their views will be given due weight in accordance with their age and
maturity.’ The National Children’s Strategy emphasises the importance of all organisations
developing children’s consultation mechanisms so that children are involved in decisions that
affect their lives.
In consideration of these influential changes, it is clear that views on childhood are changing,
with an increased awareness of the child as an active citizen in Ireland. However, children’s views
represent a rich source of data that has been largely unexplored until recently. This is because
most of the research in the past has obtained data from parents or professionals on children’s lives
and has thus been on or about children, rather than with children. This is now changing.
Increasingly, researchers are recognising the importance of directly recording children’s own
perspectives, as evidenced by recent Irish studies (McAuley and Brattman, 2002; Crisis Pregnancy
Agency, 2003; Devine, 2003; Lodge and Lynch, 2003; Shevlin and Rose, 2003) and by British
studies (Alderson, 1993; Brannen et al, 1994; Oakley et al, 1995; Alderson and Goodey, 1996; Hill
et al, 1996; Hood et al, 1996; Ireland and Holloway, 1996; Mahon et al, 1996; Coyne, 1998; Balen
et al, 2001). The National Children’s Office (NCO) — now the Office of the Minister for Children —
has a strong commitment to hearing children’s voices and has established a comprehensive
programme of research into matters that affect children’s lives (NCO, 2004 and 2005).
Despite these studies, the research into children’s participation in Ireland has been scarce and
particularly so in the area of healthcare. Shevlin and Rose (2003) carried out research into the
marginalisation of disabled children in Ireland, but there has been no research that has focused on
children’s participation in consultation and in relation to healthcare. There is limited empirical
data from the children’s perspective here in Ireland. Generally, interventions have been developed
for adult populations and these have then been extrapolated for work with children.
Increasingly, it is being recognised that children are present and future healthcare consumers, which
has resulted in calls for empowering children to be active in their healthcare (Pittman, 1992; Hart
and Chesson, 1998). This does not mean, however, that all children want to be responsible for all
decisions about their care, particularly when they are feeling sick and vulnerable. Children may want
to participate in discussions about their care so that their voices are heard in relation to their
needs rather than have full responsibility for decision-making. Alternatively, children’s needs may
vary along a dependence/independence continuum due to the effects of the illness and disruption
of the hospitalisation. This indicates the need for research of children’s experiences of participation
in consultation and within the Irish healthcare setting.
8
2 LITERATURE
REVIEW
9
‘The only thing I would change [in Ireland] would be that we would listen to our nation’s
children and young people much more, as we are the next generation that will have to
run the country. The voice of innocence can sometimes be wiser than a hundred years of
experience.’
Report of the Public Consultation for the National Children’s Strategy
(Department of Health and Children, 2000a)
These are wise words, yet they come from the mouth of one who is often seen as having limited
wisdom — they come from the mouth of a child. Lowden (2002) argues that childhood ‘belongs to
children least of all’ and that the study of children remains ‘largely a study of adults’ attitudes to
and practices with children’.
Pinning down an exact definition of childhood is difficult since children’s experiences of it differ
not only throughout history (Aries, 1962; Hart; 1991), but also geographically in modern-day
society. Childhood for a street child in Calcutta is a very different experience from that of an
affluent child growing up in the USA. Similarly, experiential differences in childhood can vary
within the same location: growing up as a homeless child in Ireland is in stark contrast to an Irish
child growing up with a roof over his or her head. Notions of weakness and need for protection are
important issues in childhood. One common theme of childhood is that it should be a happy time.
Yet this is not always the case as the assumed innocence and helplessness that accompanies the
notion of childhood can often be abused by those in power, namely adults (Thomas, 2000).
Aries (1962) highlights that prior to the 16th century, children were seen as ‘small adults’ and that
once they were over the age of 6, they were not viewed separately from adults. They were viewed
as valuable property for their contribution to family work and supporting their parents in their old
age. They remained as such up until the 18th century, when a sudden vulnerability status was
attached to children (Lowden, 2002). Hart (1991) notes that this status of the vulnerable child
went hand-in-hand with rapid changes in the conditions of immigration, industrialisation and
urbanisation at the time. Children were seen as needing protection from these changes, which were
viewed as having the potential to create undesirable behaviour and to threaten the very core of
society. Hart describes it as very much an era of ‘child-saving’. The 19th century onwards saw an
increased separation of the child from adults, to the stage where children became a special class of
people in need of protection from those who were much older and wiser.
Historically, children did not have any rights. They were either seen as property or in need of
protection (Aries, 1962; Hart, 1991; Lowden, 2002). However, the introduction of the UN
Convention on the Rights of the Child (UNCRC) in 1989 challenged the way in which children were
treated and sought to secure improvement in their lives. The UNCRC gave substance to the rights of
children and affirmed that children need special consideration and that they have a voice. This led
many nations to draw upon its principles in relation to children and to draft and amend legislation.
In Ireland, it led to the development of the National Children’s Office and the production of the
National Children’s Strategy in 2000 on how to implement children’s rights in Irish society and how
to make society more child-centred. People are beginning to take note that children have a voice,
but more importantly that it needs to be heard. Children’s views are now being sought in relation to
education, social care, disability, family law, health, play and becoming an active citizen (Thomas
and O’Kane, 1999; Murray and Hallett, 2000; Department of Health and Children, 2000b; O’Quigley,
2000; Oldfield and Fowler, 2004). One of the most challenging of these areas is that of health where
not only children can be found in a passive role, but adults (parents) also who can often find
themselves subordinate to the expert knowledge of healthcare professionals (Ross, 1997).
In Scandinavia and Hungary, health professionals are beginning to involve children in consultations
and decision-making related to healthcare (Schultz, 1993). Much of the research into children’s
right to participate and make decisions regarding their healthcare has been conducted in
Scandinavia and the USA. There exists a small body of research in the United Kingdom, while in
Ireland the research is lacking.
This review will look at the changing face of children’s rights, the child as an active healthcare
consumer, and children and empowerment in the healthcare system, before concluding with an
examination of ‘who knows best’ with regard to children’s healthcare.
10
Children’s voices, children’s rights
Rodham (1973) has described children’s rights as ‘a slogan in need of a definition’. What are
rights? Are they just words on a piece of paper, a given thing we have in society or are they
acquired through practice? Is it only when our rights are exercised or denied that we realise we
have them? Thomas (2000) argues that the debates surrounding rights are whether children should
have rights and whose obligation it is to meet those rights.
Children’s rights can be classified into two different categories — nurturance rights and self-
determination rights (Archard, 1993; Thomas, 2000; Kennedy and Mohr, 2001). Nurturance rights
(sometimes termed ‘welfare rights’) are the child’s right to the provision of beneficial objects, such
as education, housing and healthcare. Self-determination rights fall under the category of the
child’s right to participation and control over his or her life through active participation in choice.
The UN Convention on the Rights of the Child gave increased societal emphasis to the need for a
balance between the child’s nurturance rights and their self-determination rights (Lowden, 2002).
Kennedy and Mohr (2001), however, note that although a balance is what is strived for, quite often
societies can tip the balance in either favour. They argue that in the USA there is strong political
support for nurturance rights, but far less support for self-determination rights despite the large
numbers of children’s movements present in that country. Very often, exercising nurturance rights
can leave you without self-determination rights, and vice versa. Take the example of a child worker
in Guatemala: he or she may live alone and exercise their right to self-determination, yet the
absence of a home, a family, education, healthcare is an abuse of their nurturance rights through
no fault of their own. Archard (1993), drawing on Piaget’s theory of cognitive development, sees
both nurturance and self-determination rights as being determined by age — that the needs of
young children up to the age of 7 are chiefly to do with nurturance, while self-determination rights
are of limited relevance to them and only apply to middle childhood and adolescence.
Lowden (2002) states that most research on children’s rights falls into three main categories of
approaches — protectionist, liberationist and pragmatist. Lowden asserts that unfortunately
children are still defined from a protectionist adult perspective, which hinders the development of
competence. Only future research will deem if change is possible. Protectionists argue that children
need protection while they learn to exercise sound judgement and gain the knowledge they need
to manage successfully in the world. However, Archard (1993) notes that it is quite often those
who are supposed to protect children who may end up abusing them.
Liberationists argue that the rights, privileges, duties and responsibilities of adult citizens should
be made available to children and young people, and that they should have an active role in
society (Holt, 1974; Cohen, 1980; Franklin, 1986; Van Eys, 1986).
Pragmatists argue that there should be a balance between the liberationist and protectionist ideas,
one that recognises the protection needed for children while at the same time allowing some
flexibility for the child’s emerging knowledge (Hart, 1991; Harrison et al, 1997; Ross, 1997). The
study by Alderson (1993) on children’s consent to surgery found that several young patients wanted
to ‘consent’ to surgery but were unwilling to sign the consent form, thus signalling a desire to be
involved but also a desire for informed adult support. With the problems surrounding informed
consent, competency and what exactly the child’s ‘best interests’ are, the pragmatic approach seems
to be a safe one. It encompasses the best of two extreme positions and allows a balance between
protection and self-determination, which may help to overcome conflicts, confusion and problems
between the diverse approaches.
11
organisations in England (n = 849) in order to establish the extent to which children are involved
in public decision-making (Oldfield and Fowler, 2004). They found that the level of participation
was greatest in organisations that had a specific remit to work with children, but more limited in
health and criminal organisations. Similarly, in Ireland the Children’s Rights Alliance and the
National Youth Council found that voluntary organisations were more likely to consult with children
as compared to statutory organisations (McAuley and Brattman, 2002). Sloper and Lightfoot (2003)
note how little information is available on children’s involvement in healthcare treatment services,
although more information is available about children’s involvement in local government. Children
account for up to one quarter of all UK GP consultations and represent 30% of the workload in
Accident & Emergency departments (Hart and Chesson, 1998). Children are without question
important users of the health service, yet their views are given insufficient priority by policy-
makers and health service professionals.
The research studies that have been conducted with children in hospital indicate that children
have varying experiences of being consulted and involved in their care (Alderson, 1990 and 1993;
Runeson et al, 2000; Carter, 2002; Coyne, 2003) and that children are largely excluded from the
process (Angst and Deatrick, 1996). Children also experience less than optimal communication and
have unanswered informational needs (Alderson, 1990 and 1993; Epstein and Wayman, 1998;
Beresford and Sloper, 2003). Similarly, studies on children’s medical encounters in clinics reported
that all children, even the older ones, were routinely excluded from discussions and granted limited
autonomy (Strong, 1979; Silverman, 1987; Inman, 1991; Shiminski-Maher, 1993; Van Dulmen,
1998; Carter, 2002). Pearlman and Abromovitch (1987) note how quite often no explanation of
treatment or reasons for it are given to children. In their study of child-parent-physician
interaction in healthcare consultations, they found that 92% of the children studied had questions
regarding the consultation. Epstein and Wayman (1998) argue that communication with the child
patient is imperative, finding medical procedures to be lacking in ‘appropriate verbal structuring’
for infants, while conversations regarding treatment were unresponsive to children’s interaction. It
appears, therefore, that the practice of speaking with children, listening to them and involving
them is not widespread among health professionals.
It may be argued that cults of paternalism and protectionism have led to the child being seen as
passive, with decisions being made by parents or health professionals. Research has indicated that
age, communication and experience of all have a role to play in how children are perceived as
inactive, incompetent consumers of their own healthcare (Brewster, 1982; Igoe, 1991; Pittman,
1992; Oppong and Heycock, 1997; Epstein and Wayman, 1998). Some research found that children
were more likely to be involved if they were older (Alderson, 1990 and 1993; Deatrick, 1984).
Brewster (1982) argues that children under 7 years of age see illness as occurring as if by ‘magic’
or else as the result of bad behaviour; they have no real concept of their illness. From the ages of
7 to 11, children have a better understanding of their illness, but inaccurately infer the reasons for
treatment. Children from 11 years up to adolescence have a more detailed understanding and
become aware that illness can be aggravated by psychological factors (Bibace and Walshe, 1981).
It has been argued that the present level of knowledge on children’s cognitive development has
not been sufficient to provide clear guidance on children’s ability for self-determination (Hart,
1991) and developmental theories are an inadequate base for assessing children’s abilities for self-
determination (Alderson, 1993; Mayall, 1994, 1996a and 1996b).
The effect of hospitalisation can be a factor in constraining children’s active healthcare
participation. Hospitalised communication with children takes place within a typically chaotic and
unstructured environment, which can be unsettling for a child. Bonn (1993) argues that a lack of
communication leads the child to be unfamiliar with routines, procedures and people, which can
lead to psychological distress. Peterson (1989) argues that children may not always want to be
involved and adopt an information-avoidance coping style. Thompson (1994) also found
information-limiting to be an effective form of coping behaviour. LaMontagne (2000) sees coping
as being an individualised experience: children will choose to seek information actively or else
purposely avoid it, depending on their individual needs and various situations. Research has shown
how the presence of healthcare professionals and parents and a positive experience of
12
hospitalisation can increase children’s autonomy (Jago, 1987; McClowery, 1988). Jago (1987)
suggests that a positive experience of hospitalisation can help the child to be a more active health
consumer and the child ‘interested in its own health, will listen in on the doctor’s conversation and
make their own interpretation of it’. While other research has found that children are happy to
remain in a protected role (Lumley et al, 1990; Tong and Sparacino, 1994), McClowry (1988) argues
that research into the effects of hospitalisation on self-determination in children is inconclusive and
that predisposing family factors need to be considered.
What is the future for children’s active participation in their healthcare consumption? Pittman
(1992) argues that ‘empowering children and adolescents to recognise their roles in their own
healthcare should be an expectation’. Epstein and Wayman (1998) assert that to transform patients
into active healthcare consumers in a healthcare system that is traditionally paternalistic is a
‘monumental undertaking’. Similarly, Dixon-Woods et al (1999) argue that children’s right to
participation has little chance of being realised until there is more research-based evidence about
the ‘outcomes of, how the competence of children can be assessed, how information can be shared
with children, and how shared should be managed in practice’. This is an important point since
there is a deficit of good quality research on these issues. Dixon-Woods et al (1999) also make the
point that health professionals and parents require reassurance that children’s participation will not
produce adverse effects in the long term, hence the need for good quality evidence to reassure and
guide them in how to manage children’s participation.
This is the era of consumerism and consumer participation in healthcare is viewed as a requisite
for effective bargaining for quality care in the healthcare industry. But the evidence suggests that
there are many obstacles to the realisation of children’s participation in the healthcare arena.
13
willing and able to agree to a course of action based on full knowledge of what the action entails’.
According to Pursell (1995), the lack of research and experience in this area compounds the
prejudices of many healthcare staff who believe that children are incapable of giving consent.
A child may be willing and able to agree to surgery, but what if they do not possess the full
knowledge of what surgery entails? Information may be kept from children by both parents and
healthcare professionals because of the traditional view of the child as incompetent and being in
need of protection. It is difficult to see how children can grow as autonomous decision-makers if
withheld information hinders their ability to give informed consent. Thus, having the relevant
information is as important as having the ability to make a decision.
Other research has found that supplying children with information regarding procedures, including
the associated risks and benefits, helped enhance children’s understanding of the process and
consequences of participation (Lewis and Lewis, 1990) and increased their internal locus of control
and decision-making ability (Taggart et al, 1991; Tieffenberg et al, 2000). Children’s capacity to
participate is determined to a degree by the effort of preparing them to participate and the quality
of the preparation. Similarly, children learn how to make decisions by practice and therefore need
the opportunity to exercise decision-making skills. Pursell (1995) argues that if children had
competency based on their ability to make decisions rather than age, as adults do, then the onus
would be on adults to justify children’s non-involvement.
Lee (1991) argues that participation does not rest solely on consent, but on three levels: consent,
assent and permission. Consent can only be given by an autonomous person who is able to control
their own lives by being in a position of self-determination. Assent is where a child over the age of
7 may give agreement based on evidence of knowledge and understanding, and a willingness to
cooperate. Permission is where a parent gives consent on behalf of the child. Lee (ibid.) sees
adolescents as capable of giving consent; middle children as being capable of assent; and babies
and infants as requiring permission. While much of the focus is on the child’s ability to give
‘informed consent’, the child’s capacity for ‘informed refusal’ must not be diminished (Harrison
et al, 1997; Alderson, 2000; Hallstrom and Elander, 2004). The British Medical Association (2001)
reviewed the law on children’s consent and found that in most cases their wishes have been
overruled by the Court and concluded that the rights of children to refuse treatment is limited.
Thus, children may be seen as competent on some occasions and incompetent on others if they
refuse treatment. It appears that the law on children’s consent is the subject of much academic
debate and its inconsistencies can be confusing for all concerned.
Children’s capability to engage in decision-making also seems to centre on whether the child is
deemed ‘competent’ or not, and commonly held perceptions often see children as ‘incompetent’.
Health professionals, particularly doctors, have traditionally seen child patients as incompetent and
thus of subordinate status in the communication process (Aronsson and Rundstrom, 1998; Gabe et
al, 2004). Beauchamp and Childress (2001) define competence as the ‘ability to perform a task’
and a child is competent to make decisions if ‘he or she has capacity to understand the material
information, to make a judgement about the information in light of his or her values, to intend a
certain outcome, and to freely communicate his or her wishes’. The evolving capacities and
maturity of the child have been set as primary criteria by the UN Convention for determining the
degree of self-determination allowed to children. Yet there are few empirical studies of children’s
competence in decision-making and formal tools to assess this do not exist. Most authors appear
to rely on the seminal study by Weithorn and Campbell (1982), which found that children of
14 years of age had a similar level of competence to adults, while younger children were less
competent than adults. Thomas (2000), in a UK study of social care reviews with 120 children,
found that age was a determining factor in children’s ability to make decisions. He noted that from
the age of 8, participation begins and increases at a steady rate through adolescence, while below
the age of 8, children’s participation was insignificant. In separate studies, Alderson (1993) found
the mean age at which to allow children to make decisions was 10.6 years, while Rushforth (1996)
found it to be 10.3 years.
However, the sole reliance upon age as a determinant of competency has been challenged by other
research. Deatrick et al (2003) revealed shortcomings with the competency scale used by Weithorn
14
and Campbell (1982) to assess children’s competency in decision-making and advised against
reliance on these findings. Alderson and Montgomery (1996) argue that children’s experience, not
necessarily their age, can determine their understanding. The influence of experience on children’s
capacity to make decisions has been confirmed in research with very young children who have
chronic illnesses (Nitschke et al, 1982; Christensen, 1998; Alderson, 1993). In Alderson’s 1993
study with 120 children aged 8-15 years old, she found that children who experience long-term
serious illness or major surgery develop a capacity for understanding that far exceeds common
perceptions of child incompetence. She concluded that competence develops in response to
experience and reasonably high expectations rather than gradually over time through ages or
stages. This indicates that children who experience many hospitalisations may be more capable
of participation in consultations and decisions.
Deatrick et al (2003) suggest that further research is necessary to determine the influence of other
variables in addition to age on children’s capacity to participate, such as cognitive ability, past
illness, gender, parental influence and consequences of the decision. There is a general lack of
information on how various factors may influence children’s ability for self-determination. There is
also a deficit of guidance on how children’s ability for self-determination may be assessed (Hart,
1991; Dixon-Woods et al, 1999).
It is clear from the above discussion that it is a major assumption that capacity can be defined or
measured solely by a child’s age. Furthermore, age may not equate with a child’s psychosocial or
cognitive development. Therefore, judging or justifying children’s participation in consultations and
decisions by reference to their chronological age is a crude measure. It is evident that there are
many other variables that may influence children’s capacity to participate, such as cognitive
ability, insight, judgement, experience, nature of illness, family background and education, to name
a few. Although developmental milestones may give us a general idea of children’s capacities, two
children of the same age may differ quite markedly in their ability to make choices. Hence it may
be argued that apart from babies, children and young people are generally competent to have a
coherent view on a wide range of matters (British Medical Association, 2001; Thomas, 2000).
Therefore, it should follow that the onus should be on health professionals to justify non-
consultation with children about matters and decisions that affect them.
In a period spanning over 30 years, several models, frameworks and schemata have been developed
to structure the involvement of children in decision-making (Hart, 1992; John, 1996; Alderson and
Montgomery, 1996; Treseder, 1997; Lansdown, 2001). Alderson and Montgomery (1996) see
participation as operating on four levels of consultation with the child — being informed,
expressing a view, influencing, and being the main decider. Participation by consultation with the
child allows the child varying degrees of autonomy and responsibility depending on the individual
needs of the child.
Hart (1992) adapted Arnstein’s (1969) model of citizen participation, which was developed for
adults, by altering some wording to make it more appropriate for children and young people. This
was the model chosen for presentation in the National Children’s Strategy (2000). In Hart’s ‘Ladder
of Participation’, participation is commensurate with the child’s age and capacity (see Appendix
1a). The ‘rungs on the ladder’, numbering 8 in total, represent degrees of participation or
opportunities that range from tokenism to child-initiated, shared decisions with adults (see
Appendix 1b). This model has been criticised for several reasons: (1) for structuring various levels
of consultation in a hierarchy, which does not recognise that participation usually exists on a
continuum, with movement across the levels depending on the context of the consultation
(McAuley and Brattman, 2002); (2) that it was developed for children’s participation in projects
rather than general situations (Murray and Hallett, 2000); and (3) that children may have different
views on their levels of participation.
Despite these limitations, the existing frameworks are useful because they promote understanding
of the different degrees of involvement and they highlight the complexities involved in structuring
children’s involvement in consultation and decision-making.
15
Children, empowerment and the healthcare system
According to Toce (2004), children ‘belong to themselves’ and are autonomous human beings.
However, in the healthcare setting, they are traditionally seen as belonging to their parents, in need
of protection and having decisions made for them instead of by them. Alderson (1993) notes that
the tension between children’s empowerment through participation and their need for protection
can be at its most extreme in relation to healthcare. Adults have traditionally exercised control over
children making decisions about their welfare based on the belief that ‘adults know best’. Beals
(2003) and Ross (2002) both note how often the decision about what is best for the child can come
into conflict with what is best for the family. Beals (2003) states that the views of all parties
involved need to be carefully considered and respected when making healthcare decisions relating to
children and that the child’s assent should be sought in all cases. Similarly, the British Medical
Association (2001) emphasises that all children who are capable of expressing a preference should
be involved in decision-making, whether or not they are the main decision-makers.
Ross (2002), however, argues that decisions regarding children’s healthcare should be made using a
model of ‘constrained parental autonomy’, whereby respect is given to the child’s views but their
autonomy is constrained yet accommodated to respect the child’s developing personhood. Ross’s
central argument is that children need a protected period in which to develop self-determination, and
therefore children’s present-day autonomy should be limited because children have limited world
experience and so their decisions are not part of a well-conceived life plan. This view is supported by
Harrison et al (1997) who assert that it is more ‘helpful and respectful of the child to affirm the
parents’ responsibility for the care of their child while allowing the child to exercise choice in a
measure appropriate to his or her level of development and experience of illness and treatment’.
Knafl et al (1988), in a large-scale study of paediatric nurses, families and children, found children
to be seen as ‘objects’ that had things done to them rather than being involved or having a voice.
Aronsson and Rundstrom (1998) argue that there is a cult of paternalism within hospitals, whereby
children are subordinate to both medical and parental authority. They argue that the child’s
subordinate position only serves to conserve parental authority and to increase child dependence.
Coercion and subordination bring minimal choice, respect and freedom for the child — the total
antithesis of what the UN Convention on the Rights of the Child stands for. Bricher (2000) asserts
that a failure to recognise the power imbalance between adults and children can be quite useful to
adults, but denies children their rights.
Prilleltensky et al (2001) argue that a sense of personal control, empowerment and self-
determination are associated with positive mental health. Gaut and Kieckhefer (1988) state that to
become empowered in a health setting, you need first to have the power to understand what you
need to do to become well and, secondly, to have the power to accomplish this action. Quite
often, children may understand their illness but lack the power to access treatment on their own.
Alderson and Montgomery (1996) assert that parents can actively nurture empowerment in children
by encouraging them to exercise it, trusting them, having high expectations and by supporting
them regardless of the outcome.
Empowerment brings choice, but it also brings responsibility. It is worth noting that not all
children may wish to be empowered to make decisions regarding their healthcare. Alderson (1993),
in a study of children’s consent to surgery, noted that while many children were willing to consent
to surgery, they were unwilling to take full responsibility by signing the consent form. So should
children be empowered to make decisions regarding their healthcare? Alderson and Montgomery
(1996) note how over 10,000 children in the UK are the main carer for a sick or disabled relative,
some as young as 3 years of age. This is empowerment, but is it empowerment by choice? Moss-
Morris and Patterson (1995) argue that children are more likely to comply with treatment regimes
and be less distressed if they are allowed to be empowered and take some control of their health
through active participation. Some children, however, do not want to be empowered and wish to
leave the responsibility of choice to their parents and healthcare professionals, while others wish
to actively engage in some form of empowerment but do not wish to be responsible for it.
Initiatives need to be put in place to encourage autonomy in children in a healthcare setting, but
in the end it is up to the child to determine the level of empowerment they wish to engage in and
it is up to parents and healthcare professionals to encourage and respect that chosen level.
16
Conclusions
Throughout this chapter, we have looked at childhood and children’s rights from an historical
perspective, the child as an active health consumer, and children and children’s empowerment in
the healthcare system. So who really does know best when it comes to the health of children?
Liberationists believe that the autonomous child should have all the say in regard to their
healthcare (Holt, 1974; Cohen, 1980; Franklin, 1986). Protectionists argue that the child is
incompetent to make their own decisions and it is up to the parents to decide (Feinberg, 1980).
Pragmatists hold the view that a balance between protection and self-determination is essential in
children’s healthcare (Alderson, 1993; Archard, 1993; Harrison et al, 1997; Ross, 1997).
Who knows best is not really the issue. It is more about giving children a voice and paying
attention to the individual needs of each child. Furthermore, the legal and ethical concept of the
best interests of the child has become contentious because some feel it is vague, unknowable,
individualistic and open to abuse (Kopelman, 1997). Lowes (1996) argues that we should ‘ensure
every child is treated as an individual and, while encouraging a child’s right to autonomy, it must
be remembered that what is considered beneficial to one child may be detrimental to another’. It
must also be considered that involvement in consultations or decision-making may allow for the
possibility of empowerment, but is not intrinsically empowering in itself. Furthermore, if children
are involved in a tokenistic way (i.e. consultation done inappropriately), then this may
‘disempower’ children and result in them feeling undervalued.
Providing children with opportunities to participate in consultations and decision-making is of the
utmost importance. The benefits for children include better provision of information; having an
opportunity to express feelings; developing confidence; developing competence and maturity;
increased compliance and take-up of service; developing civic skills and encouraging participation
in wider society (Cohen and Emmanuel, 1998; Sloper and Lightfoot, 2003). Young people of all
ages are capable of formulating and expressing very clear ideas about the way in which local
services are planned and delivered (Elliott and Watson, 1997) and allowing children to make
decisions about simple matters facilitates the development of skills that they will need later in
relation to making complex decisions. Excluding children from participating in decision-making can
increase their fear, especially in difficult situations (Runeson et al, 2002b).
Communication has a huge role to play in giving children a voice within the healthcare setting.
Lack of information exchanged between parents and children, children and healthcare
professionals, and parents and healthcare professionals can only lead to confusion and fear. Perrin
and Perrin (1983), in their study of physicians’ perceptions of child understanding, found that the
majority of physicians did not know what to expect children to understand and treated all children
as if they understood at a cognitive level of middle childhood. Effective communication with
parents and the child could have helped alleviate this. The extent to which a child wanted to
exercise their right to autonomy would benefit greatly from honest, open and effective
communication from all parties involved. Chambers (1992) argues that children’s voices could be
empowered if nurses were to engage in advocacy on behalf of the child, whereby if they saw the
child was in distress or needed clarification, they would intervene on the child’s behalf with
parents and physicians. Nurses are in a prime position to act as advocates since they are in close
contact with the child, parents and physicians.
In conclusion, what is needed is a change in the public perception of the child as incompetent
when it comes to having a role in consultation, especially in the area of healthcare. Children are
not belongings or property that we can treat how we like, but human beings with a voice and
developing autonomy. It is up to us as older, wiser and powerful adults to respect children’s views
and to help give them their voice. Attention needs to be paid to what each individual child’s voice
is saying. Participation through consultation allows children’s voices to be heard without handing
them the responsibility to make decisions, which some of them may not be ready to make.
Children, parents, nurses and doctors need to come together to determine the individual child’s
competence, but above all to listen to their views and decide how to respect those views.
17
18
3 RESEARCH
DESIGN
19
Aims and objectives
As shown by the literature review in Chapter 2, there is a dearth of Irish research that has sought
hospitalised children’s views on their involvement in consultation and decisions about their care.
Beresford (1997) suggests that methodological and ethical arguments, together with simple failure
to accept the value of the child’s viewpoint, have precluded children in general from being accepted
as bona fide participants in research. Children may be viewed as potentially vulnerable and therefore
more at risk of having their views ignored or discredited. Having an impairment or illness
marginalises some children even further. Therefore sick children are potentially ‘doubly disadvantaged’
when it comes to being involved in research. However, the focus on children and their lives has
eventually filtered into the research arena and researchers are increasingly recognising the
importance of directly recording children’s own perspectives (Coyne 1998; Cree et al, 2002).
Eliciting children’s views and documenting their experiences are essential in providing services
that are responsive to their needs since they are the main focus of care while hospitalised. It is
imperative that children’s voices are heard and valued in decisions that affect their health and
welfare, and in identifying strategies that will enhance the hospital experience for them. Specific
data about children’s lives are critical for needs and resource assessment, planning, monitoring
and outcomes evaluation. Health-related research is immeasurably important when it comes to
improving the health and well-being of children, both individually and collectively (Miller and
Kenny, 2002). Therefore efforts directed towards those endeavours will ultimately enhance the
quality of care provided for the child and consequently for the family.
The overall aim of the present research is to examine children’s experiences of participation in
consultations and decisions about their healthcare within the hospital setting. The specific
objectives include:
• to describe children’s experiences of consultation in the healthcare setting;
• to identify the factors that enhance children’s participation in consultation and
decision-making;
• to identify the factors that hinder children from participation in consultation and
decision-making;
• to explore strategies that will empower children to participate in consultation and
healthcare decisions.
Methodology
This study adopted a qualitative approach since many researchers have noted that qualitative
methods are appropriate for collecting valid and meaningful accounts from children (e.g. Hill et al,
1996; Ireland and Holloway, 1996; Mahon et al, 1996; Morrow, 1996; Coyne, 1998; Thomas and
O’Kane, 1999; Murray and Hallett, 2000; Balen et al, 2001; Noyes, 2000; Carter, 2002).
To be effective in research with children, researchers need to be flexible in the methodology they
use in order to ensure that children’s rights and needs are respected in the research process. The
research methods should be flexible enough to take account of differences in age, individual
personalities, context and preferred form of communication. It was also considered important to
adopt an approach that would allow children to have a voice. Mayall (1996a) argues that children
may be unnecessarily silenced through a neglect of research and adults to listen to their views and
see the world through their eyes. Lansdown (1995) also argues that we have a culture of not
listening to children and thus have made few attempts to understand children’s lives in their own
terms. It is frequently noted in research studies how the child is seen more as a research ‘object’
than a research ‘subject’ (Mayall, 1996b; Alderson, 1995; Ward, 1997; Hill et al, 1996). Many studies
on children have relied on adults’ perspectives of what children think, leading some researchers to
lament that research has been ‘on’ children, only rarely ‘with’ them or ‘for’ them (Hood et al, 1996;
Faux et al, 1988). Hood et al (1996) argue that research ‘assumes adult knowledge and values as the
standard against which children’s performance is to be measured’. But children are not adults —
they do not hold adult knowledge or see their social world through adult eyes.
20
As a means of gaining an insight into children’s understanding and perceptions of their role in
consultation and decision-making in a healthcare setting, qualitative methods were most
appropriate for this study. To hear children’s voices, a method is required that values subjectivity,
enhances empowerment and allows us to enter the respondents’ world of meaning and belief.
Greene and Hogan (2005) argue that qualitative research allows us access to children and the
world around them through their own self-expression. Qualitative research also corresponds well
with young children since it allows them freedom of expression to tell what is meaningful to them.
Bricher (1999) notes that children’s voices are different to parents’ voices and are based on a
different kind of knowledge. The strength of qualitative methods, such as focus groups or
interviews, is that they usually leave the children’s perspective intact, as well as generating rich,
detailed and valid data (Bender and Eubank, 1994).
Sensitivity is another feature of qualitative methods. Where young children and adolescents are
concerned, sensitivity and a non-threatening atmosphere are of extreme importance. In this study,
we were not only dealing with young children, but also sick children. Amato and Ochiltree (1987)
note that when interviewing children, researchers need to be aware of any anxieties the child may
have or distress he or she may be in. Quantitative methods allow no such room to monitor the
child’s stress or anxiety levels. Hern et al (1998) argue that disclosure of sensitive information is
also a feature of researching children and when disclosed, such information should be handled in a
respectful manner that enhances trust. It is easier to create a relaxed and trusting atmosphere
when a researcher is present to calm any anxieties the respondent may have. This is best done
through qualitative methodology.
Looking at our research questions, the age of our respondents, the effort to hear previously
silenced voices and the sensitive nature of researching sick children, qualitative methods offer the
best means of ensuring rich, valid and, most importantly, meaningful data on the part children play
in decision-making. To be effective in research with children, researchers need to be flexible in
their research tools in order to ensure that children’s rights and needs are respected in the process.
21
Faux, 1991). Alderson (1993) interviewed 120 hospitalised children aged 8-15 years about their
views and experiences of children’s consent to treatment and she found that in every age group the
children were very keen to talk to her in detail. Individual interviews can be useful for discussing
personal experiences with children since they allow more detailed and confidential discussions to
take place than in group situations. Therefore, individual interviews were conducted with children
who preferred to be interviewed privately and who were able to handle the close nature of a one-
to-one interview.
Topic guide
An interview schedule was used to facilitate in-depth exploration of the perspectives of children
and to create discussion on children’s experiences of involvement and consultation during the
focus groups. The key topics were:
• experience of attending hospital;
• experiences of consultation;
• involvement in decision-making;
• relationship with health professionals;
• factors that enhance and hinder children from participation.
It is important to note that this topic guide only acted as a prompt as children were sharing their
individual experiences of hospital. It was intended that the interviews should remain relatively
unstructured so that the children would have the opportunity to identify issues that emerged, in
particular to their experience of hospital consultation. The approach taken was to enter the
interview with a list of broad topic areas and a set of sub-topics to be used as a guide to the
discussion. We did not seek to cover the list of topics on our interview agenda in the same order
with each participant. Instead, we adopted the approach of allowing the interview with each
participant to follow a course, which was, within the broad confines of the interview agenda,
largely determined by age and hospital experience of the participant. It is important that children’s
views are elicited in an open forum and not strictly controlled by a structured interview schedule.
Children need to be allowed to talk about their daily lives so that rapport and trust can be
established. Hence, Faux et al (1988) suggest that when children are ready to tell their story, the
researcher will get credible data, i.e. what is meaningful to the children.
Setting
In a qualitative study of this nature, there is a need to use methods, sites and respondents that
allow for the full range and variation of behaviour of the phenomena under study to emerge
(Chenitz and Swanson, 1986). Therefore, the samples in this study were drawn from three different
care settings — two children’s hospitals and one large district hospital.
Sample
The first selection criterion was that children were hospitalised with an acute or chronic illness.
The reason for including both was to gain experiences of a broad range of children who require
consultation in the healthcare setting.
The second selection criterion was that children should be aged between 7 and 18 years at the
time of interview and that there should be a balance between males and females. The reason for
selecting this age range was that children within the age range of 7-18 would have increased
cognitive, linguistic, social and emotional maturity and be able to handle an interview situation.
Also, there is a broader range of data collection techniques available to the researcher to tease out
the experiences of children within healthcare in this age group.
The third selection criterion was that the children should be divided equally between three
different hospital settings. The reason for selecting different settings is that the experiences of the
children in a specialised setting may be different from those in a generalist setting because of
different philosophies of care and different organisational constraints. Selection of three different
sites would increase the promise of quality, depth and richness in the research findings.
22
The fourth selection criterion was that children/adolescents would be interviewed without parents
or health providers present within the hospital setting in order to create the conditions of privacy
and confidentiality necessary for participation.
Since children might feel more secure being interviewed with others from their own peer group,
children were grouped within different age bands for the focus groups — 7-9, 10-13 and 14-18
years. This ensured that younger, less confident children would not feel intimidated by older
children and that more vocal adolescents did not overwhelm shy or anxious children. Those
children who were reluctant or unable to participate in a group session and who expressed a
preference for single interviews were accommodated.
In spite of careful planning, at the time of the research study there were four main reasons why
all 3 sites were not suitable for data gathering in focus groups:
• Firstly, successful infection control measures were being implemented to prevent the spread
of the winter vomiting bug in all 3 sites. Consequently, bringing children together on wards
for focus groups or moving children between wards had to be done with caution to ensure
effective outcome.
• Secondly, there was an increase in the numbers of children being admitted under 7 years of
age and many infants requiring hospital admission with bronchiolitis. This led to fewer children
being hospitalised within our proposed age groups on all 3 sites.
• Thirdly, some children within the relevant age group were unable to participate due to
feeling unwell.
• Fourthly, the children endured different routines on units (e.g. attending school, playroom,
physiotherapy, X-rays, scans, meals, medication, doctors’ rounds and family visits), thus making
gathering of children for focus groups extremely difficult. Different times during the day were
determined as opportunities to approach children and parents about the study, when activities
on the units appeared less hectic (e.g. morning time between 11am-12pm, 2-4pm and 6-7pm).
The researcher also approached children on a Saturday, when units were less ‘busy’ and more
conducive to child participation.
23
3 hospitals and follow-up discussions took place on an informal basis with healthcare providers.
An information package was compiled and distributed to all units where access to children was
permitted. The researcher also spent time trying to identify key gatekeepers on the units in order
to faciliate the data collection process.
Through liaison with the ward staff and parents, the research team ensured that children suffering
from critical illness were not approached since an interview situation could cause them further
distress. Children who were very distressed by the hospitalisation were approached at a later date,
when they were less distressed, to ascertain if they would like to be interviewed. Omitting children
who are distressed at being hospitalised may introduce bias because such children may indeed have
valid accounts to tell. However, the aim was to ensure that children would not be placed under
additional pressure when they were already experiencing difficulty coping with hospitalisation.
The researcher approached the children’s parents in order to seek permission for the child to
participate in the study. Written consent on permission to approach the child to discuss the
research study and to undertake the interview programme with their child was compiled for
parents. Parents who were not present to consent to the study (11 out of 55) were contacted by
phone and the study was discussed in considerable detail so they could make an informed decision
on their child’s participation. No parent refused. Once parental consent was received, the child was
approached by the researcher and asked to participate in the study.
The parents and children were provided with an age-appropriate letter of introduction and an
information leaflet (see Appendices 2 and 3), explaining the study and the general content of the
interview questions. It was anticipated that through the use of age-appropriate language in the
information sheets, the child would fully understand the interviewing process, thus making it less
difficult for the child. Furthermore, to enhance sensitivity to children’s concerns, the researcher
chatted informally with the children before seeking their consent to participate in the study. The
children were approached with due care and consideration, and were given the time to decide
whether or not they wanted to participate in the study. Therefore, interviews may have been
conducted on another day. Once verbal consent was obtained, the children were asked to provide
written consent to be tape-recorded.
The issue of informed consent is of utmost importance in any research, but particularly with
children who are vulnerable and sick (Mastroianni and Kahn, 2002). Considerable time was spent
creating specific information for the child to ensure each one understood the research programme.
Consent forms were compiled for parents to approach the child to participate and to assent to
their child’s participation in the study (see Appendices 4 and 5). Consent forms were also
developed for younger children, as well as for older children (see Appendices 6 and 7). Pictures of
Shrek, Spongeball, Spiderman and Rudolph were used to attract the child’s attention and elicited
lots of discussion around the character, which eased the researcher into the interview schedule.
Each participant was talked through the voluntary nature of participation and that the interview
was not a test; there were no right or wrong answers. The children were informed that they could
withdraw from the study at any time in the research process and that they would not be required
to provide a reason. The confidentiality of the data was also repeatedly emphasised.
Confidentiality of all data is a fundamental part of respecting and protecting the participant in any
study. Identification numbers were substituted for children’s names and their individual identities
were not linked to the information they provided and publicly divulged. The data collected through
the focus groups and interviews were only viewed by members of the research team. The tapes
were stored in a locked cupboard and destroyed on completion of the study. Children have the
same rights and needs as adults for privacy and confidentiality to be maintained. Adolescents are
especially concerned with confidentiality since they are often experimenting with new ideas and
ways of presenting themselves (Faux et al, 1988; Hern et al, 1998). Therefore, the children were
assured that responses would not be shared with parents or with healthcare professionals unless
they desired it. However, in the case of revelation of child protection issues, the researcher would
proceed in line with the Children First: National Guidelines for the Protection and Welfare of Children
(Department of Health and Children, 1999).
24
To ensure the welfare of children at all times during the study, the research team drew up three
protocols that were based on best practice identified in the 2002 policy document by Our Lady’s
Hospital for Sick Children, Crumlin, entitled Guidelines for good practice, and on the 1999 national
guidelines, Children First (Department of Health and Children, 1999). The first protocol was concerned
with steps to be followed should a child show signs of anxiety during the interview process (see
Appendix 8). The second protocol related to steps to be followed should a child show signs of illness
during the interview process (see Appendix 9). The third protocol related to steps to be followed
should a child disclose sensitive information during the interview process (see Appendix 10). If any
child displayed signs of discomfort or distress during an interview, then the option of withdrawing
from the interview would be immediately offered to them. The nurse manager on the ward would also
be informed of the child’s anxiety. Where there might be signs of fatigue, the remaining part of the
data collection would be modified to conclude as rapidly as possible.
All the focus groups and individual interviews were conducted by a Registered Sick Children’s Nurse
(RSCN) who has over 15 years’ experience nursing children and their families. Every effort was
taken to ensure that the research did not interfere with the children’s medical and nursing care.
Interviews were scheduled in collaboration with the children to take place at a time and venue
that suited them. The interviews were not held following an invasive procedure or when it might
be perceived as inconvenient for the child (e.g. during visiting times, experiencing pain or wanting
a rest).
The researcher approached all available children within the age category so that there was no bias
to the selection criteria. In order to ensure that all children were offered access to the study, each
unit was contacted by telephone on a daily basis to identify the number of children that met the
age criteria. The researcher visited the units and prepared the child and family. Prior to the
interview, the researcher spent time with the children to develop rapport and build trust. For
example, time was given by the researcher to get to know things about the child unrelated to the
hospital setting — things like the music they liked, what football team they supported, what pets
if any they had at home, what was their favourite TV programme, what they liked about where they
were growing up, their favourite foods. When the interview was complete, some children requested
the researcher to play ‘Connect 4’ and card games, listen to music or chat.
On completion of each interview, the children were asked if they wanted to write down anything
that was important. Many of the children’s writings confirmed issues raised during the interview
and was a method of validating the data gathered from the interviews. The researcher encouraged
the children to explain their writings as a way of developing rapport and concluding the interview
in a way that left the child comfortable about the interview process. The children’s writings
validated what they wished to say and placed emphasis on the issues pertinent to them. The
writings also validated that the interpretations of what children were saying in their writings were
not just interpreted by the ‘adult’ researcher, but have been interpreted in the children’s final
statements that concluded the interview.
All focus groups and individual interviews were tape-recorded and field notes were used to keep
a systematic record of observations made during each interview. Research has found that children
generally do not find tape-recording to be distracting (Amato and Ochiltree, 1987; Heary and
Hennessy, 2002; Balen et al, 2001). All children were encouraged to participate in testing the
equipment before each interview or focus group. There was little evidence to suggest that any of
the children were unduly affected by the use of a tape-recorder and many contacted friends or
parents afterwards, talking excitedly about having being interviewed and appeared to enjoy the
special attention given to them during the process. The average length of an interview was 40-60
minutes. (One interview exceeded that time because the participant’s hospitalisation was over an
8-week period and he wanted to spend the time discussing issues that were pertinent to his care.)
In general, interviews varied in time depending on the experiences, age and cognitive ability of
the child.
An initial pilot study helped to determine the reliability and validity of the tools and provided an
opportunity for feedback from the children themselves. It also enhanced familiarity with the
25
interview guide, knowing how and to what extent issues needed to be clarified, and understanding
what questions might be eliminated if it became necessary to shorten the discussion. The
children’s transcripts in the pilot phase were incorporated into the main study.
Data analysis
The individual interviews and focus groups were transcribed verbatim as soon as possible on
completion and the data analysed using the data analysis package QSR Nvivo 2 software. The tapes
were listened to and the transcripts read on a number of occasions to gain a sense of the overall
findings. The focus was on the collation of the concepts and categories into core categories. The
data analysis package enabled the coding of the transcripts. It also facilitated the merging of the
codes, which resulted from their constant comparison and the reading and re-reading of the
transcripts throughout the analysis. It also used the respondents’ own words where possible in the
coding process, which is important for the participatory approach.
The next step was to confirm and examine the main categories identified. These were renamed on
several occasions as they gathered more coding, which was a vital step in discovering the central
categories that emerged from the data gathered. The literature was used to further the process of
comparative analysis through seeking similarities and differences to the categories identified from
the data gathered. A thorough read through the transcripts ensured that the analysis and the
coding of themes and sub-themes accurately reflected what participants stated.
With focus groups, the group rather than the individual is the unit of analysis. The data are
analysed for content, whereby the researcher is looking for particular patterns, themes, concerns or
responses that are posed repeatedly by respondents in the focus groups (Bender and Ewbank,
1994). The individual interviews were similarly analysed for content. Then both sets of data were
collated to provide a composite view. The size of the sample (n = 55) was sufficient to permit valid
conclusions to be drawn (Carey, 1994).
The children’s quotations are reproduced throughout this report, with minimal editing to them in
order to retain the flavour of the spoken word.
26
4 FINDINGS
27
Children’s experiences of hospitalisation
The experiences of children in hospital provide background for the discussion on children’s
participation in consultations and decision-making in relation to their healthcare. Most of the
children seemed aware of the need for hospitalisation in order to receive treatment for their
condition or illness. They appeared willing to endure the discomfort of the investigations and
treatments in order to get better and return home to their everyday lives. However, they expressed
a range of fears and anxieties about the hospitalisation and frequently used the term ‘getting used
to’, which indicated significant adaptation on their part. The children used a variety of terms to
describe their fears about the hospitalisation (see Table 2).
Table 2: Children’s fears
Similar fears and concerns were identified by Barnes et al (1990), who conducted a content
analysis of 186 descriptive case studies of hospitalised children that spanned a 20-year period.
‘Fear of the unknown’ was the most frequently mentioned concern of the children, which indicates
the importance of information and preparation. Some children spoke about the importance of
‘feeling safe’ in hospital, which indicates anxieties about the environment. As one boy aged 9
said: ‘I don’t actually mind staying in hospital as long as I know that I’m safe.’
The children also spoke about the importance of their parent or parents, which was usually the
mother staying with them throughout the day and sometimes at night because they disliked being
alone. The children also relied on parents being there to help meet their needs. Research has found
that parents are a familiar presence that provides reassurance and security for children and helps
to normalise the hospital environment (Coyne, 2003). From the children’s perspectives, the
advantages of the hospitalisation included getting better, getting presents, making friends, playing
with toys and attending school. Some children reported bringing in familiar objects from home to
help them cope with the hospitalisation, like their own blanket or pillow, games, books and
photographs of family. As one girl aged 9 said: ‘Well, I’d say just bring stuff with you that you like,
that would calm you down. Like if you have something that you can’t sleep without, bring that with
you. Just have stuff to calm you down and stuff to stop you crying.’
The most common disadvantages of the hospitalisation cited by the children were losing contact
with friends and siblings, missing home comforts and schooling, and losing freedom and privacy.
The hospitalisation also generated many dislikes for the children (see Table 3).
Table 3: Children’s dislikes
28
The predominant dislikes for all the children were being in pain, operations and needles, being
bored, and lack of play facilities. Nearly all the children stated that they disliked the pain
associated with blood tests and needles. They disliked the food because it was fast food, greasy,
lack of choice and was often cold. Many children found the ward environment too noisy (e.g.
phones ringing, nurses talking, babies and children crying), which caused some to experience
difficulty sleeping. Some children resented the lack of space, tiny rooms and consequently spoke
about needing their own room, own space and more privacy. Most of the children spoke about the
lack of play facilities for their age group and being bored. Thus family, friends and peer groups
were important because they helped to keep the children occupied. As one boy aged 12 said: ‘It’s
kinda got worse as you’ve got older like, because the rooms are quite tiny and there is not much you
can do, and like there’s not much in the place to do anyway, like years ago there used to be a pool
room and we used all go down and sit in that, but now there’s nothing.’
Many of the older children reported that the play facilities were aimed at younger age groups and as
such were ‘babyish’. They had many suggestions as to how the facilities could be improved. The
inadequate play facilities would have hindered children’s attempts to keep occupied and therefore
distract themselves from painful treatments. The range of fears expressed by the children, particularly
fear of the unknown, indicates the importance of providing children with enough information so that
their fears can be allayed. However, as discussed later, most of the children experienced difficulty
gaining information and being heard in relation to their need for consultation.
29
The health professionals tended to involve both the parent (which was usually the mother) and the
child when they imparted information. Some children did not expect to be included in the
information-giving process and were quite pleased to be included. As one boy aged 14 said: ‘Yeah,
they did involve me because they talked to me a lot. I just thought I would have been in here and
they would have just explained it to Mam, but like it was a regular thing that they were explaining to
me and bringing me into all the conversations as well.’
Only a few children reported that doctors involved them actively in consultations and these were
generally children aged 13 and upwards. Similarly, research studies have found that children were
more likely to be involved in consultations and decision-making if they were older (Alderson, 1990
and 1993; Deatrick, 1984).
Age level
Although some older children described being involved, only two of them reported situations where
they felt able to query care and procedures. As one boy aged 18 said: ‘I asked her, “What do you
mean, probably be an injection?” and she said, “Oh yeah, you get an injection”. So I said, “I don’t
like injections, I prefer a cannula”, so she said “OK, we will look into that”. So I went in and they
30
scanned my head. After a few minutes he came back and he was about to just inject, so I said, “Hey,
hey, what are you doing … I said no, I prefer a cannula" and he said, “Are you sure?” and I said,
“Yeah, I don’t like injections”.’
Although this older child (aged 18) felt able to be assertive with his health professionals, he had
to keep repeating his requests before he was taken seriously. Such experiences suggest that much
younger children would require considerable courage to assert their right for inclusion and
therefore may experience difficulties in having their voices heard. It indicates that age and
maturity influence some health professionals’ style of communication with children, which has
been reported elsewhere (Runeson et al, 2001).
31
children who were unable to establish relationships with health professionals for whatever reasons
(e.g. lack of time and health professionals’ attitudes) could be inhibited from active participation.
The following comment from a girl aged 9 illustrates this: ‘I’d probably be a little shy. I don’t know
him very well. If he was a friend and if I was to see him every day, I wouldn’t be shy … I sort of
leave it to my parents.’
As can be seen from the above, a trusting relationship strongly influenced the children’s ability to
participate actively in the consultation process. This implies that children with short-term acute
illnesses may be more disadvantaged in the communication process because of lack of familiarity
with doctors.
Time
Having sufficient time with health professionals clearly enhanced the children’s ability to
participate because they had time to develop a relationship in which they felt comfortable asking
questions. It also allowed time for discussion and time for clarification of information. Children
need time to absorb information. But several children reported difficulty getting to know the
health professionals (doctors and nurses) due to a lack of time, lack of frequent contact, many
patients, busy workload and lack of continuity due to shift work. Consequently, some spoke about
the importance of having the same nurses, so that they could get to know them over time. As one
girl aged 13 said: ‘I think it would be nice if you had the same nurse, like when that nurse is on
because then you’d get to know one nurse really well. I’ve had a few different nurses, but I have got
the same nurses as well and they’re on a few times. I think it would be nice if you had the same
nurse all the time.’
32
Most children were content to share the consultation with their parents so that parents could then
help explain and clarify information received from the health professionals. Some children preferred
to hear the information first-hand from their parents because they were better at explaining the
information than the health professionals. Some children also wanted to share the consultation so
that their parents could ask their questions, which were different from the children’s questions. The
children had observed that their parents tended to ask more future-orientated questions of the
health professionals, in contrast to their own questions which would be more focused on the
immediate situation.
These points are illustrated by the following comments. One boy aged 11 said, ‘I’d rather me Mam
and Dad because you’d understand them more than the doctors, because the doctors leave you
without an answer.’ Another boy aged 12 said: ‘Yeah, I think like if the doctors and nurses were to
speak to us all together and then if you didn’t understand something, your parents could like break
the words down in an easier explanation when they’ve left.’
Many children needed their parents to explain and interpret for them because they had difficulty
understanding the medical terminology used by health professionals. Over half the children spoke
about being reluctant to ask questions of health professionals because of difficulty understanding the
terminology. The use of unusual or ‘big’ words precluded the children from contributing to the
dialogue taking place during the consultation process, which has been reported before (Beresford and
Sloper, 2003). As one boy aged 10 said: ‘I let my mummy ask the questions and she tells me. I prefer
them to talk to my mummy and my mummy can tell me. Sometimes I don’t understand what they are
talking about; they use big words so I don’t ask because I don’t remember the words sometimes.’
There is a danger that some children may become accustomed to parents acting as their advocates
in the communication process and thus fail to develop confidence in their own abilities to
communicate. Continued dependence on parents as communication brokers may mean that these
children lack an opportunity to develop participation skills. This is clearly illustrated by the
following comment from a girl aged 18, who would be seen as capable of independent thought:
‘I just feel stupid sometimes asking all these questions, but like now I realise that it’s not, I’m
asking the doctor more questions myself than having my Dad do it. I just always had my Dad do it.
I was always shy.’
It is evident from the above discussion that many children preferred their parents to be their
advocates and/or interpreters in the communication process mainly due to difficulty in
understanding the style of communication used by health professionals. This indicates that
children may be forced to rely on their parents performing such roles out of necessity rather than
out of choice.
33
Other children recounted occasions when they asked parents for information and it was withheld
from them for whatever reason, which caused them to worry. The children spoke about being
worried when parents and health professionals were talking for a long time and yet they only
received minimal information afterwards. They expressed unease at the perceived disparity between
how much information they had been given by parents and what their parents had been told. This
is clearly illustrated in the following comment from a boy aged 11: ‘They just tell you. They’d use
big words and I wouldn’t be able to understand them and then I’d ask my father what did they mean
and he wouldn’t really tell me. My father wouldn’t tell me really half of it. I think it might be bad
news then or something like that because if the doctor is talking for nearly 15 minutes or so and
your father only tells you a couple of seconds, then there has to be more in the story. It makes you
kinda worried.’
The children’s description of the communication process revealed that they subjected the parents-
professional interactions to considerable scrutiny and were highly conscious of any disparities in
the amount of information they received from parents. Clearly, parents frequently played a key role
in the communication process, which facilitated children’s need for information and allowed them
to participate in some consultations. At the same time, the role of parents could be problematic in
that they could obstruct children’s attempts to participate in the consultation process and limit
their access to information. Similarly, in the study by Young et al (2003), the children (n = 13)
identified elements of their parents’ role that both facilitated and constrained their communication
with health professionals.
34
two specific questions to the child and thereafter conduct most of the consultation with the
parents. The following comments illustrate this. One girl aged 9 said: ‘Yeah, they talk to Mummy a
lot. They like ask me my age, but they never really talk to me, they never tell me like, as I said, most
of them don’t tell me what they are going to do and how long it’s going to take and when I’m going
to see them again.’ Similarly, a boy aged 11 said: ‘Well, they talked more to my parents because
they’d just come into the room and say “Hello, David [pseudonym], how are you?” and then they
just start talking to my father and then just say goodbye.’
When health professionals directed conversations to parents only, children felt unimportant and
uncertain, therefore marginalised. It made some children feel that their parents were having the
treatment rather than them. Being excluded caused upset and worry because they lacked
information on what was happening to them. As one girl aged 9 said: ‘It felt like it was her [child’s
mother] having the operation and not me. It’s not her that’s having the operation and I’d like to
know a little bit more about it.’
Some children resorted to trying to overhear what the health professionals were saying to their
parents. Other children described situations where doctors took their parents elsewhere to talk or
they actually asked the child to leave, thus totally excluding the child from the communication
process. Many children spoke about asking parents to relay the information that they wanted to
hear first-hand. As one boy aged 11 said: ‘I’d like him [the doctor] to come in and talk to me and
tell me everything that’s going to happen because sometimes he tells me to go out of the room or
something when he’s talking.’ Again, a girl aged 17 said: ‘They just tell you, they don’t explain
everything. The first time I was getting PFT, I didn’t know what it was. I hadn’t got a clue what it
was. He didn’t say it when I was there. He said it to Mam behind the curtains. I was upset because I
don’t know what it’s for or anything like that.’
When health professionals chose to talk to parents alone, this caused the children to assume that
the information must be very serious and that something ‘bad’ was going to happen to them. This
had the effect of making them feel scared and apprehensive. As one girl aged 13 said: ‘He kept
having to talk to Mam on her own and I was getting really worried like because if they want to speak
to Mam about something, either it’s because they need to do something really bad or something’s
bad happening. I need to know because otherwise I’m going to be sitting there panicking, going all
paranoid thinking about the worst scenario and everything.’
35
Difficulty obtaining information
All the children spoke about needing information so that they would know what to expect while in
hospital. They spoke about needing information on such issues as their illness, procedures, possible
risks associated with procedures, outcome of procedures, medications, basic needs and length of
stay. The children, however, reported a general lack of information on many aspects of their
treatment and hospitalisation. One boy aged 9 said, ‘They didn’t really explain much’, while another
boy aged 12 said, ‘This morning they were talking to my parents and then he [the doctor] was
talking to the nurse, and the nurse was writing it down and I was kinda getting lost like, but nobody
would explain it to me. I had to ask myself. He wouldn’t explain unless you asked.’
One child aged 17 recalled a situation where he asked the doctor to repeat his explanation and the
doctor subsequently apologised. This child may have been capable of assertiveness, but such
action would have posed considerable challenges for shy and reserved children. Some children
spoke about feeling forced to ask questions and demanding explanations because of being excluded
from the communication process. Even then, certain doctors could ignore their queries. As one girl
aged 9 said: ‘One time I was going to the doctor and I wanted to ask him something about my ear.
He was talking to my mammy and then when he was finished I asked a question again when we were
leaving and he never answered me. It made me feel that you don’t want me to be asking that
question, you don’t want me to know what that is.’
Although the children had unmet informational needs, many of them (35 out of 55) spoke about
being afraid to ask questions of health professionals (mainly doctors) for fear of inconveniencing
or annoying them because they were busy, important, intimidating people. Asking questions could
be seen as wasting their time, therefore children were constrained from seeking information. As
one boy aged 13 said, ‘I didn’t know as much because I was afraid to ask questions. I’d say he’d just
probably be too busy and I might distract him’, while another boy aged 9 put it more succinctly —
‘I’d get into big trouble.’
Causing annoyance to health professionals could get children ‘into trouble’, the implication being
that they could be punished for improper behaviour. Children, especially those in the younger age
group, ascribed very high status to their doctors and this was reflected in a sense of their own
inferiority. This was an issue particularly for those children who were shy or lacking in confidence,
as the following comments illustrate. One girl aged 7 said, ‘I was a bit scared to tell them. I think
they’d start to laugh at me’. Another girl aged 15 said, ‘Yeah, and sometimes I don’t understand
what it means and I just leave it. I’d be afraid to ask what it meant. I’d just feel stupid’.
Even older children were afraid to ask questions because they found the doctor could be
intimidating or react angrily to repeated questioning. One girl aged 17 said, ‘I wouldn’t have the
guts to say. I don’t know, he comes across as a very intimidating man’. One boy aged 13 said, ‘If you
were asking loads of questions, they’d get annoyed. “You weren’t listening, you should have listened”.
They’d keep getting annoyed if you keep asking them questions, so then you just stop and stop
asking them questions’.
The attitude of health professionals towards questions from children was observed by the
researcher during data collection. On one occasion, a doctor replied in an angry voice in response
to a child’s question, ‘I don’t have time for this now’. Similarly, parents reported finding health
professionals difficult to approach because they appeared busy and parents feared a negative
response to questions (Skipper and Leonard, 1986). Some children noted that doctors were busy
people and therefore children should wait for an opportunity to talk with them. Yet children
described several situations where they lacked opportunities to ask questions because of a lack of
time with the health professionals. As one boy aged 11 said, ‘Nearly all the time the doctors are in
a rush’. Another boy aged 17 said, ‘The last time Dr. Kerry [pseudonym] came in to me, he stuck his
head in the door. “How are you doing? OK, bye.” That’s it and he walked off and I haven’t seen him
since. That was on Thursday’.
Popping one’s head around a door is non-verbal behaviour that conveys the distinct impression of
being too busy to spend time and represents tokenistic consultation. The following conversation
36
between three boys, aged 11-13, in one focus group illustrates the problems that children faced
with obtaining information and being involved:
‘It’s like don’t interfere when adults are talking.’
‘Yeah, this is adult conversation, don’t get in, don’t get all hurt, and don’t interrupt.’
‘Yeah, I will tell you later.’
‘But then they don’t and then you are afraid to ask again because you think they’re going to say,
they’re going to give out, “You asked that question earlier on”.’
‘It’s like don’t be rude and interrupt us and all that.’
‘When you don’t realise that you are actually rude, you just want to know what’s going on.’
Some children described waiting for the health professional to provide information and learning
that waiting was easier than asking questions because it did not cause annoyance. As one boy
aged 11 said: ‘If you keep being on them and asking all these questions, they are going to get
annoyed and they’ll tell you anything, so you shouldn’t really ask them that much, ask them a few
times, that’s it. Yeah, because they have to do their job and you just have to wait until they come to
you and if you wait they’re much nicer to you.’
This comment demonstrates that children have to assess the appropriate time when to ask
questions of adults in order to achieve a favourable response. This sounds quite a demanding task
for any child, but even more difficult for sick children. In light of the difficulties children
experienced with asking questions, it is not surprising that many children did not bother to ask
questions and consequently described just accepting and going along with whatever was planned
for them. As one boy aged 11 said: ‘I’d just be a bit nervous. You can’t exactly tell a doctor that I
want this or will you do this. I just go along with what they’re doing or whatever.’
In summary, the comments recorded indicate that, across a broad range of age groups, the children
were reluctant to ask questions, even though they wanted to be involved in the consultation
process. Children needed sufficient time with health professionals so that they could ask their
questions, get clarification and have time to digest the information. Difficulty in obtaining
information left many children feeling anxious and unprepared for procedures and treatments. The
fact that children perceived health professionals as busy, important, intimidating people indicates
that they would need considerable courage in order to ask questions of health professionals.
Therefore, shy or reserved children would be very reluctant to demand involvement in the
communication process. Even when children had the courage to ask questions, health professionals
could ignore their queries. The comments about fear of wasting health professionals’ time conveys
the impression that some children may view their needs as unimportant in relation to the needs of
the health professionals. All of these issues clearly illustrate the difficulties children experienced in
obtaining information and clarifying it with the health professionals.
37
Another child reported feeling frustrated because he had informed health professionals of an
allergy to a particular medicine on a number of occasions, but still doctors continued to prescribe
the medication, which implied that they were not listening and respecting his knowledge. One
child made the insightful comment that sick children have difficulty making their voices heard and
health professionals need to be good listeners. Some children spoke about their contributions not
being taken seriously or being paid ‘lip service’, because health professionals viewed them as
cognitively impaired due to age.
As one boy aged 13 said: ‘I’d say I don’t know what you are really talking about there and as soon
as I said the first bit of it, he’d [the doctor] just start talking over me and we were like “Will you
listen to me or something!” They don’t listen to you because you’re too young, because they think
you don’t know what you’re talking about.’
One child described a situation where she had to reply to the same questions posed by a variety of
personnel over a short period of time. This could have the effect of making the child feel that her
account was being questioned or disbelieved. Another child reported feeling very angry and upset
when a doctor made a comment about her complaining unnecessarily, which implied that she was
pretending or ‘wasting’ his time when her symptoms were genuine. Being labelled as ‘difficult’
understandably caused distress. As one girl aged 14 said: ‘One time when I was sick and I was in
one of the rooms and the doctor told my Mam that every morning I’d wake up saying what will I
complain of next. That wasn’t the case.’
Other children recounted situations where their accounts of symptoms were disbelieved, therefore
not taken seriously. Many of the children’s comments about being disbelieved were in relation to
experiencing pain. As one girl aged 13 said: ‘Yeah, you feel like you are ignored sometimes, laughed
at by everyone, like you’re asking the nurse a question and she was there, she was like, “I’m with
someone else”, and you feel like you’re being ignored with your pain and you feel no-one is listening
to you.’
The children’s accounts of being ignored or disbelieved imply a lack of recognition and respect for
their views. The feeling of being judged and disbelieved appeared to cause stress and potentially
undermined the children’s confidence. Many of the children talked about suffering pain at some
point in their hospitalisation, which caused upset, distress, suffering and restricted movement.
Some children’s pain experience was not helped because of the difficulty they experienced in
receiving attention from nurses and doctors for their pain. Byrne et al (2001) found that nurses
tried to prevent children from displaying pain behaviour and when it did occur, they construed
pain as unreal, unwarranted or not deserving of help. Children complained about having to wait for
medication for pain relief and having to ‘shout’ for painkillers.
One boy aged 18 recalled: ‘Yes, like tonight I was worried. I can’t move and can’t get up and open
the door, like what if I was so much in pain I can’t handle it, what is going to happen next. I go
stiff, I can’t move, I can’t get off the bed, and if there is no-one in there, then I won’t get any help
for a while. I think those buzzers are really important.’
There was a request by many children to have buzzers installed so that they could easily obtain
attention from nurses and consequently feel more ‘safe’. Several children spoke about the difficulty
in contacting nurses during the night and of feeling scared that they might deteriorate and nobody
would notice. Some children spoke about asking for help and their requests going unheard, which
caused frustration and annoyance. Other children reported that nurses spent too much time
chatting to other nurses and ignoring the children. Other children, however, excused the nurses’
behaviour because they felt they were very busy people with many patients and were therefore
unable to respond to individual children’s requests for help. As one boy aged 7 said: ‘Because
sometimes they have work to do and sometimes they have business with someone else and you don’t
want to disturb them. Probably I’d just wait or do it yourself.’
Other children reported instances where nurses agreed to help, but then appeared to forget their
requests. This indicates that children who are shy and reserved would need considerable confidence
to call a nurse for help.
38
Enduring procedures without adequate preparation
The children had a clear need for information, but unfortunately many reported that health
professionals tended to ‘do things’ to them with very brief, or no, explanations. One boy aged 11
described the following: ‘I was a bit nervous because I didn’t know what was going to happen to me or
anything. Well, I just thought they were talking about what they were going to do, but I didn’t really
know what they were talking about exactly. Sometimes when you go down, they have this tray of knives
and everything and you think, “Oh my god, what are they going to do, are they going to cut me up or
what?” So if the doctor doesn’t describe it to you, you don’t really know what’s going to happen.’
Some children spoke about trying to gain information themselves, but then being rushed by health
professionals to undergo a particular procedure. They needed adequate time to digest the
information and prepare themselves. As one girl aged 9 said: ‘Let the children know what they
[health professionals] are doing and what they [the children] are going in for, and not to know at
the last minute, so they can think it over what they are having done to themselves. You can’t even
think in an operation, you’re having anaesthetic to put you to sleep, you’re sleeping, you’re not
thinking, you don’t know, you don’t think it over.’ Again, a boy aged 9 said: ‘I got this book about
what it feels like, but when I started to read it they came in and they started to put it down
[nasogastric tube] so I didn’t get any time to read what it would be like.’
At the same time, many children spoke about the lack of choice with regard to procedures or
medications. Only a few children (6 out of 55) described being given choices with regard to
medications, blood pressure monitoring and method of anaesthesia. The children were aware that
there was a ward routine (e.g. sleeping, waking, eating schedule) and they had to adhere to it.
As one girl aged 11 said: ‘Well, they should tell you do you think it would be better if we do this or
that or something like that, instead of just saying that we’re going to do this the next time and they
don’t tell you that they could do another thing.’
Some children described having to endure blood tests and cannulations without the necessary
preparatory cream (called EMLA and containing a local anaesthetic) because the health professional
chose not to use it. The following conversation between a boy and girl, both aged 17, describes
such a situation:
Girl: ‘Some of them would come around and wouldn’t put cream on and …’
Boy: ‘Stab you in the hand with the needle.’
Girl: ‘Stab you and you’d be sore and be bruised. I’ll never get used to blood tests, I hate them,
yeah, I hate them.’
Boy: ‘Actually getting bloods done hurts more than getting a cannula in.’
Girl: ‘It’s just because you’re poking around for the blood.’
The children described their physiological feelings of pain during blood tests and cannulation as
poking, stabbing and stinging, and their psychological feelings as worry, fear, anxiousness,
nervousness and feeling rotten. Although many children were aware of the benefits of EMLA cream,
some actually also stated that they would not be able to ask for the spray or cream, even though
it makes the procedure less painful. One boy aged 13 said: ‘Well, I was shocked because I thought
they would have told me, and like I’m real bad for getting cannulas in, so I always ask them for the
cream, you know the cream. Yeah, but they never gave it to me, they never gave it to me then. I
didn’t want to bother the nurse.’
As demonstrated earlier, many children lacked information and were unprepared for procedures
because of a lack of time with health professionals (to ask questions, clarify things, etc). The time
factor is often used as an excuse for not preparing children adequately for procedures. Taking the
time to provide children with sufficient information to feel prepared will take time initially, but
will save time in the long term because children will be more cooperative. Previous studies show
that children need preparation (Visintainer and Wolfer, 1975; Alderson, 1990). The findings from
this study show that the children appreciated staff who were kind and who took the time to
explain procedures to them. Enduring procedures without adequate information and preparation is
a violation of children’s right to be prepared appropriately and adequately. It could also be seen as
abusive practice because health professionals are aware of the distress that blood tests cause to
39
children without adequate preparation. Rushing children to undergo procedures indicates a lack of
consideration of children’s needs. The need of health professionals to ‘get the job done’ appears to
have priority over children’s need for adequate preparation. Although children were aware of what
they needed, they found it difficult to be heard by health professionals, with the result that they
just did not ‘bother’. This implies that, with repeated experiences such as these, children may
become socialised into passive obedience, which is antithetical to exerting their autonomy.
The data indicated that parents appeared to play a key role in the consultation process and could
either facilitate or constrain children’s participation. The other key factor that hampered children’s
participation in consultations and decisions were health professionals’ attitudes and behaviour.
Children’s views and opinions were not given the due respect they deserved. Some health
professionals appeared to lack communication skills and did not adapt explanations for children’s
understanding. Most of the discussions/consultations appear to have occurred between parents and
health professionals, which excluded the children from participation. The children did not report
instances where parents demanded of health professionals that the child be included in the
discussions, but that is not to say that this did not occur. The children did recount instances
where parents constrained them from participation, which indicates that parents may have an
influence on health professionals’ behaviour.
Notwithstanding this point, what was clear was that health professionals’ behaviour did not
facilitate or encourage children’s participation in consultations. Furthermore, there were many
examples where health professionals actively obstructed children’s participation and appeared to
hold negative views on their competence to participate. Children appeared to occupy a marginal
subordinate position relative to health professionals’ powerful status in the interaction process.
40
involved in making ‘small’ decisions, such as decisions related to everyday events, diet and
medications, but that they would leave the ‘serious’ decisions (e.g. operations) to their parents and
health professionals. As one girl aged 13 said: ‘Like, small, if they were to ask you “Do you want
tablets or medicine?”, yeah, of course, you can make those decisions. It’s your body, you should
decide what you want to do.’ Similarly, another girl aged 10 said: ‘Small and big [decisions]. If you
wanted medicine, like if you’re only having a little operation and a cut gets infected, that would only
be little … and big would be like an operation that was life-threatening.’
In relation to the decision about hospitalisation, the children generally described reporting being
unwell to their mother, their mother then phoned the relevant doctor and consequently the child
was admitted to hospital. Most of the children said that the doctors made the decision of
hospitalisation and that they went along with that decision because they wanted to get better.
Only one child, aged 13, reported that her parent tried to involve her in the decision to be
admitted to hospital. She said: ‘Well, my Mum said is it really painful and I was just like, I was in
loads of pain so I was like yeah, it’s really painful. She goes do you want to, we can go to the
hospital now and it might be something so simple that they won’t have to even look at you without
knowing, so we can just go in and just make sure you’re OK and I was like yeah, because I didn’t
want the pain to go on because it was actually so painful. I couldn’t explain it, I thought I was
dying, so she did consult me and I did agree with it because I didn’t want to go on with the pain.’
41
exclude children from participating in decisions on matters of healthcare that concern them, but
that parents and health professionals should be involved in the decision-making process also.
Children need support and guidance, and should not be left to take decisions alone since this could
be an onerous responsibility.
42
A boy aged 18 commented: ‘Yeah, if you see the reaction in the way the person is, if they’re mature
enough to understand what’s going on and they’re accepting, and they know what’s going on and
then their reaction to what’s going on is good and you think this is a person you can talk to and
listen to.’ This indicates that doctors need to have a relationship with children in order to judge
their competence over time. Thus, involvement in decision-making is not a single event, but rather
a process that is dependent on the patient-provider relationship, as noted in a study by the British
Medical Association (2001).
Another child, a girl aged 13, made the point that children need practice and help at being
independent and taking decisions. She was encouraged to make choices and be involved in
decisions by her parents and as a result feels more confident in taking decisions: ‘Well, I definitely
think there’s an age that people [can make decisions] like teenagers … well, it depends on what
type of person they are, if they’re kind of shy and childish, I’m not slanging anybody, a childish kind
of person, they will need help with deciding things because then it’s kind of like independent, and
probably they haven’t had as much time making decisions for themselves. But I’m not one of those
people. I’ve been given decisions all my life and my parents told me that it’s my choice to make
them, but if I needed any help they’d be like there for me, which was kind of nice because I thought
like I was in control of my life, but yet my parents were still beside me, making sure I was OK all the
time. So I definitely think that there definitely should be questions asked to children if like they’re
the type of people who answer their own questions and make their own decisions in life.’
Similarly, Gabe et al (2004) noted that the extent to which children are encouraged by their
parents to take responsibility for decision-making (in the home) would influence the children’s
willingness to take an active part in the consultation and their subsequent compliance with
whatever is decided.
The above discussion indicates that as well as encouragement to participate, children also need
support and guidance from their parents and health professionals. Indeed, some children reported
that with their parents’ help, they could be involved in the consultation process and in the making
of decisions. As one girl aged 9 said: ‘Yeah, but with their parents to help them maybe. Some of
them might not be that intelligent to know what they really should do with their lives, to make that
happen in their lives or not.’
It could be argued that when children are not given the opportunity to participate and not allowed
to practice making decisions, they are prevented from developing competence in their own
abilities. Some of the data supports this notion because one older child, a girl aged 18, who
ordinarily would be seen as capable of independent decision-making, spoke about being reluctant
to make decisions because her father usually made all the decisions and she lacked confidence in
her abilities: ‘I always like the decision to be made by my doctor or my dad. I don’t know, I guess
it’s just I don’t feel confident enough to make my own decisions. I just don’t.’
In Deatrick’s (1984) research with chronically disabled adolescents (n = 24) on decisions about
orthopaedic surgery, the children experienced difficulties with their involvement in the decision-
making process. They reported fears and concerns about the responsibility for decision-making due to
difficulty interpreting the varying opinions of doctors and to difficulty handling parents’ spoken or
unspoken desires. Consequently, Deatrick strongly recommended that children’s participation in the
decision-making process should be supported by information and guidance from parents and health
professionals. Deatrick’s findings correlate with those of the present study, in that children spoke
about the need for support and guidance from parents primarily and then from health professionals.
43
the medicine and everything, like all the scanning and everything, they have already decided what to
do before they come to me and ask me. But like the way, I might be starting another medicine on
Monday, they have already probably decided that I’m going to start another medicine.’
One child, a girl aged 13, felt that doctors generally do not see children as capable of making
decisions and as such do not involve them: ‘Well, I know that a lot of doctors don’t actually listen
to the children because they actually don’t think they’re responsible to make their own decisions.’
Some children felt annoyed that they were excluded from the decision-making process and felt that
they had a valuable contribution to make. As one boy aged 12 said: ‘I think I know my own
asthma. I could say like, I might know if I’m going to get better or anything, so if they [the doctors]
say to you “Oh, you have to go to hospital”, you might think, I mightn’t need to go to hospital, like
this has happened loads of times and I felt fine a few hours later. They’re the ones making the
decisions and they don’t give you a say.’
Others spoke about being asked about hospital services and then no action being taken. As one
girl aged 17 said: ‘They’ve been asked before. One actually came in and said “What would ye like in
your playroom?” about two years ago, and that stuff never showed up. Like we were sat down and
asked what we actually wanted and we said a few jigsaws, puzzles, even a little shelf for drawing
books or whatever, books that are actually readable not like kids books.’
The involvement of children in a decision-making process without follow-up or feedback has been
described as tokenism (Treseder, 1997). The lack of follow-up could discourage children from
participating and offering suggestions for improvement.
44
5 DISCUSSION AND
CONCLUSIONS
45
Children’s experiences of being in hospital
The advantages and disadvantages of hospitalisation cited by the children in this study were quite
consistent with those reported in previous research (Gusella et al, 1998; Sartain et al, 2000; Carney
et al, 2003; Coyne, 2003; Battrick and Glasper, 2004).
The most common advantages cited by the children included getting better, getting presents, making
friends, playing with toys and attending school. The children reported keeping occupied through
various means, such as playing board games, using computer games, watching television and videos,
reading magazines and listening to music. The issue that appeared to be of key significance for all
the children, irrespective of age, was friendships and relationships with peer groups, which has been
reported elsewhere (La Greca et al, 1995; Gusella et al, 1998). Friendships were viewed as a positive
aspect of hospitalisation, which helped pass the time and relieved boredom. The maintenance of
usual activities, such as playing, talking with friends and attending school, could be viewed as
helping to restore a sense of normality to the children’s lives within the hospital setting.
The most common disadvantages of hospitalisation cited by the children were losing contact with
friends and siblings, missing home comforts and schooling, and losing freedom and privacy. The
predominant dislikes for all the children were being in pain, operations and needles, being bored
and lack of play facilities.
Some of the older children and those with chronic illnesses spoke at length about the inadequate
play facilities and had many suggestions on how the facilities could be improved. Inadequate play
facilities have been reported in other studies with children in hospital (Gusella et al, 1998; Kari et
al, 1999; Cross and Gregory, 2002), while inadequate numbers of play specialists have been
identified by the National Children’s Office in its National Play Policy (NCO, 2004). Play serves an
important function for hospitalised children in that it restores a sense of normality, reduces
anxieties, serves as an outlet for tensions and conflicts, facilitates communication and speeds
recovery (Lansdown, 1996; Children in Hospital Ireland, 2000). Therefore, keeping occupied
through play is an important strategy that children use to cope with hospitalisation. Inadequate
play facilities potentially hampers their abilities to deal with the situation.
It was evident from the study that provision for adolescents’ social and play needs was inadequate
across the three hospitals concerned, which indicates the need for better provision of play
facilities and consideration of separate units for adolescents. The National Play Policy specifically
states that all the recommendations in relation to play that are outlined in the report by the
Children in Hospital Ireland (2000) be actively implemented and that the therapeutic value of play
should be recognised and actively promoted by health boards and hospitals (Association for the
Welfare of Children in Hospital, 1993).
Although the children accepted the need for hospitalisation in order to get better, they
nevertheless expressed a range of fears and anxieties and frequently used the term ‘getting used
to', which indicated significant adaptation on their part. Their fears were mainly in relation to
being separated from family, unfamiliar environment and people, and fear of the unknown, all of
which have been reported elsewhere (Barnes et al, 1990; Glazebrook and Sheard, 1994). It is
generally accepted that psychological distress in children is partly due to the unfamiliarity of
routines, procedures and health professionals in a hospital ward, as evidenced by the increased use
of preparatory procedures and pre-hospital admission programmes (Lansdown, 1996). This indicates
the importance of providing children with enough information so their fears may be allayed. Yet,
the findings revealed that many children experienced a lack of information and thus a lack of
preparedness before investigations and treatments took place. Thus children’s fears may have been
exacerbated through a lack of information.
46
nurses who provided information that was generally related to the children’s condition and
procedures. Only a few children (n = 5) reported feeling able to ask questions and that health
professionals were willing to offer explanations and information. Their questions were mainly in
relation to meeting basic needs, e.g. whether procedures would be painful, length of stay and
discharge date. These particular children often used the expression ‘they tell me enough’, but did
not elaborate on the point. This could imply that they wanted minimal information and when it
was provided they were relieved. Alternatively, it could mean that they did not know what
information they should know and therefore were satisfied with a brief explanation.
It appeared that some nurses and doctors made a good attempt to explain procedures and events to
the children by using props or simple words. The children also spoke about receiving information from
their parents. Some children appeared to gain information from other sources, such as reading books,
watching hospital soap operas, pre-admission visits and through repeated hospitalisations. The
explanations, pre-admission visits and prior experience helped to prepare children for what to expect
during the hospitalisation. Some children spoke about being asked questions, which were mainly in
relation to symptoms. They felt it was important to be listened to and their experiences were that
nurses generally listened to them. They appreciated staff who were calm, kind and who took their
time with them, which has been reported in other studies (Hockenberry-Eaton and Minick, 1994).
The children liked receiving information because it helped prepare them for what to expect and
thus ameliorated their worries and provided reassurance. Receiving information and being informed
of the risks involved were reassuring for some children rather than upsetting. These findings are
consistent with other studies, which have shown that when children acquire knowledge about their
condition, treatment, likely pain and prognosis, they are more willing to cooperate and endure
painful treatments more patiently, show less emotional upset and recover better (Visintainer and
Wolfer, 1975; Reissland, 1983; Peterson and Toler, 1986; Alderson, 1990). The overall effect of
being involved in the consultation process was that the children reported feeling valued, involved
and less anxious.
47
‘adult-like’. There is evidence in the literature that younger children who protest by crying are often
ignored and not given credibility by health professionals (Bricher, 2000; Runeson et al, 2001).
Children found it easier to ask questions when the person they were in contact with was more
familiar to them. Knowing the doctors and nurses helped children to feel confident in their good
intentions and thereby provided reassurance. Children who had formed relationships with the
health professionals spoke about feeling able to ask questions and participate in discussions about
their care. This suggests that children need to feel comfortable with health professionals in order
to ask questions, which is not unusual when one considers the powerful position that adults hold
in relation to children’s subordinate status. Health professionals who were approachable and willing
to chat encouraged children’s participation since they felt confident to ask questions and seek
information. However, some children reported difficulty getting to know the doctors due to a lack
of time, lack of frequent contact, many patients and busy workload. Other children spoke about
difficulty knowing nurses because of a lack of time and lack of continuity due to shift work. It is
not surprising then that only older, chronically ill children reported forming trusting relationships
with health professionals.
Adequate contact time with professionals seemed to be a significant factor that influenced the
development of the child-professional relationship. Several studies of nurse-family relationships have
found that the development of trust is a process that takes time to develop, so the concept of time
is a fundamental element in the process of getting to know someone (Thorne, 1993; Robinson,
1996; Bricher, 1999). Similarly, Wilson et al (1998) found that a consistent care-giver was the most
significant factor that influenced the development of a trusting relationship between children and
health professionals. This suggests that the absence of a primary carer on a daily basis may
discourage the children from trying to build relationships, particularly with nurses. In like manner,
infrequent contact and lack of time with doctors could hinder the development of child-doctor
relationships. Furthermore, children may require more time to become open to a relationship with
health professionals because of their age, personality or fears of professionals generally.
48
involved in the consultation process, which has been revealed in other studies (Alderson, 1990 and
1993; Epstein and Wayman, 1998).
Most children were reluctant to ask questions of health professionals because of their attitude and
style of communication. The children listed factors such as not knowing the health professionals;
fear of causing ‘trouble’ by asking questions; lack of time and difficulty contacting health
professionals; being ignored; being disbelieved; and health professionals not listening. These
factors presented serious obstacles to the children’s involvement and consequently may explain
their reliance on their parents for information and to act as their intermediaries. This indicates
that children may be forced to rely on their parents as advocates or intermediaries out of necessity
rather than out of choice. There is a danger that some children with repeated hospitalisations may
become so accustomed and reliant on their parents asking questions that they never get the
opportunity to develop their own skills and confidence to enable them to participate actively.
There was evidence that this had occurred with an older child with a chronic illness, who was
aware that she was now, at the age of 18, only beginning to be involved in discussions and
decisions about her care and treatment.
Although parents appeared to play an important role as advocates, they could also obstruct
children from participating actively in the consultation process, which has been reported before
(Angst and Deatrick, 1996; Horner, 1999; Runeson et al, 2001; Tates et al, 2002; Beresford and
Sloper, 2003; Young et al, 2003). Some children described situations where their parents inhibited
their attempts to participate in the consultation process by answering questions on their behalf,
by telling them to stay quiet, by reprimanding them for interrupting the discussion and by not
supporting their attempts to gain information. There were also occasions where children asked
parents for information and the parents withheld it for whatever reason. Lack of support from
parents in relation to children’s active participation sends a clear message to children that they
should be seen and not heard. Thus, some children’s silence may have reflected their motivation to
conform to socially desired behavioural norms for children, rather than a reluctance to participate.
What this evidence indicates is that, firstly, many parents played a key role in enhancing children’s
opportunities to participate and, secondly, if parents do not support their children’s participation,
then this further limits the children’s opportunities for participation in decision-making.
However, it could be argued that parents may withhold information out of a desire to protect their
child rather than being difficult, which has been reported elsewhere (Young et al, 2003). Parents’
actions may have reflected their need to protect their child’s well-being and thus were well
intentioned. But this does not fully explain why some parents overruled their children or silenced
them in front of the health professionals.
There may be several different reasons why parents do not support their children’s participation.
Parents may themselves feel unequal to the authority of the health professionals, particularly
doctors, and therefore may feel more comfortable adopting a subordinate position. Equally, parents,
because of their culture or social class, may be socialised to a role that values obedience to
professionals. Parents’ beliefs may be heavily influenced by their own upbringing, in that they may
have been discouraged from participation and thus may apply the same parenting style to their own
children. Parents may hold the belief that children should be seen rather than heard, and therefore
will discourage children’s active participation. Alternatively, parents’ emotional involvement may
decrease their ability to support their children in stressful situations. The potentially inhibitory role
played by parents in the child-professional interaction process indicates that this is an important
area for further research and an area in which to explore a change in practice.
49
wanted to be included in the consultation process, but were generally excluded. The data indicated
that consultations were largely carried out between parents and health professionals, which tended
to leave children without a voice in matters that affected them. Many of the children used the
phrase ‘they didn’t tell me anything’ to illustrate their experiences and they strongly resented
being excluded from the communication process.
Children also described situations in which health professionals, usually doctors, would direct one
or two specific questions to them and then conduct most of the consultation with the parents. This
could be seen as a tokenistic consultation, whereby the children are involved minimally at the
start of the consultation. Similarly, seminal work on children’s medical encounters in clinics
(Strong, 1979; Silverman, 1987) and studies on wards reported that all children, even the older
ones, were involved in a tokenistic way and routinely excluded from discussions (Shiminski-Maher,
1993; Van Dulmen, 1998; Tates and Meeuwesen, 2001; Carter, 2002; Beresford and Sloper, 2003).
Some children noted that the doctor’s consultation ranged from ignoring them completely to
discussing decisions with other doctors or their parents in front of them. Similarly, observational
studies of patient-professional interactions have revealed that children are often relegated to a
non-participant status in consultations (Epstein and Wayman, 1998; Pearlman and Abromovitch,
1987; Stewart et al, 1981; Tates and Meeuwesen, 2001). Discussing children’s treatment in their
presence, often with only minimal efforts to involve them, clearly demonstrates who is in control
and whose decisions matter. Talking over child patients is a means by which language may be used
to exert power.
Children felt they had a right to be involved because they were the people with the illness and
therefore should be the focus of the information exchange. It made some children feel that their
parents were having the treatment rather than them. This marginalisation has been reported in
observational studies of doctor-parent-patient interaction (Tates and Meeuwesen, 2001). Some
children resorted to trying to overhear what the health professionals were saying to their parents
and also asking parents to explain. This was not always possible; some children reported being
completely excluded from the consultation because doctors took their parents elsewhere to talk or
they actually asked the child to leave. When health professionals excluded them, this resulted in
the children feeling worried because they lacked information on what was happening to them.
Furthermore, they assumed that the information must be very serious and consequently felt very
scared and apprehensive.
Nearly all the children (n = 52) spoke about wanting information so that they would know what
to expect while in hospital and also to make them feel more involved. Information-seeking was a
predominant coping strategy used by the children during hospitalisation, which is consistent with
other research (Caty et al, 1984; Peterson, 1989). However, many children reported a deficit of
information on many aspects of their treatment and hospitalisation. Children described situations
where they received conflicting information from health professionals regarding admission, length
of stay, discharge plan and playing sports and general activities. Although some children tried to
obtain information by asking questions of health professionals, they reported that their questions
were frequently ignored and not answered. This indicated to some children that health professionals
did not want them to ask questions and or were unwilling to provide information. Such experiences
undoubtedly acted as a strong deterrent to these children contributing to future consultations.
Children felt that they should be allowed the opportunity to hear information directly, ask
questions and volunteer their own information, therefore being actively involved. However, the
findings revealed that a broad range of children (from 7-17 years) were reluctant to ask questions,
even though they wanted to be involved in the consultation process. Consequently, they reported
accepting and going along with whatever was planned for them. Many children (n = 35) spoke
about being afraid to ask questions of health professionals due to fear of inconveniencing or
annoying them because they were viewed as such busy and important adults. Children needed
sufficient time with health professionals in order to ask their questions and have time to digest
the information. However, many children felt unable to ask questions because of ‘being rushed’
50
during their interactions with health professionals. Even older children experienced difficulty asking
questions. Some reported being afraid to ask questions because they found the doctor could be
intimidating or react angrily to repeated questioning. Consequently, they described learning
through experience — that waiting for the health professional to provide information was easier
than asking questions because it did not cause annoyance and the health professional was then
much ‘nicer’ to them. The children also described having to assess the appropriate time when to
ask questions of adults in order to achieve a favourable response. Gauging the right time to ask
questions of adults seems quite a demanding burden on sick children and should not be expected
of them, but unfortunately it seemed to be the reality for some.
Thus, the attitudes and behaviour of health professionals were significant factors that influenced
children’s ability and willingness to participate in the consultation process. Similarly, Lewis and
Lewis (1990) found that the principal obstacle to children’s participation came from doctors
because their power status was threatened, they lacked the skills to deal with empowered children
and, moreover, they lacked conviction that it was beneficial for children. The present findings
indicate that many health professionals, particularly doctors, do not actively support or facilitate
children’s participation in consultations.
Children had difficulty being heard. Many children (n = 32) reported expressing their needs to
nurses and doctors, but then of not being listened to and their views not being taken seriously.
Several children spoke about doctors lacking knowledge of their medical history, doctors giving
different opinions or being uncertain, and a lack of correspondence or exchange between medical
teams. They spoke about providing health professionals with information on various issues (such as
allergies, the medicines that worked for them and their preferences with regard to blood tests and
cannulation techniques) and then of their views being disregarded. Some children felt that they
were judged, disbelieved and labelled as ‘difficult’, and this compounded the stresses they were
already dealing with, which has been reported elsewhere (Carter, 2002). Many of the children’s
comments about being disbelieved were in relation to expressions of pain. This finding is
supported by other research studies, which found that nurses often misinterpret children’s
expressions of pain, underestimate their pain and provide inadequate pain relief (Gonzalez et al,
1993; Byrne et al, 2001; Carter, 2002). Some children felt that health professionals did not ‘care
about them’ because their expressions of pain went unheard. Similarly, other studies have found
that children perceive inadequate pain relief as a form of punishment or evidence of an uncaring
attitude from health professionals (Ross and Ross, 1984; Gaffney and Dunne, 1986 and 1987).
The children wanted health professionals to listen and therefore it was frustrating for them when
health professionals did not take account of their views and respect their knowledge. Some children
felt that their contributions were not given due weight and consideration because health
professionals viewed them as cognitively impaired and therefore lacking understanding. The
children’s accounts of being ignored or disbelieved imply a lack of recognition and respect for
children’s views. It could be argued that such experiences would serve as a strong deterrent to
children’s active participation in the communication process.
Some children experienced difficulties contacting nurses for assistance with basic needs,
particularly in relation to pain relief. The children were concerned because they had no means of
contacting the nurses, such as the use of a call system, and consequently if parents were not
around, they would have to rely on someone passing by or shouting in the hope that a nurse might
hear them. The same concerns about contacting nurses was reported over 30 years ago, when
Hawthorn (1974) found that children had no means of attracting a nurse’s attention other than
calling, setting out to look for one or finding someone else to act for them. Some children spoke
about asking for help and their requests going unheard, while other children reported instances
when nurses agreed to help but then appeared to forget their requests. Some children noted that
nurses were very busy with other patients and so were reluctant to ‘bother’ the nurses because this
could cause them to get annoyed. Other children viewed nurses’ actions as being uncaring because
nurses chose to spend time chatting to other nurses and ignore the children. This indicates that
many children experienced difficulties being heard, which is of concern with regard to the quality
of care provided for children in hospital.
51
The lack of information meant that children often had to endure procedures without adequate
preparation. Children generally reported that health professionals tended to ‘do things’ to them
with very brief, or no, explanations. Similarly, Epstein and Wayman (1998) found that the
conversations surrounding hospital procedures tended to be sparse, unresponsive and non-
contingent, which suggested that health professionals treated the children as passive objects
rather than active participants. Again, Knafl et al (1988), in their study of families and children
(n = 62), found that the child was like an object that had things done to it, rather than being
involved or having a voice.
Many children spoke about the lack of choice with regard to procedures and treatments. They noted
that there was a ward routine and that children had to adhere to it. They also described situations
where they were not given time to think about the information and digest its implications. Some
children described being pressurised to undergo procedures with minimal explanations and no
preparation. For example, some children described having to endure blood tests and cannulations
without the necessary preparatory cream (called EMLA and containing a local anaesthetic) because
the health professional chose not to use it. Although many children mentioned the benefits of the
cream, some actually stated that they would not be able to request for it to be used because they
did not want to cause ‘bother’. It appears that children were generally reluctant to challenge health
professionals about inadequate care. Similarly, the study by Runeson et al (2002a) explored
children’s participation in decision-making during hospitalisation (n = 24) and found that children
were forced to undergo procedures with minimal preparation and information.
Consequences of non-consultation
The children used a wide variety of terms to describe their feelings about being excluded from the
consultation process. These included feeling disappointed, sad, confused, angry, ‘rotten’, worried,
misled, shocked, betrayed, lonely and ignored. The children clearly wanted to be involved in the
consultation process and the range and depth of feelings listed here clearly illustrate the impact of
non-involvement on many children. It may be argued that such feelings, allied with an imposed
subordinate role, may adversely affect the physical and emotional welfare of the child. Children’s
perceptions of their personal value are important because research suggests that there is a
relationship between self-esteem, self-worth and medical outcomes (Schmale and Iker, 1971;
Herzlich, 1973; Norbeck and Tilden, 1983; Prilleltensky et al, 2001).
The consequence of non-consultation was that some children felt forgotten and depersonalised, as
if they were inanimate objects with no feelings. The depersonalisation effect of being treated as a
non-person has been described by Goffman (1961) as ‘the wonderful brand of non-person treatment
whereby the patient is greeted with what passes as civility, and said farewell to in the same
fashion, with everything in between going on as if the patient weren’t there as a social person at
all, but only as a possession someone left behind’. Similarly, adult patients who experienced such
situations reported feeling insignificant and objectified, which resulted in feelings of fear, anger
and shame (Drew, 1986). If adults feel objectified, then these feelings could be compounded for
children who are in a more vulnerable position because of their illness, their limitation of
understanding and their lack of control over what is happening to them.
52
hampered children’s participation in consultations and decisions appeared to be the attitudes and
behaviour of the health professionals. These findings are consistent with previous research in this
area. Many other research studies have reported that health professionals’ attitudes play a key role
in whether children are allowed or encouraged to participate (Strong, 1979; Silverman, 1987; Lewis
and Lewis, 1990; Runeson et al, 2001).
The research in this area indicates that doctors do not support children’s participation in
consultations for a variety of reasons. These include lack of time, chaotic environment, loss of
power and control, having their views and approaches questioned, not agreeing with the child’s
wishes, uncertainty about a child’s competence, lack of conviction of the benefit to children and
lack of communication skills with children (Strong, 1979; Van Eys, 1986: Lewis and Lewis, 1990;
Rylance, 1996). According to some paediatricians, there has been a tendency to overlook the rights
of children to express their views because of paediatricians’ protective attitudes and overcautious
assessment of children’s ability to understand and reason in an environment of traditional adult
superiority (Strong, 1979; Van Eys, 1986: Rylance, 1996). Alternatively, doctors could be
threatened by the notion of empowered children who could possibly challenge them and/or the
status quo. American paediatricians Lewis and Lewis (1990) concluded from their research over an
18-year period that the principal resistance to children’s participation in consultations and
decision-making came from the doctors themselves because their power status was threatened,
they lacked the skills to deal with empowered children and they lacked conviction that it was
beneficial for children. Thus, the evidence indicates that negative beliefs on the part of health
professionals about children’s participation may be a central issue.
However, others would argue that the organisational and legal setting needs to be taken into
account, as well as the beliefs and agenda of health professionals (Gabe et al, 2004). Doctors often
experience difficulty reconciling opposing objectives: on the one hand, children are seen as
immature and in need of protection, while on the other hand, they have a right to be heard
(British Medical Association, 2001). Gabe et al (2004) suggest that the poor standard of
accommodation in the public health system (e.g. restricted space on wards), time pressures and
tight time-slots result in hurried consultations. There is a risk that when children’s need for
information and consultation are weighed against saving time or money, the short-term solution
(lack of information) is given preference. Tates et al (2002) suggest that doctors may actually be
orientated towards facilitating children’s participation, but may be constrained by the parents’
presence and demands for inclusion. They argue that previous research on doctor-patient
interactions has failed to consider how parents may influence the behaviour of health
professionals. The communicative needs of children and parents may be quite different and health
professionals may experience difficulties meeting both parties’ requirements in a time-pressured
consultation. This brief discussion indicates that further research is required to address these
important issues more thoroughly.
53
Although some children appeared content to leave the ‘serious’ decisions to health professionals
and parents, other children wanted that responsibility. They spoke about having the right to take
‘serious’ decisions, such as admission to hospital and operations. These children ranged in age from
7 to 18, which indicates that age or cognitive levels may not be the sole influence on decision-
making. Some children felt that they should be involved in the decision-making process when they
were older. When asked what would be an appropriate age, they provided a range of ages, from 9
to 18, with many choosing 16 as the cut-off point. Similarly, in Alderson’s (1993) research with
children (n = 120), the children had views about whether they wished to give consent to treatment
or delegate that responsibility to their parents, and many children could propose an age when they
thought it was appropriate to decide for themselves. Weithorn and Campbell (1982) noted that
children as young as 9 appeared able to participate meaningfully in personal healthcare decision-
making. The range of ages supplied indicates that children do not see decision-making as occurring
at a set age or set point in their lives; rather, it was an evolving process dependent on individual
factors. This suggests that health professionals need to assess each child’s ability and willingness
to participate, rather than making assumptions about involvement based solely on the child’s age.
At the same time, some children noted that it depended on the individual rather than their age.
One child made the point that health professionals need to assess the child’s maturity and ability
to understand before including them in taking serious decisions. Other children felt that it was the
child’s responsibility to inform parents and health professionals when they were ready to take on
the responsibility for decision-making. Angst and Deatrick (1996) found that many parents
(n = 48) were waiting for the health professionals to tell them when it was appropriate to involve
their children in decisions and planning their care. If this is the case, then parents may never
encourage children’s participation unless they receive permission from health professionals. Some
children were aware that they required practice, help and support from parents in order to become
confident in making decisions. It appeared that some children were encouraged by parents to play
a part in decision-making and thus were accustomed to participation. This indicates that children’s
capacity to participate is determined to a degree by the effort of preparing them to participate and
the quality of the preparation. Thus parents play an influential role in children’s ability to
participate, which indicates that an awareness of the family’s usual pattern of decision-making can
be helpful. Parents may need guidance and encouragement from health professionals before they
can enable and support their children’s participation in decision-making.
Some children described situations where they were excluded from the decision-making process,
which is not surprising in light of their experiences of non-consultation discussed above. Some
children felt that health professionals make the decisions and then present the children with a fait
accompli, thereby allowing no opportunity for discussion. Similarly, Hallstrom and Elander (2004)
found that children and parents made few decisions themselves and even if they disagreed with
the decision made, few decisions were reconsidered. One child felt that doctors generally do not
see children as having the competence to make decisions and as such do not elicit their views or
involve them. According to Rylance (1996), a consultant paediatrician, there has been a tendency
to overlook the rights of children to express their views because of paediatricians’ protective
attitudes and overcautious assessment of children’s ability to understand and reason in an
environment of traditional adult superiority. The children were aware that they were excluded from
the decision-making process, which resulted in feelings of annoyance; they felt that they had a
valuable contribution to make and resented being excluded. Preventing children from involvement
restricts their ability to gain competence in decision-making over time. Other children spoke about
being asked about hospital services and then no action being taken. The involvement of children in
a decision-making process without follow-up or feedback has been described as tokenism (Treseder,
1997). The lack of follow-up could discourage children from future participation and offering
suggestions for improvement.
As can be seen from the discussion above, some children wanted to be involved in ‘small’
decisions, while others were willing to be involved in ‘serious’ decisions. It was evident that many
children desired involvement in the decision-making process, particularly in everyday decisions
about their care and treatment. The literature on decision-making is replete with lengthy debates
54
about children’s informed consent, which does not address the issue of participation in simple
everyday decisions. When the issue of decision-making is mentioned, most people tend to think of
court cases that involve decisions about life threatening treatment (rather than simple everyday
decisions). These examples tend to be extreme and unusual, yet they have led to uncertainty and
anxiety about routinely involving children in decision-making.
The findings indicate that the issue of decision-making should be seen as being on a continuum
rather than on an ‘all or nothing’ basis. It should also be viewed as a process that is dependent on
the type of decision, child, parents and health professionals’ opinions, and the situational context.
The findings support the position that the assessment of competence should be based on children’s
functional ability, not on age or outcome of decision (Alderson and Montgomery, 1996; Rylance,
1996). However, this may be problematic since it is easy to assume that children are competent
only if they make the decisions that doctors want them to make (Dixon-Woods et al, 1999). The
children in the present study held clear views on the issue of decision-making and could recount
the factors that would influence their participation in the decision-making process. As seen earlier,
some children wanted to leave decisions to adults or share decisions with them. Unwillingness to
participate should not be interpreted as incompetence since choosing to leave decisions to adults
demonstrates the ability to choose. The children showed quite sophisticated reasoning in their
awareness that due to their cognitive immaturity they often need help with decision-making.
Hence, it may be argued that children of all ages, apart from babies, are capable of making
decisions and as such they have a right to be informed and included in decisions that affect their
welfare. Health professionals, therefore, have a responsibility to justify non-involvement of children
in matters and decisions that affect them.
Frameworks of consultation
Several models, frameworks and schemata have been developed over the years to structure the
involvement of children in consultation and decision-making (Hart, 1992; John, 1996; Alderson
and Montgomery, 1996; Hermerén, 1996; Treseder, 1997; Cohen and Emmanuel, 1998; Lansdown,
2001). Most of these writers have based their frameworks on the model developed by Hart (1992)
called the ‘Ladder of Participation’, which was originally created by Arnstein (1969) as a model of
citizen participation (see Appendix 1a). The ‘rungs’ on Hart’s Ladder number 8 in total and
represent degrees of participation or opportunities that range from tokenism to child-initiated,
shared decisions with adults (see Appendix 1b). Some of the findings from the present study can
be linked to 3 rungs on this ladder — ‘Assigned but informed’, which is participation, ‘Tokenism’
and ‘Manipulation’, both of which are seen as ‘non-participation’.
Hart’s model was developed for children’s participation in projects rather than general situations.
Therefore, it is not surprising that it does not contribute towards a full understanding of the
findings on consultation in this study. Hart’s model has also been criticised for structuring various
levels of consultation in a hierarchy, which does not recognise that participation usually exists on
a continuum, with movement across the levels depending on the context of the consultation
(McAuley and Brattman, 2002) and also that children may have different views on their levels of
participation. The model has also been criticised for assuming that child-initiated and directed
participation is the eventual aim for all those who wish to empower children and that other ‘levels’
of participation are merely steps on the way to that eventual goal (Treseder, 1997).
Treseder (1997) provides an alternative model that displays all levels of participation as equal forms
of good practice in a circular layout (see Appendix 11). Although this model is an improvement on
previous ones, the description of the levels does not easily fit with the context of the healthcare
setting. The level of ‘Child-initiated and directed’ participation will be inappropriate in most
healthcare decisions because adults need to be available and responsible for the children, especially
during illness. The findings from this study are structured in a new framework (adapted from
Treseder’s model) that illustrates children’s experiences rather than the ideal (see Appendix 12).
From the children’s data, it is possible to devise a framework that encompasses the levels of
participation from their perspectives, thus a framework for best practice (see Figure 1). The children
55
described 7 levels of participation in consultations, which were being informed; asking questions;
expressing a view; being listened to; being heard; involved in decision-making; shared decision-
making; and being the main decider.
From the children’s perspectives, all of these levels were important at different stages of their
hospitalisation. Being involved in the communication process was more important for many
children than necessarily being involved in serious decision-making. This was understandable in
light of the difficulties the children experienced in having their views heard and being involved in
discussions about their care in hospital. The levels could also be arranged in a circular diagram to
illustrate that they were not ranked in order of importance.
Figure 1: Levels of participation
Being main
decider
Shared decision-making
Involved in decision-making
Being informed
Asking questions
Expressing a view
Being listened to
Being heard
Conclusions
It was clearly evident that children want to be involved in the consultation process. Most of the
children wanted to be consulted about their care and to be respected as having opinions about their
care and treatment. Many children felt that they had a right to be involved because it was about
matters that affected them and their bodies. They spoke about needing information so that they
could understand their illness, be involved in their care, prepare themselves for procedures and
direct their actions towards ‘getting well’ again. It was clear that some children were consulted and
received information and explanations. When children were provided with information and consulted
about their care, they felt less anxious about undergoing operations and treatment.
It was also evident that some children used an information-limiting strategy to cope, in that they
did not want to hear any ‘bad news’. Many children preferred their parents to be their advocate or
intermediary in the consultation process because of the difficulty understanding medical
terminology and not wanting to know ‘bad things’. Thus for some children, parents play a key role
in protecting them in the consultation process. At the same time, some children reported that
parents constrained their participation in consultations and that they felt excluded by doctor-
parent interactions. Thus parents’ role appeared to both facilitate and constrain communication
interactions for the children.
It may be argued that the children’s dependence on their parents as brokers in the consultation
process arose out of necessity because they encountered considerable obstacles that hindered their
active participation. Most children experienced considerable difficulties in expressing their views,
getting their views taken seriously and obtaining sufficient information to enable them to
participate in discussions concerning their welfare in hospital. Their voices were not heard and
they appeared to occupy a marginal position in consultations. They described feeling upset, angry
56
and depressed when health professionals ignored or excluded them. They expressed a need to be
included in the consultation process, but were hampered mainly by the actions of the health
professionals. By restricting the amount of information given to children, health professionals limit
children’s opportunities to participate and leave them with limited choice in the decision-making
process. The findings suggest that the practice of speaking with children, listening to them and
involving them in the decision-making process is not common practice for many health
professionals. This is a situation that needs to be improved since a lack of involvement may
contribute negatively to children’s self-worth, their recovery from illness and overall adaptation.
By health professionals communicating with and involving children, this emphasises that children
are important, enhances children’s coping skills and contributes to effective adaptation.
Some children wanted to be involved in the decision-making process, while others were happy for
the decisions to be left to parents and health professionals. Participation rights do not necessarily
confer the right to be the main decider, nor do children want to exclude their parents from the
process. The findings indicate that the issue of decision-making should be seen as existing on a
continuum rather than an ‘all or nothing’ approach. The view of ‘all or nothing’ obscures the fact
that there are multiple levels and ways to involve children in decision-making. There are also
different types of decisions ranging from minor to major and the decision-making process can
change depending on the situational context. The findings indicate that decision-making for
children is a complex process that evolves over time and that may be shared or contested with
parents and health professionals depending on the type of decision. It suggests a pragmatic
approach, which recognises that children need protection while at the same time allowing
flexibility for the child’s emerging knowledge and self-determination.
The focus of this study was on children’s experiences of consultation and involvement in decision-
making. Most of the data was in relation to the process of consultation, which indicates that being
listened to, being heard and being given the opportunity to ask questions is what is of most
importance for children. They also want to be involved in the decision-making process, but for
most children the process of consultation is of key importance. Some authors see participation in
consultation as being synonymous with decision-making, but this is not always the case. Giving
children a voice in matters that affect them directly is not the same as giving them the sole
responsibility for decisions and their consequences.
Health professionals, like all adults, may be open to the possibility of consulting children, but they
may also have reservations. They might find certain aspects of consulting children difficult for
reasons that include a lack of time, chaotic environment, loss of power and control, having their
views and approaches questioned, not agreeing with the child’s wishes, not being able to do what
they want to do, and feeling worried about lack of necessary skills and competence. There is a
need for further research to determine the attitudes and feelings of health professionals about
consultation with children; clearly, the findings demonstrate that some must have reservations
about involving children. This information could then be used to identify interventions that will
encourage professionals to re-evaluate their practices and embrace a more flexible approach to
children’s participation in consultations and decisions.
There is also a need for further research into parents’ perceptions of children’s involvement in
consultations and decisions. This is because parents have a significant influence on their children’s
competence to participate. The research on child and parent participation in healthcare decisions
is limited, in that the process of involvement has not been examined. There is a lack of
information on how parents, children and health professionals interact in relation to the decision-
making process. Few studies of doctor-child-parent communication have taken the triadic nature of
this communication into account. There is a lack of method and assessment tools devised for
healthcare settings to assess the competence of children of different intellectual abilities,
including those with intellectual disabilities. Children have the right to participate effectively, but
recognition of the wider benefits of this participation is essential to sustaining this wider process
of cultural and organisational change. There is a need for a programme of research that more
clearly identifies and quantifies the range of personal, social and economic benefits of children
gaining greater influence over decision-making and participation in healthcare.
57
58
BIBLIOGRAPHY
59
Alderson, P. (2000) Young Children’s Rights: Exploring Beliefs, Principles & Practice. London: Jessica
Kingsley Publications.
Alderson, P. (1995) ‘Will you help us with our research?’ Archives of Disease in Childhood, No. 72,
pp. 541-42.
Alderson, P. (1993) Children’s consent to surgery. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Alderson, P. (1990) Choosing for children: Parents’ consent to surgery. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Alderson, P. and Goodey, C. (1996) ‘Research with disabled children: How useful is child-centred
ethics?’ Children & Society, Vol. 10, No. 2, pp. 106-16.
Alderson, P. and Montgomery, J. (1996) Healthcare choices: Making decisions with children.
London: Institute for Public Policy Research.
Amato, P.R. and Ochiltree, G. (1987) ‘Interviewing children about their families: A note on data
quality’, Journal of Marriage and the Family, No. 49, pp. 669-75.
Angst, D.B. and Deatrick, J.A. (1996) ‘Involvement in health care decisions: Parents and children
with chronic illness’, Journal of Family Nursing, Vol. 2, No. 2, pp. 174-94.
Archard, D. (1993) Children’s rights and childhood. London: Routledge.
Aries, P. (1962) Centuries of childhood. London: Jonathan Cape.
Arnstein, S. (1969) ‘Eight rungs on the ladder of citizens’ participation’, Journal of the American
Institute of Planners, No. 35, pp. 216-24.
Aronsson, K. and Rundstrom, B. (1998) ‘Child discourse and parental control in paediatric
consultations’, Text, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 159-89.
Atherton, T. (1994) ‘The Rights of the Child in Healthcare’, in The Child and Family, B. Lindsay
(ed.). London: Balliere Tindall.
Association for the Welfare of Children in Hospital (1993) Play for Child Patients in Irish
Hospitals. Dublin: Association for the Welfare of Children in Hospital.
Balen, R., Holroyd, C., Mountain, G. and Wood, B. (2001) ‘Giving children a voice:
Methodological and practical implications of research involving children’, Paediatric Nursing,
Vol. 12, No. 10, pp. 24-29.
Barnes, C.M., Bandak, A.G. and Beardslee, C.I. (1990) ‘Content analysis of 186 descriptive case
studies of hospitalised children’, Maternal-Child Nursing Journal, No. 19, pp. 281-96.
Battrick, C. and Glasper, A. (2004) ‘The views of children and their families on being in hospital’,
British Journal of Nursing, Vol. 13, No. 6, pp. 328-38.
Beals, D.A. (2003) ‘Bioethics in practice’. Available at: www.emedicine.com
Beauchamp, T. and Childress, J. (2001) Principles of biomedical ethics (6th edition). Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Bender, D.E. and Ewbank, D. (1994) ‘The focus group as a tool for health research: Issues in
design and analysis’, Health Transition Review, Vol. 4, No. 1, pp. 26-76.
Beresford, B. (1997) Personal accounts: Involving disabled children in research. London: HMSO.
Beresford, B.A. and Sloper, P. (2003) ‘Chronically ill adolescents’ experiences of communicating
with doctors: A qualitative study’, Journal of Adolescent Health, Vol. 33, No. 3, pp. 172-79.
Bibace, R. and Walshe, M.E. (1981) ‘Development of children’s concept of illness’, Paediatrics,
No. 66, pp. 912-17.
Bonn, M. (1993) ‘The effects of hospitalization on children: A review’, South African Journal of
Nursing, Vol. 17, No. 2, pp. 20-24.
60
Brannen, J., Dodd, K., Oakley, A. and Storey, P. (1994) Young people, health and family life.
Buckingham: Open University Press.
Brewster, A.B. (1982) ‘Chronically ill hospitalized children’s concept of their illness’, Paediatrics,
Vol. 69, No. 3, pp. 355-62.
Bricher, G. (2000) ‘Children in hospital: Issues of power and vulnerability’, Pediatric Nursing, Vol. 26,
No. 3, pp. 33-39.
Bricher, G. (1999) ‘Paediatric nurses, children and the development of trust’, Journal of Clinical
Nursing, No. 8, pp. 451-58.
British Medical Association (2001) Consent, rights and choices in health care for children and
young people. London: British Medical Association.
Byrne, A., Morton, J. and Salmon, P. (2001) ‘Defending against patients’ pain: A qualitative
analysis of nurses’ responses to children’s post-operative pain’, Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
No. 50, pp. 69-76.
Carey, M.A. (1994) ‘The group effect in focus groups: Planning, implementing and interpreting
focus group research’, in Critical Issues in Qualitative Research Methods, J.M. Morse (ed.). Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Carney, T., Murphy, S., McClure, J., Bishop, E., Kerr, C., Parker, J., Scott, F., Shields, C. and
Wilson, L. (2003) ‘Children’s views of hospitalisation: An exploratory study of data collection’,
Journal of Child Health Care, Vol. 7, No. 1, pp. 27-40.
Carter, B. (2002) ‘Chronic pain in childhood and the medical encounter: Professional ventriloquism
and hidden voices’, Qualitative Health Research, Vol. 12, No. 1, pp. 28-41.
Caty, S., Ellerton, M. and Ritchie, J. (1984) ‘Coping in hospitalised children: An analysis of
published case studies’, Nursing Research, No. 33, pp. 277-82.
Chambers, M. (1992) ‘Who Speaks for the Children?’ Journal of Clinical Nursing, No. 1, pp. 73-76.
Chenitz, W.C. and Swanson, J.M. (eds.) (1986) From practice to grounded theory: Qualitative
research in nursing. Menlo Park, CA: Addison-Wesley Publishing.
Children in Hospital Ireland (2000) Update on play facilities report. Dublin: Children in Hospital
Ireland.
Christensen, P.H. (1998) ‘Difference and similarity: How children’s competence is constituted
in illness and its treatment’, in Children and Social Competence: Arenas of Action, I. Hutchby and
J. Moran-Ellis (eds.). London: Falmer.
Cohen, H. (1980) Equal rights for children. New Jersey: Rowman and Littlefield.
Cohen, J. and Emmanuel, J. (1998) Positive participation: Consulting and involving young people
in health-related work: A planning and training resource. London: Health Education Authority.
CORI (2004) Press Release on Child Poverty. Dublin: Conference of the Religious of Ireland.
Available at: www.cori.ie
Coyne, I.T. (2003) A grounded theory of disrupted lives: Children, parents and nurses in the
children’s ward, unpublished PhD thesis. London: King’s College, University of London.
Coyne, I.T. (1998) ‘Researching children: Some methodological and ethical considerations’, Journal
of Clinical Nursing, No. 7, pp. 409-16.
Cree, V.E., Kay, H. and Tisdall, K. (2002) ‘Research with children: Sharing the dilemmas’, Child
and Family Social Work, No. 7, pp. 47-56.
Crisis Pregnancy Agency (2003) Towards a strategy to address the issue of crisis pregnancy.
Dublin: Crisis Pregnancy Agency.
61
Cross, R. and Gregory, S. (2002) ‘Giving children a voice’, Emergency Nurse, Vol. 10, No. 6,
pp. 11-15.
Deatrick, J. (1984) ‘It’s their decision now: Perspectives of chronically disabled adolescents
concerning surgery’, Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, No. 7, pp. 17-31.
Deatrick, J.A. and Faux, S.A. (1991) ‘Conducting qualitative studies with children and
adolescents’, in Qualitative nursing research: A contemporary dialogue (2nd edition), J.M. Morse
(ed.). London: Sage Publications.
Deatrick, J.A., Dickey, S.B., Wright, R., Beidler, S.M., Cameron, M.E., Shimizu, H. and Mason,
K. (2003) ‘Correlates of children’s competence to make healthcare decisions’, Journal of Clinical
Ethics, Vol. 14, No. 3, pp. 152-63.
Department of Health and Children (2000a) Report of the Public Consultation for the National
Children’s Strategy. Dublin: Department of Health and Children.
Department of Health and Children (2000b) The National Children’s Strategy: Our Children —
Their Lives. Dublin: Department of Health and Children.
Department of Health and Children (1999) Children First: National Guidelines for the Protection
and Welfare of Children. Dublin: Department of Health and Children.
Devine, D. (2003) ‘Voicing concerns about children’s rights and status in school’, in Encouraging
Voices: Respecting the insights of young people who have been marginalised, M. Shevlin and R. Rose
(eds.). Dublin: National Disability Authority.
Dixon-Woods, M., Young, B. and Heaney, D. (1999) ‘Partnerships with children’, British Medical
Journal, Vol. 3, No. 19, pp. 778-80.
Drew, N. (1986) ‘Exclusion and confirmation: Phenomenology of patients’ experiences with
caregivers’, Image: Journal of Nursing Scholarship, Vol. 18, No. 2, pp. 39-43.
Dyer, C. (1999) ‘English teenager given heart transplant against her will’, British Medical Journal,
No. 319, p. 209.
Elliott, E. and Watson, A. (1997) ‘But the doctors aren’t your mum?’ Health Matters, No. 30,
pp. 321-29.
Epstein, L. and Wayman, K. (1998) ‘Interruptions in development: The communicative context
of the young hospitalized child’, Infants & Young Children, Vol. 10, No. 3, pp. 32-43.
Faux, S.A., Walsh, M. and Deatrick, J.A. (1988) ‘Intensive interviewing with children and
adolescents’, Western Journal of Nursing Research, Vol. 10, No. 2, pp. 180-94.
Feinberg, J. (1980) ‘The child’s right to an open future’, in Who’s Child? Children’s Rights, Parental
Authority and State Power, W. Aiken and H. LaFollette (eds.). Boston: Littlefield, Adams.
Flatman, D. (2002) ‘Consulting Children: Are We Listening?’ Paediatric Nursing, Vol. 14, No. 7, pp. 28-31.
Franklin, B. (1986) ‘Children’s Political Rights’, in The Rights of Children, B. Franklin (ed.).
Oxford: Blackwell.
Gabe, J., Olumide, G. and Bury, M. (2004) ‘It takes three to tango: A framework for
understanding patient partnership in paediatric clinics’, Social Science & Medicine, No. 59,
pp. 1071-79.
Gaffney, A. and Dunne, E.A. (1987) ‘Children’s understanding of the causality of pain’, Pain,
No. 29, pp. 91-104.
Gaffney, A. and Dunne, E.A. (1986) ‘Developmental aspects of children’s definitions of pain’,
Pain, No. 26, pp. 105-17.
Gaut, D.A. and Kieckhefer, G.M. (1988) ‘Assessment of self-care agency in chronically ill
adolescents’, Journal of Adolescent Health Care, No. 9, pp. 55-60.
62
Glazebrook, C.P. and Sheard, C. (1994) ‘A prospective study of factors associated with delayed
discharge in school-aged children undergoing ward-based minor surgery’, International Journal of
Nursing Studies, Vol. 31, No. 6, pp. 487-97.
Goffman, E. (1961) Asylums. New York: Doubleday.
Gonzalez, J.C., Routh, D.K. and Armstrong, F.D. (1993) ‘Differential medication of child versus
adult post-operative patients: The effects of nurses’ assumptions’, Child Health Care, No. 22,
pp. 47-59.
Greene, S. and Hogan, D. (2005) Researching children’s experiences: Approaches and methods.
London: Sage Publications.
Gusella, J.L., Ward, A. and Butler, G.S. (1998) ‘The experience of hospitalised children: How well
do we meet their developmental needs?’ Children’s Health Care, Vol. 27, No. 2, pp. 131-45.
Hallstrom, I. and Elander, G. (2004) ‘Decision-making during hospitalization: Parents’ and
children’s involvement’, Journal of Clinical Nursing, No. 13, pp. 367-75.
Harrison, C., Kenny, N.P., Sidarous, M. and Rowell, M. (1997) ‘Bioethics for clinicians: Involving
children in medical decisions’, Canadian Medical Association Journal, Vol. 156, No. 6, pp. 825-28.
Hart, C. and Chesson, R. (1998) ‘Children as consumers’, British Medical Journal, No. 316,
pp. 1600-03.
Hart, R.A. (1992) ‘Children’s Participation: From Tokenism to Citizenship’, Innocenti Essays, No. 4.
Florence: UNICEF Innocenti Research Centre.
Hart, S.N. (1991) ‘From Property to Person Status: Historical Perspective on Children’s Rights’,
American Psychologist, Vol. 46, No. 1, pp. 53-59.
Hawthorn, P.J. (1974) Nurse — I want my Mummy. London: Royal College of Nursing.
Heary, C. and Hennessy, E. (2002) ‘The use of focus group interviews in pediatric healthcare
research’, Journal of Pediatric Psychology, No. 27, pp. 47-56.
Hermerén, G. (1996) Kunskapens pris (‘The price of knowledge’), 2nd edition. Stockholm:
Humanistsk-samhällsvetenskapliga forskningsrådets förlagstjänst.
Hern, M., Miller, M., Sommers, M. and Dyehouse, J. (1998) ‘Sensitive topics and adolescents:
Making research about risk behaviours happen’, Pediatric Nursing, No. 21, pp. 173-86.
Herzlich, C. (1973) Health and illness: A social psychological analysis. London: Academic Press.
Hill, M., Laybourn, A. and Borland, M. (1996) ‘Engaging with primary-aged children about
their emotions and well-being: Methodological considerations’, Children and Society, Vol. 10, No. 2,
pp. 129-45.
Hockenberry-Eaton, M. and Minick, P. (1994) ‘Living with cancer: Children with extraordinary
courage’, Oncology Nursing Forum, No. 21, pp. 1025-31.
Holt, J. (1974) Escape from Childhood. Harmondsworth: Pelican.
Hood, S., Kelley, P. and Mayall, B. (1996) ‘Children as research subjects: A risky enterprise’,
Children and Society, Vol. 10, No. 2, pp. 117-29.
Hoppe, M.J., Wells, E.A., Morrison, D.M., Gillmore, M.R. and Wilsdon, A. (1995) ‘Using focus
groups to discuss sensitive topics with children’, Evaluation Review, Vol. 19, No. 1, pp. 102-14.
Horner, S. (1999) ‘Asthma self-care: Just another piece of schoolwork’, Pediatric Nursing, No. 25,
pp. 597-604.
Igoe, J. (1991) ‘Empowerment of children and youth for consumer self-care’, American Journal of
Health Promotion, Vol. 6, No. 1, pp. 55-65.
63
Inman, C.E. (1991) ‘Analysed interaction in a children’s oncology clinic: The child’s view and
parent’s opinion of the effect of medical encounters’, Journal of Advanced Nursing, Vol. 16, No. 7,
pp. 782-93.
Ireland, L. and Holloway, I. (1996) ‘Qualitative health research with children’, Children and
Society, Vol. 10, No. 2, pp. 155-64.
Jago, D. (1987) ‘Communicating with Children in Hospital’, Maternal and Child Health, June,
pp. 186-88.
John, M. (1996) Children in charge: Children’s rights to a fair hearing. London: Jessica Kingsley.
Kari, J.A., Donovan, C., Li, J. and Taylor, B. (1999) ‘Teenagers in hospital: What do they want?’
Nursing Standard, Vol. 24, No. 13, pp. 49-51.
Kennedy, S.S. and Mohr, W.K. (2001) ‘The conundrum of children in the US healthcare system’,
Nursing Ethics, Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 213-17.
Knafl, K.A., Cavallari, K.A. and Dixon, D.M. (1988) Pediatric hospitalization: Family and nurse
perspectives. Illinois: Scott, Foresman & Co.
Kopelman, L.M. (1997) ‘The best interest standard as threshold, ideal and standard of
reasonableness’, Journal of Medicine and Philosophy, No. 22, pp. 271-89.
La Greca, A.M., Auslander, W.F., Greco, P., Spetter, D., Fisher, E.B. and Santiago, J.V. (1995)
‘I get by with a little help from my family and friends: Adolescents’ support for diabetes care’,
Journal of Pediatric Psychology, Vol. 20, No. 4, pp. 449-76.
LaMontagne, L. (2000) ‘Children’s coping with surgery: A process-oriented perspective’, Journal
of Pediatric Nursing, Vol. 15, No. 5, pp. 307-12.
Lansdown, G. (2001) ‘Promoting children’s participation in democratic decision-making’, Innocenti
Insight, No. 6. Florence: UNICEF Innocenti Research Centre.
Lansdown, G. (1995) Taking Part: Children’s participation in decision-making. London: Institute
for Public Policy Research.
Lansdown, R. (1996) Children in hospital: A guide for family and carers. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Lee, L. (1991) ‘Ethical issues relating to research involving children’, Journal of Paediatric
Oncology Nursing, Vol. 8, No. 1, pp. 24-29.
Lewis, M.A. and Lewis, C.E. (1990) ‘Consequences of empowering children to care for
themselves’, Paediatrician, Vol. 17, No. 2, pp. 63-67.
Lodge, A. and Lynch, K. (2003) ‘Young people’s equality concerns: The invisibility of diversity’, in
Encouraging Voices: Respecting the insights of young people who have been marginalised, M. Shevlin
and R. Rose (eds.). Dublin: National Disability Authority.
Lowden, J. (2002) ‘Children’s Rights: A Decade of Dispute’, Journal of Advanced Nursing, Vol. 37,
No. 1, pp. 100-07.
Lowes, L. (1996) ‘Paediatric nursing and children’s autonomy’, Journal of Clinical Nursing, No. 5,
pp. 367-72.
Lumley, M.A., Abeles, L.A., Melamed, B.G., Pistone, L.M and Johnson, J.H. (1990) ‘Coping
outcomes in children undergoing stressful medical procedures: The role of child environment
variables’, Behavioural Assessment, No. 12, pp. 223-38.
Mahon, A., Glendinning, C., Clarke, K. and Craig, G. (1996) ‘Researching children: Methods and
ethics’, Children and Society, Vol. 10, No. 2, pp. 145-54.
Mastroianni, A.C. and Kahn, J.P. (2002) ‘Risk and responsibility: Ethics, Grimes v Kennedy
Krieger, and public health research involving children’, American Journal of Public Health, Vol. 92,
No. 7, pp. 1073-76.
64
Mates, D. and Allison, K.R. (1992) ‘Sources of stress and coping responses of high school
students’, Adolescence, Vol. 27, No. 106, pp. 461-74.
Mauthner, M. (1997) ‘Methodological aspects of collecting data from children: Lessons from three
research projects’, Children and Society, No. 11, pp. 16-28.
Mayall, B. (2002) Towards a Sociology for Childhood: Thinking from children’s lives. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Mayall, B. (1996a) ‘Politics and practice in research with children’, International Journal of
Psychology and Psychotherapy, Vol. 14, No. 3, pp. 199-203.
Mayall, B. (1996b) Children, health and the social order. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Mayall, B. (1995) ‘The changing context of childhood: Children’s perspectives on health care
resources including services’, in The Health of our Children, B. Botting (ed.). London: HMSO.
Mayall, B. (ed.) (1994) Children’s childhoods: Observed and experienced. London: Falmer Press.
McAuley, K. and Brattman, M. (2002) Hearing Young Voices: Consulting Children and Young People,
including those experiencing poverty or other forms of social exclusion, in relation to public policy
development in Ireland. Dublin: Children’s Rights Alliance and National Youth Council of Ireland.
McCormick, L.K., Crawford, M., Anderson, R.H., Gittelsohn, J., Kingsley, B. and Upson, D. (1999)
‘Recruiting adolescents into qualitative tobacco research studies: Experiences and lessons learned’,
Journal of School Health, Vol. 69, No. 3, pp. 95-99.
McClowery, S.G. (1988) ‘A review of the literature pertaining to the psychosocial responses of
school-aged children to hospitalisation’, Journal of Paediatric Nursing, Vol. 3, No. 5, pp. 296-311.
Miller, P.B. and Kenny, N.P. (2002) ‘Walking the moral tightrope: Respecting and protecting
children in health-related research’, Healthcare Ethics, Vol. 11, No. 3, pp. 217-29.
Miller, S. (2001) ‘Facilitating decision-making in young people’, Paediatric Nursing, Vol. 13, No. 5,
pp. 31-35.
Morrow, V. (1996) Listening to children: A handbook of current practice. London: Routledge.
Moss-Morris, R. and Patterson, J. (1995) ‘Understanding children’s concepts of health and
illness: Implications for developmental therapists physical and occupational therapy’, Paediatrics,
Vol. 14, No. 3, pp. 95-108.
Murray, C. and Hallett, C. (2000) ‘Young people’s participation in decisions affecting their
welfare’, Childhood, Vol. 7, No. 1, pp. 11-25.
NCO (2005) Guidelines on how to involve children and young people in your work. Dublin: National
Children’s Office.
NCO (2004) Ready, Steady, Play! A National Play Policy. Dublin: National Children’s Office.
Nitschke, R., Humphrey, B., Sexauer, C., Catron, B., Wunder, S. and Jay, S. (1982) ‘Therapeutic
choices made by patients with end stage cancer’, Journal of Pediatrics, Vol. 101, No. 3, pp. 471-76.
Norbeck, J.S. and Tilden, V.P. (1983) ‘Life stress, social support and emotional disequilibrium in
complications of pregnancy: A prospective multivariate study’, Journal of Health and Social
Behaviour, No. 34, pp. 30-46.
Noyes, J. (2000) ‘Enabling young ‘ventilator-dependent’ people to express their views and
experiences of their care in hospital’, Journal of Advanced Nursing, Vol. 31, No. 5, pp. 1206-15.
Oakley, A. et al (1995) ‘Health and cancer prevention: Knowledge and beliefs of children and
young people’, British Medical Journal, No. 310, pp. 1029-33.
O’Quigley, A. (2000) Listening to children’s voices — The findings and recommendations of recent
research. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation.
65
Oldfield, C. and Fowler, C. (2004) Mapping children and young people’s participation in England.
Nottingham: Department for Education and Skills.
Oppong, O.A. and Heycock, E.G. (1997) ‘Adolescent health services — through their eyes’,
Archives of Disease in Childhood, No. 77, pp. 115-19.
Our Lady’s Hospital for Sick Children (2002) Guidelines for good practice. Dublin: Our Lady’s
Hospital for Sick Children, Crumlin.
Pearlman, N. and Abromovitch, R. (1987) ‘Visit to the paediatrician: Children’s concerns’, Journal
of Paediatrics, Vol. 110, No. 6, pp. 988-90.
Perrin, E.C. and Perrin, J.M. (1983) ‘Clinician’s assessment of children’s understanding of illness’,
American Journal of Diseased Child, No. 137, pp. 874-78.
Peterson, L. (1989) ‘Coping by children undergoing stressful medical procedures: Some
conceptual, methodological and therapeutic issues’, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Vol. 57, No. 3, pp. 380-87.
Peterson, L. and Toler, S.M. (1986) ‘An information seeking disposition in child surgery
behaviour’, Health Psychology, Vol. 5, No. 4, pp. 343-58.
Pittman, K. (1992) ‘Awakening child consumerism in healthcare’, Paediatric Nursing, Vol. 18,
No. 2, pp. 132-36.
Prilleltensky, I., Nelson, G. and Peirson, L. (2001) ‘The role of power and control in children’s
lives: An ecological analysis of pathways toward wellness, resilience and problems’, Journal of
Community Applied Social Psychology, No. 11, pp. 143-58.
Pursell, E. (1995) ‘Listening to children: Medical treatment and consent’, Journal of Advanced
Nursing, No. 21, pp. 623-24.
Reissland, N. (1983) ‘Cognitive maturity and the experience of fear and pain in hospital’, Social
Science and Medicine, No. 17, pp. 1389-95.
Robinson, C.A. (1996) ‘Health care relationships revisited’, Journal of Family Nursing, Vol. 2,
No. 2, pp. 152-73.
Roche, J. (1999) ‘Children: Rights, participation and citizenship’, Childhood, Vol. 6, No. 4,
pp. 475-93.
Rodham, H. (1973) ‘Children under the Law’, reprinted in Rethinking Childhood: Perspectives
on development and society (1976), A. Skolnick (ed.). Boston: Little Brown.
Ross, L.F. (2002) Children, Families and Health Care Decision-making. Oxford: Clarendon.
Ross, L.F. (1997) ‘Health care decision-making by children: Is it in their best interest?’ Hastings
Centre Report, Vol. 27, No. 6, pp. 41-45.
Ross, D.M. and Ross, S.A. (1984) ‘Childhood pain: The school-aged child’s viewpoint’, Pain, No. 20,
pp. 179-91.
Runeson, I., Elander, G., Hermerén, G. and Kristensson-Hallstrom, I. (2000) ‘Children’s consent
to treatment: Using a scale to assess degrees of self-determination’, Pediatric Nursing, Vol. 26, No. 5,
pp. 16-22.
Runeson, I., Enskar, K., Elander, G. and Hermerén, G. (2001) ‘Professionals’ perceptions of children’s
participation in decision-making in healthcare’, Journal of Clinical Nursing, No. 10, pp. 70-78.
Runeson, I., Hallstrom, I., Elander, G. and Hermerén, G. (2002a) ‘Children’s participation in the
decision-making process during hospitalisation: An observational study’, Nursing Ethics, Vol. 9,
No. 6, pp. 583-98.
Runeson, I., Hallstrom, I., Elander, G. and Hermerén, G. (2002b) ‘Children’s needs during
hospitalisation: An observational study of hospitalised boys’, International Journal of Nursing,
Vol. 8, No. 3, pp. 158-66.
66
Rushforth, H. (1996) ‘Nurses’ knowledge of how children view health and illness’, Paediatric
Nursing, Vol. 8, No. 9, pp. 23-27.
Rylance, G. (1996) ‘Making decisions with children’, British Medical Journal, Vol. 312, No. 7034,
pp. 1-3.
Sartain, S., Clarke, C. and Heyman, R. (2000) ‘Hearing the voices of children with chronic
illness’, Journal of Advanced Nursing, Vol. 32, No. 4, pp. 913-21.
Schmale, A.H. and Iker, H. (1971) ‘Hopelessness as a predictor of cervical cancer’, Social Science
and Medicine, No. 5, pp. 95-100.
Schultz, K. (1993) ‘In Hungary, children help decide’, Hastings Centre Report, Vol. 23, No. 2, p. 21.
Shevlin, M. and Rose, R. (2003) Encouraging Voices: Respecting the insights of young people who
have been marginalised. Dublin: National Disability Authority.
Shiminski-Maher, T. (1993) ‘Physician-patient-parent communication problems’, Pediatric
Neurosurgery, Vol. 19, No. 2, pp. 104-08.
Silverman, D. (1987) Communication and medical practice: Social relations in the clinic. London:
Sage Publications.
Skipper, J.K. and Leonard, R. (1986) ‘Children, stress and hospitalisation: A field experiment’,
Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, Vol. 9, No. 4, pp. 275-87.
Skolnick, A. (ed.) (1976) Rethinking Childhood: Perspectives on development and society. Boston:
Little Brown.
Sloper, P. and Lightfoot, J. (2003) ‘Involving disabled and chronically ill children and young
people in healthcare service development’, Child: Care, Health & Development, Vol. 29, No. 1,
pp. 15-20.
Stewart, T.J., Pantell, R.H., Dias, J.K., Wells, P.A. and Ross, A.W. (1981) ‘Children as patients:
A communication process study in family practice’, Journal of Family Practice, No. 13, pp. 827-35.
Strong, P.M. (1979) The ceremonial order of the clinic: Parents, doctors and medical bureaucracies.
London: Routledge.
Taggart, V., Zuckerman, A., Sly, R., Steinmueller, C., Newman, G., O’Brien, R., Schneider, S. and
Bellanti, J. (1991) ‘You can control asthma: Evaluation of an asthma education programme for
hospitalised inner-city children’, Patient Education and Counselling, No. 17, pp. 35-47.
Tates, K. and Meeuwesen, L. (2001) ‘Doctor-parent-child communication: A review of the
literature’, Social Science and Medicine, No. 52, pp. 839-51.
Tates, K., Elbers, E., Meeuwesen, L. and Bensing, J. (2002) ‘Doctor-parent-child relationships:
A “pas de trois” ‘, Patient Education and Counselling, No. 48, pp. 5-14.
Thomas, N. (2000) Children, Family and the State: Decision-making and child participation.
Basingstoke: Macmillan.
Thomas, N. and O’Kane, C. (1999) ‘Children’s experiences of decision-making in middle
childhood’, Childhood, Vol. 6, No. 3, pp. 34-39.
Thompson, M.L. (1994) ‘Information-seeking coping and anxiety in school-age children
anticipating surgery’, Children’s Healthcare, Vol. 23, No. 2, pp. 87-97.
Thorne, S.E. (1993) ‘Relationships with health care providers’, in Negotiating health care: The
social context of chronic illness, S.E. Thorne (ed.). London: Sage Publications.
Tieffenberg, J., Wood, E., Alonso, A., Tossutti, M. and Vincente, M. (2000) ‘A randomized trial
of ACINDES: A child-centered training model for children with chronic illnesses (asthma and
epilepsy)’, Journal of Urban Health, Vol. 77, No. 2, pp. 280-97.
67
Toce, S.S. (2004) ‘Ethical decision-making for the child with a life-limiting condition’, Care of the
Dying. Available at: www.careofthedying.org
Tong, E. and Sparacino, P. (1994) ‘Special management issues for young adolescents and young
adults with congenital heart disease’, Paediatric and Neonatal Cardiology, Vol. 6, No. 1, pp. 199-214.
Treseder, P. (1997) Empowering children and young people: Promoting involvement in decision-making.
London: Save the Children/Children’s Rights Office.
United Nations (1989) Convention on the Rights of the Child. Geneva: United Nations.
Van Dulmen, A.M. (1998) ‘Children’s contribution to pediatric outpatient encounters’, Pediatrics,
Vol. 102, No. 3, pp. 563-68.
Van Eys, J. (1986) ‘Children as decision-makers’, Journal of the Association of Pediatric Oncology
Nurses, Vol. 3, No. 3, pp. 18-22.
Visintainer, M.A. and Wolfer, J.A. (1975) ‘Psychological preparation for surgical pediatric
patients: The effect of children’s and parents’ stress responses and adjustment’, Pediatrics, No. 56,
pp. 187-202.
Ward, L. (1997) Seen and Heard: Involving disabled children and young people in research and
development projects. York: Joseph Rowntree Foundation.
Weithorn, L.A. and Campbell, S.B. (1982) ‘The competency of children and adolescents to make
informed treatment decisions’, Child Development, No. 53, pp. 1589-99.
Wilkinson, J. (2003) ‘Young people with cancer — How should their care be organised?’ European
Journal of Cancer Care, No. 12, pp. 65-70.
Wilson, S., Morse, J.M. and Penrod, J. (1998) ‘Developing reciprocal trust in the care-giving
relationship’, Qualitative Health Research, Vol. 8, No. 4, pp. 446-65.
Young, B., Dixon-Woods, M., Windridge, K.C. and Heney, D. (2003) ‘Managing communication
with young people who have a life-threatening chronic illness: A qualitative study of patients and
parents’, British Medical Journal, Vol. 326, No. 7384, pp. 305-09.
68
APPENDICES
69
Appendix 1a:
Hart’s Ladder of Participation
Child-initiated, shared
8 decisions with adults
Child-initiated
7 and directed
6
Adult-initiated, shared Degrees of
decisions with children
participation
Consulted
5 and informed
Assigned
4 but informed
3 Tokenism
2 Decoration Non-
participation
1 Manipulation
70
Appendix 1b:
Hart’s Degrees of Participation
7. Youth-initiated and directed is when young people initiate and direct a project or
programme. Adults are involved only in a supportive role.
5. Consulted and informed is when youth give advice on projects or programmes designed
and run by adults. The youth are informed about how their input will be used and the
outcomes of the decisions made by adults.
4. Assigned but informed is where youth are assigned a specific role and informed about
how and why they are being involved.
3. Tokenism is where young people appear to be given a voice, but in fact have little
or no choice about what they do or how they participate.
2. Decoration is where young people are used to help or ‘bolster’ a cause in a relatively
indirect way, although adults do not pretend that the cause is inspired by youth.
1. Manipulation is where adults use youth to support causes and pretend that the causes
are inspired by youth.
71
Appendix 2:
Parent Information Letter
Dear Parent,
We are carrying out research into children and their role in
consultation and decision-making in a hospital setting and would
like your assistance. Our project is funded by the National Children’s
Office and our aim is to give children a voice and for their opinions
to be heard. We are looking to interview children aged 7 to 18 years
to participate in focus groups on decision-making. These focus
groups will last for approximately 45 minutes and will be conducted
by trained researchers who are also qualified paediatric nurses. We
may approach you for your consent to allow your child to participate
in these focus groups and if consent is given, we will also seek
consent from your child prior to participation. Confidentiality is
assured and your child may withdraw from the focus group at any
stage. We would greatly appreciate you and your child’s co-operation
to help make this research enhance the lives of children, but
ultimately to allow their voices to be heard.
If you have any queries, please do not hesitate to contact us, either
by approaching us directly or at the telephone number below.
Yours sincerely,
Eilis Hayes
Research Assistant
72
Appendix 3:
Information Sheet for Older Child
Dear Participant,
My name is Eilis and I am writing not only to inform you about this
research study, but also to invite you to become involved in the study.
The aim of the study is to explore how children feel about being
involved in consultations and decisions about their care and whether
children want to be involved in these decisions. I would like to talk
to you about your experiences of involvement in care and decisions
whilst in hospital. The questions I will ask will be easy to answer
and if you do not want to answer some of them that is okay. Some
examples of the questions I might ask you are: ‘What it is like being
in hospital?’ and ‘What care you like to do for yourself?’ and ‘Are you
involved in decisions about your care?’ It is hoped that the results
from this study will be used to help to improve the care provided
for other parents and their children who come into hospital.
The information will be collected through the use of interviews.
Therefore should you agree to participate in the study I (the
researcher) will arrange to conduct an interview with you on the
ward at a time that is convenient for you. The interview will take
approximately one hour and will be tape-recorded. You are under no
obligation to participate in the study and the information that you
provide will be viewed only by the researcher and will not be shared
with your parents or nurses.
If you have any questions that you would like to ask me, you can
call me on my phone XXXXX or ask your parents to call me and
I will call into you to answer your questions.
Thank you for taking the time to read this information sheet.
Eilis
73
Appendix 4:
Parental Consent — Permission to approach child
Signed:
Dated:
Witnessed:
Dated:
74
Appendix 5:
Parental Consent — Permission for child participation
Signed:
Dated:
Witnessed:
Dated:
75
Appendix 6:
Older Child Consent
This research study has been explained to me. The researcher has
answered all of my questions and I understand what will happen
during the study. All of my questions have been answered and I do
want to take part in the study.
Child’s Name:
Child’s Signature:
Witnessed:
Dated:
76
Appendix 7:
Younger Child Consent
If you would like to help in my project, all you have to do is write your
name below. If your answer is yes, then I will come to talk with you.
Name:
Thank you
77
Appendix 8:
Protocol 1: Child displaying anxiety during interview
Signed:
78
Appendix 9:
Protocol 2: Signs of illness during interview
Signed:
79
Appendix 10:
Protocol 3: Disclosure of sensitive information during interview
Signed:
80
Appendix 11:
Treseder’s Degrees of Participation
81
Appendix 12:
Children’s Levels of Participation
82
PUBLISHED BY THE STATIONERY OFFICE DUBLIN
To be purchased from the
GOVERNMENT PUBLICATIONS SALES OFFICE,
SUN ALLIANCE HOUSE,
MOLESWORTH STREET, DUBLIN 2