Traditional Bullying Victimization and Cyberbullying Perpetration The Role of Anger Rumination and Self-Control
Traditional Bullying Victimization and Cyberbullying Perpetration The Role of Anger Rumination and Self-Control
To cite this article: Huaibin Jiang, Yinchuan Jin & Qun Yang (2025) Traditional Bullying
Victimization and Cyberbullying Perpetration: the Role of Anger Rumination and Self-Control,
Psychology Research and Behavior Management, , 877-886, DOI: 10.2147/PRBM.S507510
Correspondence: Yinchuan Jin; Qun Yang, Department of Clinical Psychology, School of Medical Psychology, Fourth Military Medical University, No. 169,
Changle West Road, Xincheng District, Xi’an, Shaanxi, 710032, People’s Republic of China, Email [email protected]; [email protected]
Background: The transition from being a victim of traditional bullying to engaging in cyberbullying is an emerging area of research.
However, not all adolescents who experience traditional bullying go on to perpetrate cyberbullying. Grounded in the General
Aggression Model, this study investigates the longitudinal association between traditional bullying victimization and cyberbullying
perpetration, focusing on the underlying mechanisms.
Methods: In a longitudinal design, 442 middle school students (49.80% female, Mage = 13.02, SD = 0.85) completed a survey
including the Victim Scale, Anger Rumination Scale, Self-Control Scale, and Cyberbullying Scale at baseline enrollment and at six-
month follow-up.
Results: Key findings include: (1) T1 Traditional bullying victimization did not directly predict later T2 cyberbullying perpetration;
(2) T2 Anger rumination mediated the relationship between T1 traditional bullying victimization and T2 cyberbullying perpetration;
(3) T1 Self-control moderated the link between T2 anger rumination and T2 cyberbullying perpetration, with stronger association
observed in adolescents with lower self-control.
Conclusion: These results highlight crucial pathways from traditional bullying victimization to cyberbullying perpetration in
adolescents.
Keywords: traditional bullying victimization, anger rumination, self-control, cyberbullying perpetration
Introduction
The advent of the digital age has profoundly impacted people’s lives, particularly those in middle school settings. As
digital natives, adolescents benefit from the convenience and wealth of information available on the Internet but are also
vulnerable to cyberbullying, which involves harmful behaviors enacted through digital platforms such as the Internet or
mobile phones.1 Adolescents are at a critical developmental stage characterized by unbalanced physical and mental
growth and limited cognitive abilities.2 The anonymity provided by cyberspace facilitates the perpetration of cyberbully
ing and complicates efforts to address and mitigate its impact. Both perpetrators and victims of cyberbullying may suffer
from psychological and behavioral issues such as anxiety and social withdrawal, which can impede their mental health
and social interactions.3,4 Research has identified a relationship between being bullied and being a bully.5,6 While the
global prevalence of cyberbullying is well documented, traditional bullying victimization remains a significant issue,
particularly in school contexts. Traditional bullying, characterized by physical and verbal aggression, continues to affect
a large number of adolescents, and its consequences often extend to the digital realm, facilitating the onset of
cyberbullying behaviors. The association between traditional bullying victimization and subsequent cyberbullying
perpetration has been explored in several studies.7 However, much of the existing research in this area has been
conducted in Western contexts, where cultural, social, and educational dynamics may differ from those in Asian schools.
Recent studies indicate that the factors influencing bullying behaviors in Asian contexts, such as the influence of
collectivist values, teacher-student relationships, and family dynamics, may play a unique role in shaping bullying
experiences.8–10 These studies highlight the need for further investigation into how traditional bullying victimization in
Asian schools contributes to cyberbullying perpetration and the mechanisms that drive this process.
Given the distinct cultural and social dynamics within Asian school settings, it is essential to explore these issues in
more depth. Although the relationship between traditional bullying and cyberbullying has been established, there is a lack
of research that delves into the specific pathways linking traditional bullying victimization to cyberbullying, particularly
within the Asian context. Understanding these pathways is crucial for developing culturally appropriate and effective
prevention and intervention strategies. This study aims to fill this gap by examining how traditional bullying victimiza
tion leads to cyberbullying perpetration, with a focus on the mediating role of anger rumination and the moderating role
of self-control. In doing so, it hopes to contribute both theoretically and practically to the reduction of cyberbullying
among adolescents in Asia.
Self-Control as a Moderator
The General Aggression Model (GAM) posits that individual characteristics (eg, self-control) and environmental
influences (eg, victimization from traditional bullying) shape adolescents’ cognitive processes (eg, anger rumination)
and behaviors (eg, engaging in cyberbullying).13 Self-control, defined as the capacity to manage or modify one’s
responses, is essential for achieving adaptive and socially acceptable outcomes.25 Strong self-control enhances the ability
to suppress anger-related rumination,26 allowing individuals to focus more effectively on goal-directed activities while
minimizing intrusive, ruminative thoughts. In contrast, those with poor self-control may struggle to regulate future-
oriented thoughts, leading to increased rumination.27,28 Research indicates that high self-control mitigates tendencies
toward anger and aggressive behavior, disrupting cycles of rumination and alleviating negative emotional states.29
Furthermore, self-control moderates the link between anger rumination and cyberbullying.30 Specifically, low self-control
exacerbates the connection between anger rumination and cyberbullying behavior, whereas high self-control individuals
are better equipped to regulate their temper and impulses, diminishing the influence of anger rumination on cyberbullying
tendencies.
Method
Participants and Procedure
The study sampled students from two middle schools in Fujian, China. Initially, a representative city in Fujian Province
was selected as the target community. Within this community, two middle schools with comparable characteristics in
terms of size, teaching quality, and student demographics were randomly chosen. In each school, four classes were
randomly selected, and two waves of surveys were conducted in April 2023 (T1) and October 2023 (T2). A stratified
random sampling method was employed to ensure the broad representativeness of the sample. The surveys were
administered by trained graduate students who obtained consent from the participants and their parents one week
prior. At T1, 486 valid questionnaires were collected, and at T2, 422 valid follow-up questionnaires were gathered.
The dropout rate was 13.17%, with 64 participants lost between T1 and T2. No significant differences were found in
bullying, anger rumination, self-control, or cyberbullying scores between those who dropped out and those who
completed both surveys. The participants were aged between 12 and 16 years (Mage = 13.02, SD = 0.85), with 210
females (49.80%). The data collection procedures received approval from the Ethics Committee of the first authors’
institution. Additionally, six months have been used as an appropriate longitudinal period in prior studies.31,32
Measures
Victim Scale
The Chinese adaptation of the Victim Questionnaire was employed to assess experiences of traditional bullying
victimization.11,33 This instrument includes six items, each rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1 = “never”, 4 = “more
than four times”). An example question is: “Have classmates ever kicked, pushed, or threatened you?” Higher aggregate
scores reflect greater victimization levels. The scale has been validated with adolescents, showing satisfactory reliability
and validity.7 In this research, Cronbach’s α was 0.76.
angry”. Higher total scores signify higher tendencies for anger rumination. Prior studies have confirmed its psychometric
soundness among adolescents.35 The Cronbach’s α coefficient in the present study was 0.94.
Self-Control Scale
The Self-Control Scale in its Chinese version was utilized to measure levels of self-control.36,37 It comprises 13 items,
scored on a 4-point scale (1 = “strongly disagree”, 4 = “strongly agree”). A sample statement is: “Others think I exhibit
strong self-discipline”. Items 8 and 10 are reverse-coded. Higher overall scores indicate better self-control. This scale has
demonstrated robust reliability and validity in prior research.38 In this study, Cronbach’s α was 0.72.
Cyberbullying Scale
Cyberbullying perpetration was assessed using the Cyberbullying Scale.39,40 The scale includes six items rated on a 4-
point scale (1 = “never”, 4 = “several times a week”). A representative item is: “I share others’ private information
online”. Higher total scores correspond to higher levels of cyberbullying perpetration. Previous studies have demon
strated the reliability and validity of the scale.7 In this research, Cronbach’s α was 0.78.
Data Analyses
Descriptive statistics and Pearson’s correlation analysis were conducted using SPSS 25.0 to explore the fundamental
characteristics and interrelations among the variables. The mediation of anger rumination between traditional bullying
victimization and cyberbullying perpetration was examined using Model 4 from Hayes’ PROCESS macro.41
Additionally, the moderating role of self-control in the link between anger rumination and cyberbullying perpetration
was analyzed through Model 14. Gender was controlled for in the analysis to account for its potential influence on the
study variables.
Results
Preliminary Analyses
To address the issue of potential common method bias, which may arise from self-reported measures, diagnostic testing
was performed. Harman’s single-factor test was utilized to evaluate common method bias across all items related to the
four variables: traditional bullying victimization, anger rumination, self-control, and cyberbullying perpetration.
Consistent with the threshold of 40%,42 the analysis revealed ten factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. The first factor
accounted for 22.61% of the total variance, significantly below the critical 40% cutoff. These findings suggest that
common method bias is not a major concern in this study.
The descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1. Correlation results show that traditional bullying victimization at
T1 is positively associated with anger rumination at T2. Anger rumination at T2, in turn, is positively linked to
cyberbullying perpetration, while self-control at T1 is negatively associated with anger rumination at T2.
Mediation Analysis
Figure 1 presents the mediation analysis results. Traditional bullying victimization at T1 was found to significantly
predict anger rumination at T2, which, in turn, significantly predicted cyberbullying perpetration at T2. Bootstrap testing
with a 95% confidence interval revealed that anger rumination completely mediated the link between traditional bullying
victimization and cyberbullying perpetration (ab = 0.06, SE = 0.05, 95% CI = [0.001, 0.180]). The mediation effect
accounted for 83.64% of the total effect.
Discussion
To better understand the link between traditional bullying victimization and cyberbullying, this study explores their
longitudinal relationship, focusing on the mechanisms and conditions under which traditional bullying victimization
associated with cyberbullying perpetration. The findings partially validate the hypotheses, revealing that while traditional
bullying victimization does not directly lead to cyberbullying perpetration, it exerts an indirect link through anger
rumination. Additionally, self-control moderates the relationship between anger rumination and cyberbullying
perpetration.
more easily activated. The anonymity and concealment of cyberspace make adolescents more prone to engage in
cyberbullying and cyber retaliation as a way to retaliate and vent.22,24
Therefore, anger rumination can be seen as a key mediating mechanism that not only intensifies victims’ negative
emotions but also weakens the inhibitory effect of learned helplessness on cyberbullying behavior, thereby indirectly
linking traditional bullying victimization to cyberbullying behavior.
Limitations
This study acknowledges several limitations. First, the use of a longitudinal design, while helpful in exploring trends over
time, does not provide strong evidence for causality.53 As such, future research would benefit from employing more
rigorous experimental designs to better understand the causal relationship between traditional bullying victimization and
cyberbullying perpetration. Second, unobserved individual-level factors (such as personality, mental health history, and
coping mechanisms) and family-level factors (such as parenting style, socioeconomic background, and family stressors)
may significantly influence traditional bullying victimization and subsequent behaviors, such as cyberbullying perpetra
tion. Therefore, the results should be interpreted with caution. Future research should incorporate these factors and utilize
longitudinal or multilevel models to further explore their impact. Third, this study use different time points for measuring
self-control (T1) and anger rumination (T2). Although previous research has supported the use of T1 for moderating
variables and T2 for mediating variables, this design choice limits our ability to directly assess changes in these variables
over time. Future research could consider using more consistent time points to measure both self-control and anger
rumination, allowing for a more comprehensive exploration of their dynamic interactions. Forth, this study did not
include measures of traditional bullying perpetration or cyberbullying victimization. This omission may limit our
understanding of the interplay between these behaviors and the primary constructs investigated. Future research should
consider incorporating these measures to provide a more comprehensive analysis. Lastly, the sample in this study was
limited to adolescents from Fujian, China, meaning that the results may not be generalizable to other regions or cultural
contexts. To strengthen the external validity of the conclusions, future studies should include a broader and more diverse
sample, spanning different geographical locations and cultural backgrounds.
Implications
Despite its limitations, this study makes significant contributions to both theoretical understanding and practical
applications. Theoretically, it is the first to employ a longitudinal design to explore the relationship between traditional
bullying victimization and cyberbullying perpetration, offering a fresh perspective on this complex issue. By integrating
the GAM, this research extends the existing literature by examining how traditional bullying victimization is linked with
cyberbullying perpetration, bridging the gap between traditional and online aggression frameworks. Unlike previous
studies that primarily focused on environmental or social factors, this study highlights the significant role of anger
rumination as a cognitive-emotional mechanism that could help explain the connection between victimization and
aggressive online behaviors.
Practically, these findings provide a foundation for developing targeted intervention strategies. By focusing on
enhancing adolescents’ self-control and promoting healthier emotional regulation, such interventions could effectively
mitigate the risk of cyberbullying perpetration. Moreover, the study offers insights for designing prevention programs
that address both the immediate emotional consequences of bullying victimization and the long-term cognitive patterns
that may perpetuate aggression in digital contexts. This integrated approach holds promise for reducing cyberbullying
behaviors and fostering safer online environments for adolescents.
Ethical Statement
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Fujian Polytechnic Normal University (2022-003) and all methods
were complied with the Declaration of Helsinki. We have obtained the informed consent from the study participants and
the parents/legal guardians.
Acknowledgments
We extend our heartfelt gratitude to the schools, teachers, parents, and adolescents for their invaluable support in making
this research possible.
Funding
This research was supported by the Joint Founding Project of Innovation Research Institute of Xijing Hospital
(LHJJ24XL09) and Rapid Response Project of Air Force Medical University (2023KXKT057).
Disclosure
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References
1. Smith PK, Slonje R. Cyberbullying: the nature and extent of a new kind of bullying, in and out of school. In: Jimerson SR, Swearer SM, Espelage
DL, editors. Handbook of Bullying in Schools: An International Perspective. New York: Routledge; 2009:249–262.
2. Larsen B, Luna B. Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev.
2018;94:179–195. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.005
3. Anyerson SGT, Jorge ER, Lesmes GZ. The effect of bullying and cyberbullying on predicting suicide risk in adolescent females: the mediating role
of depression. Psychiatry Res. 2024;337:115968. doi:10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2024.115968
4. Elizabeth C, Estefanía E, Martínez-Monteagudo MC, Beatriz D. Emotional adjustment in victims and perpetrators of cyberbullying and traditional
bullying. Soc Psychol Edu. 2020;23(4):917–942. doi:10.1007/s11218-020-09565-z
5. Camacho A, Runions K, Ortega-Ruiz R, et al. Bullying and cyberbullying perpetration and victimization: prospective within-person associations. J
Youth Adolesc. 2023;52:406–418. doi:10.1007/s10964-022-01704-3
6. Falla D, Ortega-Ruiz R, Runions K, Romera EM. Why do victims become perpetrators of peer bullying? Moral disengagement in the cycle of
violence. Youth Soc. 2022;54(3):397–418. doi:10.1177/0044118X20973702
7. Liang H, Zhu F, Li X, Jiang H, Zhang Q, Xiao W. The link between bullying victimization, maladjustment, self-control, and bullying: a comparison
of traditional and cyberbullying perpetrator. Youth Soc. 2024. doi:10.1177/0044118X241247213
8. Son H, Jang H, Park H, Subramanian SV, Kim J. Exploring the trajectories of depressive symptoms associated with bullying victimization: the
intersection of gender and family support. J Adolesc. 2024. doi:10.1002/jad.12450
9. Park H, Son H, Jang H, Kim J. Chronic bullying victimization and life satisfaction among children from multicultural families in South Korea:
heterogeneity by immigrant mothers’ country of origin. Child Abuse Negl. 2024;151:106718. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2024.106718
10. Kim J, Fong E. The influence of bullying victimization on acculturation and life satisfaction among children from multicultural families in South
Korea. J Ethnic Migrat Stud. 2024;50(18):4718–4737. doi:10.1080/1369183X.2024.2359678
11. Olweus D. Bullying at School: What We Know and What We Can Do. Oxford: Blackwell; 1993.
12. Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. Human aggression. Ann Rev Psychol. 2002;53(1):27–51. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231
13. Dewall CN, Anderson CA, Bushman BJ. The general aggression model: theoretical extensions to violence. Psychol Viol. 2011;1(3):245–258.
doi:10.1037/a0023842
14. Chu XW, Fan CY, Liu QQ, Zhou ZK. Stability and change of bullying roles in the traditional and virtual contexts: a three-wave longitudinal study
in Chinese early adolescents. J Adolesc. 2018;47(11):2384–2400. doi:10.1007/s10964-018-0908-4
15. Faye M, Mona K, Tahany G, Joanne D. Risk factors for involvement in cyber bullying: victims, bullies and bully–victims. Child Youth Services
Rev. 2011;34(1):63–70. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2011.08.032
16. Raskauskas J, Stoltz AD. Involvement in traditional and electronic bullying among adolescents. Develop Psychol. 2007;43(3):564–575.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.564
17. Zhu W, Lu D, Li C, Tian X, Bai X. Longitudinal associations between ostracism, anger rumination, and social aggression. Curr Psychol. 2023;43
(4):3158–3165. doi:10.1007/S12144-023-04279-9
18. Sukhodolsky DG, Golub A, Cromwell EN. Development and validation of the anger rumination scale. Pers Individ Dif. 2001;31(5):689–700.
doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00171-9
19. Sourander A, Klomek AB, Ikonen M, Lindroos J, Helenius H. Psychosocial risk factors associated with cyberbullying among adolescents: a
population-based study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(7):720–728. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.79
20. Schacter HL, Juvonen J. The effects of school-level victimization on self-blame: evidence for contextualized social cognitions. Develop Psychol.
2015;51(6):841–847. doi:10.1037/dev0000016
21. Quan F, Huang J, Li H, Zhu W. Longitudinal relations between bullying victimization and aggression: the multiple mediation effects of anger
rumination and hostile automatic thoughts. Psych J. 2024;13(5):849–859. doi:10.1002/pchj.760
22. Yang J, Li W, Wang W, Gao L, Wang X. Anger rumination and adolescents’ cyberbullying perpetration: moral disengagement and callous-
unemotional traits as moderators. J Affect Disorders. 2021;278:397–404. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.090
23. Yang J, Li W, Gao L, Wang X. How is trait anger related to adolescents’ cyberbullying perpetration? A moderated mediation analysis. J Interpers
Violence. 2022;37(9–10):NP6633–NP6654. doi:10.1177/0886260520967129
24. Camacho A, Ortega-Ruiz R, Romera EM. Longitudinal associations between cybervictimization, anger rumination, and cyberaggression. Aggress
Behav. 2021;47(3):332–342. doi:10.1002/ab.21958
25. Wills TA, Pokhrel P, Morehouse E, Fenster B. Behavioral and emotional regulation and adolescent substance use problems: a test of moderation
effects in a dual-process model. Psychol Addict Behav. 2011;25(2):279–292. doi:10.1037/a0022870
26. Hofmann W, Schmeichel BJ, Baddeley AD. Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(3):174–180. doi:10.1016/j.
tics.2012.01.006
27. Baumeister RF, Vohs KD, Tice DM. The strength model of self-control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2007;16(6):351–355. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
8721.2007.00534.x
28. Heatherton TF, Wagner DD. Cognitive neuroscience of self-regulation failure. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(3):132–139. doi:10.1016/j.
tics.2010.12.005
29. Eisenberg N, Smith CL, Spinrad TL. Effortful control: relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. In: Vohs KD,
Baumeister RF, editors. Handbook of Self-Regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications. Guilford Press; 2011:263–283.
30. Zheng X, Chen H, Wang Z, Xie F, Bao Z. Online violent video games and online aggressive behavior among Chinese college students: the role of
anger rumination and self-control. Aggress Behav. 2021;1–7. doi:10.1002/ab.21967
31. Leung CJ, Cheng L, Yu J, Yiend J, Lee TMC. Six-month longitudinal associations between cognitive functioning and distress among the
community-based elderly in Hong Kong: a cross-lagged panel analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2018;265:77–81. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2018.04.045
32. Zhang R, Chen J, Zhang C, Xu W. Longitudinal association of mindfulness with aggression and non-suicidal self-injury in adolescence: the
mediating role of shame-proneness. Aggress Behav. 2024;50:e22121. doi:10.1002/ab.22121
33. Zhang WX, Wu JF, Kevin J. Revision of the Chinese version of olweus child bullying questionnaire. Psychol Dev Edu. 1999;2:8–12. In Chinese.
doi:10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.1999.02.002
34. Luo Y, Liu Y. The reliability and validity of Chinese version anger rumination scale in college students. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2017;25(4):667–670.
In Chinese. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2017.04.017
35. Tao Y, Niu H, Li Y, et al. Effects of personal relative deprivation on the relationship between anger rumination and aggression during and after the
COVID-19 pandemic lockdown: a longitudinal moderated network approach. J Adolesc. 2023;95(3):596–608. doi:10.1002/JAD.12140
36. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL. High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. J
Personal. 2004;72(2):271–322. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x
37. Tan S, Guo Y. Revision of self-control scale for Chinese college students. Chin J Clin Psychol. 2008;16(5):468–470. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-
3611.2008.05.022
38. Zhou H, Wei X, Jiang H, et al. The link between exposure to violent media, normative beliefs about aggression, self-control, and aggression: a
comparison of traditional and cyberbullying. Aggress Behav. 2022;1–7. doi:10.1002/ab.22057
39. Shapka JD, Maghsoudi R. Examining the validity and reliability of the cyber-aggression and cyber-victimization scale. Comp Human Behav.
2017;69:10–17. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.12.015
40. Xie XL, Zhang H, Ju K, Xiao BW, Liu JS, Jennifer DS. Examining the validity and reliability of the short form of cyberbullying and
cybervictimization (CAV. Scale Chin J Clin Psychol. 2022;30(5):1155–1159. In Chinese. doi:10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2022.05.029
41. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York: Guilford Press;
2013.
42. Podsakoff PM, Mackenzie SB, Lee JY, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and
recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol. 2003;88(5):879–903. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879
43. Juvonen J, Gross EF. Extending the school grounds? Bullying experiences in cyberspace. J School Health. 2008;78:496–505. doi:10.1111/j.1746-
1561.2008.00335.x
44. Segovia-González M, Ramírez-Hurtado JM, Contreras I. Analyzing the risk of being a victim of school bullying. the relevance of students’ self-
perceptions. Child Indic Res. 2023;16:2141–2163. doi:10.1007/s12187-023-10045-x
45. Denson TF. The multiple systems model of angry rumination. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2013;17(2):103–123. doi:10.1177/1088868312467086
46. Hou Y, Li X, Xia L-X. Common mechanisms underlying the effect of angry rumination on reactive and proactive aggression: a moderated
mediation model. J Interpers Violence. 2024;39(5–6):1035–1057. doi:10.1177/08862605231201819
47. Hagger MS, Wood C, Stiff C, Chatzisarantis NLD. Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2010;136
(4):495–525. doi:10.1037/a0019486
48. Dvorak RD, Simons JS. Moderation of resource depletion in the self-control strength model: differing effects of two modes of self-control. Pers Soc
Psychol Bull. 2009;35(5):572. doi:10.1177/0146167208330855
49. Muraven M, Collins RL, Shiffman S, Paty JA. Daily fluctuations in self-control demands and alcohol intake. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19
(2):140–147. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.19.2.140
50. de Ridder DT, Lensveltmulders G, Finkenauer C, Stok FM, Baumeister RF. Taking stock of self-control: a meta-analysis of how trait self-control
relates to a wide range of behaviors. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2012;16(1):76. doi:10.1177/1088868311418749
51. Jensen-Campbell LA, Knack JM, Waldrip AM, Campbell SD. Do big five personality traits associated with self-control influence the regulation of
anger and aggression? J Res Personality. 2007;41(2):403–424. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.05.001
52. Wright MF, Harper BD, Wachs S. The associations between cyberbullying and callous-unemotional traits among adolescents: the moderating effect
of online disinhibition. Pers Individ Dif. 2018;140(1):41–45. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.001
53. Mitchell MA, Maxwell SE. A comparison of the cross-sectional and sequential designs when assessing longitudinal mediation. Multivariate Behav
Res. 2013;48(3):301–339. doi:10.1080/00273171.2013.784696