Sharon Inkelas, Cheryl Zoll - Reduplication - Doubling in Morphology-Cambridge University Press (2005)
Sharon Inkelas, Cheryl Zoll - Reduplication - Doubling in Morphology-Cambridge University Press (2005)
Reduplication
R EDUP L I CAT I ON
DOU BL I N G I N M O RPHO LO G Y
SHA RO N IN K E LA S
University of California, Berkeley
and
CH ERYL Z O L L
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS
Cambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town, Singapore, São Paulo, Delhi
Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9780521114509
A catalogue record for this publication is available from the British Library
Acknowledgments page xi
Table of languages xiii
Abbreviations used in morpheme glosses xxi
1 Introduction 1
1.1 Two approaches to duplication 2
1.2 Morphological Doubling Theory 6
1.2.1 The morphology of reduplication 7
1.2.2 Constructions in morphology 11
1.2.3 Constructional semantics 13
1.2.4 Constructional phonology 16
1.2.5 The phonology of reduplication 18
1.3 Phonological copying 20
1.4 Distinguishing the two types of duplication 22
1.5 Wrapup and outline of book 23
vii
viii Contents
Notes 213
References 225
Index of languages 245
Index of names 247
Index of subjects 251
Acknowledgments
The broad focus of this book has given us the opportunity to benefit from
the expertise of a multitude of linguists whose interest and insightful input
have contributed immeasurably to the construction of a morphological the-
ory of reduplication. Orhan Orgun and Larry Hyman challenged and inspired
us throughout. Juliette Blevins, Andrew Garrett, Michael Kenstowicz, Teresa
McFarland, Anne Pycha, Richard Rhodes, Ronald Sprouse, and Donca Steri-
ade all read the manuscript in various stages and facilitated the development of
our ideas with their penetrating critiques. We owe a debt of gratitude as well
to Terry Crowley, Laura Downing, Danny Fox, Claire Lefebvre, Frank Licht-
enberk, Johanna Nichols, Nick Sherrard, Galen Sibanda, Rajendra Singh, Ken
VanBik, and many others who generously provided us with useful materials
and shared their knowledge about languages, phenomena, and theories once
unfamiliar to us. We also thank our students, east and west, for their stimulating
interest and willingness to participate in the development of MDT. The work
benefited significantly from the feedback of audiences at a number of colloquia
and conferences, including Phonology 2000 (MIT/Harvard), NAPhC (Concor-
dia University, Montreal), the 1999 Linguistics Society of America Meeting,
and the 2002 Graz Reduplication Conference (Graz, Austria), and from discus-
sions at UC Berkeley and MIT during various less formal presentations. Much
of this book was written with the generous support of the Radcliffe Institute for
Advanced Study at Harvard University, which assisted one of the authors with
a fellowship in 2001–2002. We are grateful also to the Linguistics Department
at UC Davis for providing office space and a library card during one crucial
summer. Anne Nesbet, Nancy Katz, Klara Moricz, and David Schneider pro-
vided unending encouragement. Vineeta Chand sustained us with her expert
and cool-headed copy-editing, and Anne Pycha took time in a busy semester
to research the language table. We thank, finally, our families: the children we
xi
xii Acknowledgments
raise on our laps while we type, Jem, Eli, and Lydia, and our spouses, Orhan
Orgun and Eric Sawyer, who remove them at crucial moments for their occa-
sional bath or hot meal, and who provide boundless moral and logistical support.
This book is dedicated to our children, and to Curry Sawyer, whose inspiration,
practical assistance, and unparalleled ability to get things done make everything
possible.
Table of languages
This table of languages is a compilation of information from the sources consulted in this work and the SIL Ethnologue,
available from SIL International at http://www.ethnologue.com.
xxi
xxii Abbreviations used in morpheme glosses
This book concerns itself with a select subtype of repetition, namely gram-
matical doubling or duplication effects within words, illustrated by the forms in
(2). Word-internal reduplication may be partial (a) or total (b, c); it may involve
perfect identity between copies (c) or exhibit imperfect identity (a, b).2
The mechanism of reduplication and manner in which copies can differ from
each other have been a foundational concern in theoretical and descriptive
1
2 Introduction
linguistics over the past twenty-five years. They constitute the central interest
of this book.
attribute to the need for Yoruba syllables to begin with a consonantal onset.
Although Alderete et al. (1999) treat Yoruba gerunds as resulting from mor-
phological reduplication, Akinlabi and Kawu instead view them as an instance
of what Newman (2000) calls “pseudoreduplication” or what Urbanczyk (1998)
calls “non-reduplicative copying.” The data below are taken from Kawu
1998:3:
for example, Marantz & Wiltshire 2000 for a recent overview).5 The example
in (7), using the Warlpiri form for ‘girls’ (Nash 1986:130), illustrates total redu-
plication under the dominant derivational approach taken in the 1980s (Marantz
1982; Clements 1985; Kiparsky 1986; Mester 1986; Steriade 1988). Red, a
skeletal Prosodic Word affix marking the plural, is fleshed out by copying the
base segments and associating the copies by rule to the Red template.
(7) Morphological reduplication by phonological copying
Affixation Copy and Association
Red plural + PWd → Red PWd
kamina
kamina kamina
‘girl’ ‘girls’
not reduplicative (see, for example, Weeda 1992; McCarthy & Prince 1999b and
Chapter 3), but is found even in clearly “syntactic” reduplication constructions
where MS feature duplication is unequivocally at work.
Consider, for example, the phenomenon in Fongbe (Kwa) which Lefebvre
and Brousseau (2002) analyze as syntactic doubling of the verb. Verb dou-
bling occurs in four syntactic constructions: temporal adverbials (9a), causal
adverbials (9b), factives (9c), and predicate clefts (9d). In each case, an extra
copy of the verb appears initially in the verb phrase. The fronted copy of the
verb can be identical to the main verb; crucially, for some speakers, it can also
be truncated to its first syllable (Collins 1994, cited in Lefebvre & Brousseau
2002:505):
(9) a. sı́sɔ́ ∼ sı́ Kɔ́kú sı́sɔ́ tlóló bɔ̀ xὲsı́ ı̀ Bàyı́
tremble Koku tremble as.soon.as and fear get Bayi
‘As soon as Koku trembled, Bayi got frightened’
b. sı́sɔ́ ∼ sı́ Kɔ́kú sı́sɔ́ útú xὲsı́ ı̀ Bàyı́
tremble Koku tremble cause fear get Bayi
‘Because Koku trembled, Bayi got frightened’
c. sı́sɔ́ ∼ sı́ é-è Bàyı́ sı́sɔ́ ɔ́, vέ nú mi
tremble op-res Bayi tremble, def bother for me
‘The fact that Bayi trembled bothered me’
d. sı́sɔ́ ∼ sı́ wὲ, Kɔ́kú sı́sɔ́
tremble it.is Koku tremble
‘It is tremble that Koku did’
In Fongbe, the only difference between the main verb and its fronted copy is
the optional truncation; both copies can be assumed to have phonologically and
semantically identical inputs.
A second likely motivation for the lack of attention to the issue of whether
phonological copying or MS feature duplication underlies reduplication is
simply that so many cases of canonical morphological reduplication appear
amenable to either approach. By way of illustration, consider again the case of
total pluralizing reduplication in Warlpiri, for which a standard phonological
copying analysis was presented in (7). Suppose, instead, that plural formation
in Warlpiri is governed by a morphological construction, such as the one shown
in (10), which dictates double insertion of the singular form of the noun. In
this case, there would be no phonological copying. Such an analysis differs
from the account of Hebrew argued for by Landau only in that it is the mor-
phology rather than the syntax that provides multiple instantiation of identical
features.
6 Introduction
kamina kamina
While the analysis in (10) is logically possible, the prevailing intuition that
word reduplication should be treated as phonological copying (as in (7)) has
precluded the development of a detailed theory of word reduplication based on
MS feature duplication.
This book addresses this asymmetry in previous approaches to morphological
reduplication, surveying a wide range of duplication effects and developing
numerous arguments to support the use of MS feature duplication, formalized
as Morphological Doubling Theory, for morphological reduplication, while
reserving phonological copying as the correct analysis of purely phonologically
driven duplication.
Moravcsik wrote this passage at a time when it was thought that the first type
of reduplication did not exist; she states (p. 305) that no language possesses
a reduplicative construction “which involves the reduplication of a syntactic
constituent regardless of its form . . . in reduplication reference is always made
both to the meaning and to the sound form of the constituent to be reduplicated.”
Similar statements are made on p. 315, fn. 8.
Our subsequent research has revealed some of the missing data that sup-
ports Moravcsik’s original hypothesis that reduplication does not necessarily
involve phonological identity. A number of morphological constructions require
semantic identity, semantic similarity or (in some cases) semantic dissimilarity
between their daughters. Among the cases of this sort, discussed in Chapter 2,
are languages exhibiting “synonym compounding,” in which the two members
of the compound are phonologically distinct, perhaps etymologically distinct
8 Introduction
synonyms (e.g. Khmer peel-weeliə ‘time,’ from Sanskrit peel ‘time’ + Pali
weeliə ‘time’; Ourn & Haiman 2000:485). In the Khmer and Vietnamese
examples below, the meanings of these constructions can be lexicalized but
frequently are the same as the meaning of the individual parts. Page numbers
for Khmer and Vietnamese refer to Ourn and Haiman 2000 and Nguyen 1997,
respectively:
It is argued in Chapter 2 that any theory with the ability to model these
constructions already has the ability to model reduplication and does not need
recourse to extra mechanisms like a Red morpheme or base-reduplication
correspondence. Some related arguments against a morphemic approach to
reduplication can be found in Saperstein 1997.
Another type of case discussed in Chapter 2 is divergent allomorphy, in which
the two copies – “base” and “reduplicant,” to use traditional terminology –
differ in their morphological makeup. Divergent allomorphy provides striking
evidence for the MS feature duplication approach because it clearly shows that
the two copies can have different morphological inputs, as long as they are
semantically matched. Recall that in Hebrew VP-fronting, alluded to in (6), the
two copies of the verb appear in different forms. Chechen (North Caucasian)
likewise illustrates the possibility of divergent allomorphy when a construction
calls for only semantic identity between independent copies. Chechen exhibits
syntactic reduplication to satisfy the requirements of a second position clitic
(Conathan & Good 2000; see also Peterson 2001 on the closely related language
Ingush). As shown in (13), from Conathan and Good (2000:50), chained clauses
are marked by an enclitic particle ʔa, which immediately precedes the inflected,
phrase-final, main verb. The enclitic must be preceded by another element in the
same clause. Two types of constituent may occur before the verb (and enclitic
particle) in the clause: an object (13a), or a deictic proclitic or preverb (13b).
Morphological Doubling Theory 9
The Chechen reduplicant occurs in infinitive form, while the main verb is
inflected. Inflected verbs require a different form of the verb stem from that used
in the infinitive; in some cases the stem allomorphy is clearly suppletive, e.g.
Dala ‘to give’ vs. lwo ‘gives,’ or Dagha ‘to go’ vs. Duedu ‘goes.’ As Conathan
and Good (2000:54) observe, the result is that Chechen can exhibit suppletive
allomorphy differences between base and reduplicant; they cite as one example
the reduplicated verb phrase Dagha ‘a Duedu, based on ‘go.’
What is going on in Chechen is the use of two verbs with (almost) the same
meaning. MS feature duplication allows for divergent allomorphy of this sort,
since there is no requirement that multiple tokens of concurring feature bundles
be expressed identically. Divergent allomorphy in reduplication is, however,
impossible to generate with phonological copying, since the normal base-to-
reduplicant copying process cannot introduce an allomorph into the reduplicant
that is not present in the base. Therefore, evidence that morphological redupli-
cation exhibited divergent allomorphy would provide strong support that MS
feature duplication, rather than phonological copying, is the driving force in
reduplication.
Divergent allomorphy does indeed occur in morphological reduplication.
Consider, for example, a fragment of data from Sye (Central-Eastern Oceanic;
Crowley 1998; 2002d).9 Sye presents the type of morphological divergence in
which reduplicant and base contain different suppletive allomorphs of the same
morpheme. The main points of Sye reduplication are these:
(14) a. Most verb roots in Sye appear in two different shapes: Stem1 and Stem2
b. Each affixation construction selects for one of the two stem shapes
c. Reduplication in morphological contexts calling for Stem1 yields two
copies of Stem1
d. Reduplication in contexts that call for Stem2 surfaces as Stem2-Stem1
10 Introduction
Because the phonological relationship between Stem1 and Stem2 is not fully
predictable, the “reduplicant” in a Sye reduplicated verb cannot always be
described as a phonological copy of the “base,” as phonological theories of
reduplication would require. Rather, in at least some cases the reduplicant and
base consist of different suppletive allomorphs of the same morpheme.
Ndebele (Nguni; Bantu) presents a different kind of divergent allomorphy in
reduplication: the reduplicant contains semantically empty morphs not present
in the base (Downing 1999a; 2001; Sibanda 2004; Hyman, Inkelas & Sibanda
to appear). One such morph is the stem-forming -a (see Chapter 2 for a fuller
discussion). Reduplication, which targets the verb stem and contributes the
meaning that “the action is done for a short while before it stops or is done
from time to time, perhaps not very well” (Sibanda 2004:282), truncates the
first copy of the verb stem to two syllables. When the root is itself disyllabic
or longer, the reduplicant consists of its initial two syllables. The outputs of
reduplication here and throughout are shown with the final vowel in place, the
standard citation form for verb stems. Data are taken from Hyman, Inkelas, and
Sibanda to appear (HIS) and Sibanda 2004 (S):
There are some conditions, however, under which empty morphs can appear
in the reduplicant which are not present, because nothing motivates their pres-
ence, in the base. One condition is when the verb root is monosyllabic. As
Morphological Doubling Theory 11
illustrated by the cases in (18), based on example (6) from Hyman, Inkelas,
and Sibanda to appear (see also Downing 2001; Sibanda 2004:284), subjunc-
tive and perfective suffixes are not in the domain of reduplication; they cannot
be duplicated. The disyllabicity requirement on the reduplicant must be satis-
fied, however, and in Ndebele compels the insertion of the semantically empty
stem-forming morpheme -a:
⎡ Syntax = N ⎤ ⎡ Syntax = N ⎤
⎢Semantics = X ⎥ /z/ ⎢Semantics = 'book' ⎥
⎢ ⎥ ⎢ ⎥ /z/
⎣ Phonology = Y ⎦ ⎣ Phonology = [bυk] ⎦
from verbs (e.g. resi ‘grate coconut,’ re-resi ‘coconut grater’; Lynch & Ross
2002:442). In Ulithian (Micronesian; Lynch 2002b), reduplication derives
intransitive verbs from nouns, e.g. sifu ‘grass skirt,’ sif-sifu ‘wear a grass skirt’;
yaŋi ‘wind,’ yaŋi-yaŋi ‘blow’ (p. 799). The same is true in the Central-Eastern
Oceanic language Niuafo’ou, e.g. maka ‘stone,’ maka-maka ‘to be stony’ (Early
2002:856). In Marquesan (Lynch 2002a), “[s]ome verbs are derived from nouns
by reduplication,” e.g. ivi ‘bone,’ ivi-ivi ‘be thin,’ niho ‘tooth,’ niho-niho ‘be
notched’ (p. 872). In Siroi (Trans-New Guinea; Wells 1979), noun reduplication
yields an intransitive verb meaning ‘act like N’, in the -k- class (all Siroi verb
stems fall into one of four classes), e.g. zon ‘John,’ zon-zon-k-ate ‘John-John-
k-3sg.pres = he is acting (like) John’; ragitap ‘turtle,’ ragitap-ragitap-k-ate
‘he is acting like a turtle,’ gua ‘young child,’ gua-gua-k-ina ‘young child-
young child-k-3s.past = she acted childishly’ (p. 35; hyphenation has been
modified from the original). A similar pattern in which the first copy of the verb
also possesses a class marker connotes pretence, e.g. malmbi-k-et-malmbi-k-et-
ng-ate ‘cry-k-1sg.pres-cry-k-1sg.pres-ng-3sg.pres = he is pretending to
cry’ (p. 36). In Nadrogā (Central-Eastern Oceanic; Geraghty 2002:841), redu-
plication is used “to form intransitives of patient-oriented verbs,” thus vuli ‘[to
be] turned over,’ vuli-vuli ‘turn over’. (Agent-oriented verbs form frequentatives
when reduplicated, e.g. tola-vi-a ‘look at it,’ tola-tola-vi-a ‘look repeatedly at
it.’)
Idiomatic reduplicative constructions are similar to exocentric compounds,
like pick-pocket, blue-hair, etc. The meaning of the whole is not the result
of adding together the meanings of the parts; rather, it must be stipulated as a
property of the construction as a whole. The difference between pick-pocket and
e.g. the Banoni form re-resi ‘coconut grater’ is that the meaning of pick-pocket
is lexicalized, whereas in Banoni there is a semantic generalization over all
instances of this reduplication construction. The instrumental noun semantics
of Banoni verb reduplication is listed, in MDT, as a property of the mother node
of the schematic reduplication construction, as shown below:
⎡ Syntax: V ⎤ ⎡ Syntax: V ⎤
⎢ Semantics: y ⎥ ⎢ Semantics: y ⎥
⎢ ⎥ ⎢ ⎥
⎣Phonology: truncation⎦ ⎣Phonology: identity⎦
16 Introduction
⎡ Syntax: N ⎤
⎢Semantics: ‘coconut grater’⎥
⎢ ⎥
⎣ Phonology: re-resi ⎦
⎡ Syntax: V ⎤ ⎡ Syntax: V ⎤
⎢Semantics: ‘grate coconut’⎥ ⎢Semantics: ‘grate coconut’⎥
⎢ ⎥ ⎢ ⎥
⎣ Phonology: resi → re ⎦ ⎣ Phonology: resi → resi ⎦
general to the language, that drive pural suffix allomorphy; the cophonology of
the compounding construction assigns compound stress.
Cophonologies play a highly important role in nonconcatenative morphol-
ogy, such as truncation, zero-derivation and ablaut constructions. These phe-
nomena have traditionally fallen outside the scope of phrase-structure rules or
subcategorization frames (see, for example, Lieber 1980), yet they are centrally
important in the morphology of many languages. Constructions for trunca-
tion, zero-derivation and ablaut are illustrated below. English nickname for-
mation exemplifies truncation, in (25a), whose cophonology (k ) deletes all
but the initial syllable (loosely speaking) of the input name. Zero-derivation is
exemplified in (25b) by English noun-to-verb conversion of the sort Kiparsky
(1982c) located in Level 1 of the morphology (e.g. pérmit (n.), permı́t (v.)).
Since English noun-to-verb conversion potentially results in stress shift, stress
assignment must be part of Cophonology q . Ablaut is illustrated by the German
umlaut-only plural formation (e.g. Vater ‘father.sg’, Väter ‘father.pl’). In
German, the relevant cophonology (r ) requires back stressed vowels to front:
(26) Noun
⎡ pluralization in English
⎤⎡ Example: ⎤
Syntax = N Syntax = N
⎣ Semantics = plural of X ⎦ ⎣ Semantics = ‘books’ ⎦
Phonology = 2 (Px , /z/) Phonology = 2 (/bυk/, /z/) = [bυks]
⎡ | ⎤ ⎡ | ⎤
Syntax = N Syntax = N
⎣Semantics = Semx ⎦ ⎣Semantics = ‘book’ ⎦
Phonology = Px X
Phonology = /bυk/ X
The cophonology of this construction adds the plural /z/ ending as well as
performing voicing assimilation.
(27) [zzz]
⇐ Cophonology Z
[xxx] [yyy]
Cophonology X ⇒ | | ⇐ Cophonology Y
/Stemi/ /Stemi/
truncating one, and outputs a CVC syllable. The second daughter is subject to an
identity-preserving cophonology whose output matches its input. Together the
two pieces are subject to the cophonology of the mother node, which imposes
gemination of the consonant cluster at the juncture between the two copies.
[kir] [kiraa]
Truncates ⇒ | | ⇐ Preserves identity
/kira/i /kira/i
‘call’ ‘call’
like a single consonant or vowel, and in part because the doubling has a purely
phonological purpose, rather than being associated with a change in meaning.
Restricting the use of phonological constituent copying to cases motivated by
phonological necessity, resonates with recent arguments by Gafos (1998b),
Hendricks (1999) and, to some extent, Zuraw (2002) that a theory should pro-
vide only one type of analysis to single-segment copying within a word, not
two. It is argued in this work that the proper analysis of such cases is phonologi-
cal copying, not morphological reduplication; although Gafos and Zuraw adopt
the seemingly opposite view that the unified analysis should be reduplication
(though without templates), their analyses, which use reduplicative morphemes
(Gafos) or the constraint reduplicate (Zuraw), are conceptually more con-
sistent with phonological copying or assimilation than with morphological
reduplication.
The Hausa and Yoruba onset copying cases discussed above, for example,
are unequivocal examples of phonological copying. In both Hausa and Yoruba,
the duplication of the consonant is driven purely phonologically, by the need for
a syllable onset. Spokane (Interior Salish) provides another such example, dif-
fering from Hausa and Yoruba in that phonological copying occurs in only one
allomorph of an otherwise nonreduplicative affix. The repetitive form of a verb
is formed by infixing /e/ into what would (due to unstressed schwa deletion)
be an initial consonant cluster (30a). For stems beginning with only a single
consonant, however, the stem-initial consonant is doubled, with /e/ appear-
ing between the two copies (30b) (Black 1996:210ff., Bates & Carlson 1998:
655).
result in identical inputs. Chapters 3 to 6 all discuss, in various ways, the evi-
dence that morphological reduplication does not presuppose or enforce phono-
logical identity between its daughters. Chapter 7 presents an argument that
phonological copying not only presupposes identity but actively enforces it,
solidly distinguishing the two types of process.
The various chapters of this book demonstrate that MDT is more descriptively
adequate and at the same time more constrained in its predictions than existing
phonological approaches to reduplication. Chapter 2 presents the basic morpho-
logical evidence for MDT, focusing on data new to the theoretical literature on
reduplication that demonstrate the need for a theory in which the two daughters
in a reduplication construction are morphologically independent. Chapters 3,
4, and 5 closely investigate the phonology of reduplication. Chapter 3 covers
languages which motivate the phonological independence of the two daughters
and begins to make the case that reduplication-specific phonology is part of a
general phenomenon of morphologically conditioned phonology. This broader
context reveals that Coerced Identity theories are too narrow; by giving redu-
plicative phonology a special status, such theories are unable to account for the
24 Introduction
25
26 Evidence for morphological doubling
This chapter shows that a theory that hews to the theses in (2) is the right way
to approach reduplication; the main insights of MDT correspond to the data
better than do the main insights of phonological copying theories like BRCT.
The additions to phonological theory which BRCT embodies are, therefore,
unnecessary.
We begin with the demonstration that there are clear cases of reduplica-
tion in which the essential identity between the copies is semantic, rather than
phonological. These cases support the Thesis of Semantic Identity.
It is common for the two copies in reduplication to differ phonologically.
This asymmetry could have two causes: (a) the copies are identical in input but
differ in output because of normal or special reduplicative phonology, or (b)
the copies are different in input. These two scenarios are contrasted, below:
Any existing theory – MDT, Full Copy, BRCT – can describe scenario (4a).
Scenario (4b) is the one that is unique to MDT: base and reduplicant have
Morphological targets: affix reduplication 27
This construction is semantically iconic: the affix appears twice, and its
semantic contribution is reflected twice (by increasing the degree of absence of
the property designated by the noun with which the affix combines), and could
even be represented as multiple affixation rather than as reduplication per se.
In the following cases of affix reduplication, however, the meaning of redupli-
cation is not at all related to the meanings of the individual affixes being doubled.
Rather, the meaning is an arbitrary property of the reduplication construction
itself.
28 Evidence for morphological doubling
shows, however, that the relevant distinction between reduplicated roots and
reduplicated affixes is not phonological, but morphological. In Dyirbal, Amele,
and the other languages discussed in §2.1, reduplicated affixes can be of any
size, as can reduplicated roots.6
Although somewhat of a side issue, it is worth noting that the existence of
affix doubling, especially in conjunction with root doubling, appears to support
theories of morphology which treat affixes as morphological constituents. In
realizational theories in which affixed stems lack internal constituent structure,
e.g. the A-morphous theory of morphology developed by Anderson (1992), it
is difficult to express affix doubling and root doubling as instances of the same
phenomenon.
generally. There is, in fact, nothing unique morphologically about the empty
morphs that occur in reduplication. The factors that motivate the presence of
empty morphs in reduplication are the same factors that motivate their presence
in nonreduplicative constructions. These factors can be either phonological or
morphological.
Many morphological constructions require the presence of a morphological
formative which has no clearly isolable meaning but plays a structural role. Final
vowels in Bantu are a good example. Verb stems in many Bantu languages are
required to end in one of a small set of “Final Vowel” (FV) suffixes, so called
because they close off a verb stem to further suffixation. Some FV suffixes do
make a semantic contribution; for example, in Ndebele, the FV suffix -e marks
subjunctive mood, and the FV suffix -ile markes perfective aspect (12). But
there is usually one FV suffix which is semantically vacuous, serving only the
structural role of closing off the verb. In Ndebele the FV suffix -a meets this
description. It is simply the “elsewhere” FV (see, e.g., Sibanda 2004):
Wise (1986) proposes empty morph status for the /t/ formative which
appears in the PreAndine languages of Peru (which include the Campa,
Amuesha, and Piro-Apurinã languages). Although in Axininca Campa and other
Campa languages the distribution of /t/ appears wholly governed by phonology
(Wise 1986:579; see also, for example, Payne 1981; Spring 1990; McCarthy &
Prince 1993), Wise cites examples from Piro and Amuesha (p. 578) where the
cognate formative “is not necessary on phonological grounds; it occurs simply
because certain verb roots and suffixes . . . cannot occur word or stem finally
and no other potentially closing suffix . . . occurs” (p. 581). In this description,
/t/ has a function similar to the final vowel in Bantu; it is a semantically empty
element which serves to close off a stem morphologically.
The elements Hockett (1954) termed “markers” of constructions are also pro-
totypical empty morphs. Hockett’s example was the English conjunction and,
but many examples occur in morphology proper. Aronoff (1994) points to theme
vowels in European languages, particularly those of Latin, as being “[a]mong
the best-known examples of empty morphs” (p. 45); the bindungs-elements
commonly found in compounding constructions constitute another. English
has this to only a limited degree, e.g. huntsman, batsman, swordsman, clans-
man; but many compounding constructions in other languages illustrate the phe-
nomenon more clearly, e.g. Serbo-Croatian vod-o-paad ‘water-marker-fall =
Morphotactic asymmetries: empty morphs 33
To take one other example, the Athapaskan languages Slave and Navajo use
(h)e- (Rice 1989:149, 132–35, 426) and yi- (McDonough 1990:138ff.), respec-
tively, to augment verb roots which lack prefixes. According to McDonough,
verb prefixes belong to a constituent (the I-stem) which is obligatory in every
verb and which cannot be empty.9
34 Evidence for morphological doubling
The analysis of empty morphs adopted here is as follows. Some empty morphs
are associated with particular constructions, in which they are obligatory. These
are the kind seen in §2.2.1. Others, however, are listed in the lexicon without
being mentioned in any particular constructions; these include the phonolog-
ically beneficial morphs in §2.2.2. Their distribution in inputs is regulated by
the grammar, in a way that we model here by building on existing proposals
in Optimality Theory for determining the lexical representation of morphemes
(Lexicon Optimization) and, in cases of allomorphy, for selecting among the
different lexical allomorphs of a morpheme.10 Lexicon Optimization (Prince &
Smolensky 1993; see also Inkelas 1995; Itô, Mester & Padgett 1995; Yip 1996,
among others) is a deterministic method for establishing underlying repre-
sentations by considering all of the possible inputs that would generate a given
output, and selecting the most harmonic. In some cases, more than one underly-
ing allomorph for a given morpheme must be memorized, in a form determined
by Lexicon Optimization. We follow Dolbey 1996; Kager 1996; Sprouse 1997;
Steriade 1997; 1999; Sprouse in preparation in assuming that these listed allo-
morphs compete for position in words calling for the semantic content that they
share. The grammar selects that allomorph which phonologically optimizes the
word containing it.
Combining these two proposals yields the following approach to determin-
ing morphological inputs: the input candidates considered for a word with
meaning (M) are those sets of lexically listed morphological formatives whose
meanings add up to M. These combinations can differ from each other in sev-
eral ways. They can vary by lexical allomorph (as in the cases considered by
Dolbey and Kager); they can vary by including, or not including, semanti-
cally empty morphemes, the case in question here. The presence or absence of
a semantically empty morph has no effect on the meaning (M) of any given
candidate.
The tableaus in (14) evaluate competing possible morphological inputs for
the Chichêwa words meaning ‘eat (imperative)’ and ‘explain (imperative).’
These tableaus mirror those used for Lexicon Optimization by Inkelas 1995; Itô,
Mester & Padgett 1995 and for allomorph selection by Dolbey 1996; Sprouse
1997; in preparation. In each case the optimal input is the one judged most har-
monic by standards of output well-formedness and input–output faithfulness;
each input is compared, separately, to the full range of possible output strings. In
the first tableau, for ‘eat (imperative),’ the competing inputs are /dy-/, dy-a/, and
/i-dy-a/. By virtue of containing the same root, /dy/, all mean ‘eat’; the absence
of other semantically contentful morphemes results (by means that cannot be
fully explored here) in the default interpretation of imperative. The input /dy-a/
Morphotactic asymmetries: empty morphs 35
has one empty morph; the input /i-dy-a/ has two. Each input is compared with
three reasonable outputs, [dy], [dya], [idya], which are evaluated for well-
formedness. Constraints on syllabification (not formalized here, but abbreviated
as syll) eliminate the output [dy] from consideration, as it is not a well-formed
syllable. Disyll favors the disyllabic output [idya], while *struc (arbitrar-
ily evaluated in terms of syllable count) favors the more compact outputs.
IO-Faith favors those input–output pairs which are faithful over those which
exhibit epenthesis or deletion. Because Disyll outranks *struc, the output
[idya] is favored over the other two outputs; because IO-Faith is high-ranked,
the input /i-dy-a/ is optimal for the output [idya], and this input–output pairing
is therefore selected as optimal overall. The presence of empty morphs in the
input for ‘eat (imperative)’ is optimal because the morphs contribute to the
well-formedness of the output without disrupting faithfulness (as epenthesis,
for example, would). The second tableau, for ‘explain (imperative),’ exhibits a
parallel set of candidate inputs and outputs. The difference between ‘eat’ and
‘explain’ is phonological size; the root – fotokozer- is already polysyllabic,
meaning that Disyll is satisfied by all of the input–output pairs under consid-
eration. The input /i-fotokozer-a/, with two empty morphs, violates *struc to
a greater degree than does the input without the /i-/ empty morph, which wins
the overall competition.
(14)
input output
candidates candidates
a. i. /dy-/ [dy] *! * *
ii. [dya] *! * *
b. i. /dy-a/ [dy] *! * * *
ii. [dya] *! *
iii. [idya] *! **
ii. [dya] *! * *
E iii. [idya] **
36 Evidence for morphological doubling
input output
candidates candidates
[fotokozera] *! *****
[ifotokozera] *! ******
[ifotokozera] ******!
From here forward we will assume that an approach of this kind regulates
the distribution of empty morphs in inputs, without providing similarly lengthy
justifications for every case.
the linker -ep, e.g. akemir-em ‘is getting up’ vs. akemir-ep-ir-em ‘keeps getting
up,’ while attenuative reduplication uses the linker -elp, e.g. itir-em ‘thinking’
vs. it-elp-itir-em ‘half-thinking’ (Breen & Pensalfini 1999:6–7). In Jaqaru, a
Jaqi language of Peru (Hardman 2000), two total reduplication constructions
are distinguished only by the linker consonant separating the two copies. Inten-
sive adjective reduplication separates the two copies with the palatal glide y;
intensive verbs use the palatal affricate linker ch. Hardman presents the fol-
lowing minimal pair, based on t’usqi ‘dust’: t’usqi-y-t’usqi ‘very like smoke’
(the intensive adjective) vs. t’usqi-ch-t’usqi ‘to be causing a lot of dust’ (the
intensive verb) (p. 54). For some additional examples of linker constructions,
see Moravcsik 1978. Linker morphs may be assumed to be semantically empty
when there is no positive evidence that they, as opposed to the reduplication
they co-occur with, distinctively contribute any specific meaning to the con-
struction. What is clear is that they are concomitants of reduplication whose
presence could not be predicted based on other knowledge of the language.
The constituent structure of reduplication constructions with linker morphs
is not always determinate. If the so-called “linker” forms a morphological con-
stituent with one of the daughters (15b), then its presence disrupts phonological
identity. If, on the other hand, the linker is a third daughter of the construction
(15a), then identity is not disturbed.
Only in the presence of evidence for a structure like (15b) (or its mirror image)
do linker morphs support the claims of MDT. Unfortunately, the morphological
constituency of linker morphs is difficult to determine in many cases; a similar
problem is presented by bindungs-elements in compounding. The claims of
MDT are more clearly illustrated by cases in which the empty morph unam-
biguously belongs to one daughter or the other.
In the Mon-Khmer language Pacoh, for example, an empty morph can
occur, under certain conditions, in the base of reduplication. Verb reduplication
expresses the meaning of simulation; reduplicating ‘laugh’ yields a meaning of
‘pretend to laugh’ (Watson 1966:95). Both copies – base and reduplicant – are
subject to prosodic size conditions. The second copy must be monosyllabic;
the first must be disyllabic. If the input root is disyllabic, then the first copy is
intact, while the second is truncated to the final (main) syllable of the input verb
38 Evidence for morphological doubling
(16a). If the input root is monosyllabic, then the second copy is intact, while
the first is augmented with the semantically empty morph qâN- (16b). All of
the reduplicated forms cited in (16) are preceded by the verb táq ‘to do’:
(16) a. kacháng ‘to laugh’ táq kacháng-cháng ‘to pretend to laugh’
qaqay ‘sick’ táq qaqay-qay ‘to act sick’
b. cha ‘to eat’ táq qân-cha-cha ‘to pretend to eat’
bı́q ‘to sleep’ táq qâm-bı́q-bı́q ‘to pretend to sleep’
pôk ‘to go’ táq qâm-pôk-pôk ‘to pretend to go’
We may assume, building on the discussion in §2.2.2, that the Pacoh lexicon
includes the empty morph qâN-. Though normally absent from inputs because
its presence would gratuitously violate *struc, qâN is available to inputs that
would not otherwise produce stems meeting the prosodic minimality condi-
tions. It is needed in the input to the first copy in reduplication to satisfy the
disyllabicity requirement; it is not needed in the second copy.
(17) [qâmpôkpôk] [‘pretend to go’] ⇐ Phonological output of mother
On this analysis of Pacoh, the inputs of the daughters can differ morphotacti-
cally but are still identical semantically. Cases of this sort, in which the base is
more complex than the reduplicant, do not necessarily distinguish among theo-
ries of reduplication; however, cases in which the reduplicant is more complex
than the base provide strong support for MDT.
Empty morphs in the reduplicant play a significant role in Bantu reduplica-
tion, as discussed, for example, in the significant bodies of insightful work
on Kinande (Mutaka & Hyman 1990; Downing 1998b; 2000a), Chichêwa
(Hyman & Mtenje 1999), Siswati and Kikerewe (Downing 1997a; b; 1999a;
c). Here the focus will be on Ndebele, drawing on work by Downing 1999a;
2001, Sibanda 2004, and Hyman, Inkelas & Sibanda to appear. Ndebele has
two empty morphs: yi, which augments subminimal words, and -a, which is
the default final suffix in verb stems (see below). Both also aid in achieving
reduplicant disyllabicity in verb stem reduplication.
As noted above, Ndebele exhibits a common Bantu verb stem reduplication
pattern: the first copy of the verb stem is restricted to the CVCV prosodic shape,
while the second is not phonologically restricted. Standard practice is to refer
Morphotactic asymmetries: empty morphs 39
to the first copy as the “reduplicant” and the second as the “base”; of course,
in MDT the two copies have equal morphological status. Reduplication adds
the meaning of ‘do here and there, a little bit.’ As shown below, a cursory look
at the phenomenon suggests that the reduplicant consists simply of the initial
CVCV portion of the base. Verbs are shown in the infinitive, marked by the
prefix uku-:
(18) Unreduplicated stem Reduplicated counterpart
For the Dstem meaning ‘eat,’ based on the consonantal root dl-, the optimal
input for the reduplicant is /dl-a-yi/, which can faithfully achieve a disyllabic
output; the optimal input for the base is /dl-/, which is both faithful and struc-
turally minimal (since no size conditions constrain it).
Morphotactic asymmetries: empty morphs 41
Alderete et al. (1999) have argued for treating Melodic Overwriting as affixa-
tion; in MDT, the affix in question is generated within one of the copies, the one
traditionally termed the reduplicant. The affix is a semantically empty morph
required by the construction, parallel to the empty linker morphs discussed in
§2.2.1 except in the one respect that it supplants, instead of coexisting with,
input material. (Thus, in retrospect, the Ndebele data discussed in §2.2.1 could
be analyzed in terms of Melodic Overwriting, insofar as the empty -yi and -a in
the reduplicant potentially supplant input affixes.) Based on a proposal made
for comparable data in Inkelas 1998, we assume that the competition between
empty affix and input material is driven by faithfulness: the output must be
faithful prosodically, e.g. in syllable count, to the input size of the base of affix-
ation. In a case like Vietnamese càphê+càph-i ê´ c, the input base of the affixed
copy is càphê; faithfulness to its syllable count forces the suffix i ê´ c to replace ê:
(25) càphêcàphiêc
subpattern of reduplication replaces the final V with /u/ in the first copy and
final V with /e/ in the second copy: this is Melodic Overwriting in both copies
(data from Haiman 1980:126):
Sticking with the affixation approach to Melodic Overwriting, the Hua redu-
plication construction can be analyzed as calling for two different stem types:
a u-stem, formed by suffixing empy -u, and an e-stem, formed by replacing the
final stem vowel with -e.13
[kvek-u]u-stem [kvek-e]e-stem
The daughters of the Hua reduplication construction are formed by the stem-
forming constructions illustrated below.
[ ][F] u [ ][F] e
[kveku] [kveke]
signifies plurality. According to Wells (1979:37), “[t]he infix -g- replaces the
central consonant in two-syllable words and is added in one-syllable words,”
as shown below.14 The digraph “ng” represents the prenasalized stop [ŋg]:
(29) a. tango maye → tango mage-mage
‘man’ ‘good’
‘a mature man’ ‘mature men’
b. tango sungo → tango sugo-sugo
‘man’ ‘big’
‘a ruler’ ‘rulers’
c. tango kuen → tango kugen-kugen
‘man’ ‘tall’
‘a tall man’ ‘tall men’
2.3.1.1 Kawaiisu
Root allomorphy interacts with reduplication in Kawaiisu (Numic, Uto-
Aztecan; Zigmond, Booth & Munro 1990). Kawaiisu retains the remnants of
historic alternations, primarily between p∼v, p∼b, t∼r, t∼d, k∼g, kw∼gw,
which show up synchronically as allomorphy, e.g. for ‘head,’ toci ∼ roci, or
for ‘taste,’ kama ∼ gama. Only some roots show allomorphy. This lack of pre-
dictability, coupled with the fact that for roots in /p/ and /t/ the nature of the
allomorphy is unpredictable, requires the allomorphy to be treated as lexically
suppletive.
For those roots exhibiting allomorphy, the voiced-initial allomorph is used
in second position in compounding (32a) and in repetitive/inceptive aspectual
reduplication (32b) (Zigmond, Booth & Munro 1990:8, 97):
(32) a. Compounding
i. kama ∼ gama ‘taste (intr.)’ ʔowa-gama- 80, 211
‘to taste salty’
ii. kar - ∼ gar - ‘sit’ kwinuur -gar-d 221
(name of mountain
near Tehachapi)
cf. kwinuur -d 221
‘to prepare yucca’
iii. toci ∼ roci ‘head’ c ga-roci 199
‘rough-head = tangle-
haired’
b. Reduplication
i. kaʔa ∼ gaʔa ‘eat’ ka-gaʔa-na=ina ʔ vi 8, 97
‘he’s starting to eat now’
ii. tono ∼ dono ‘hit, pierce, etc.’ to-dono-kwee-d =ina 97, 284
‘he stabbed him repeatedly’
iii. kiya ∼ giya ‘play, laugh’ ki-giya 214
iv. tahna ∼ rahna ‘put down/away’ ta-rahna 272
v. t niya ∼ d niya ‘to tell’ t-dniya 280
The fact that the two daughters in reduplication can consist of different lexi-
cally listed allomorphs (potentially truncated) of the same morpheme strongly
supports the prediction of MDT that the essential identity in reduplication is
semantic but not phonological. No phonological copying theory of reduplica-
tion can account for this sort of effect.
The productivity of this particular construction in Kawaiisu is somewhat
questionable; not all roots exhibiting allomorphy in general show different
allomorphs in reduplication. However, Kawaiisu is by no means the only
example of its kind.
Synonym and antonym constructions 49
Similar data is reported for Apma, spoken on the same island (Pentecost) as
Raga; Walsh (1982:238) cites the following examples of reduplicated roots in
Stem1 and Stem2 contexts:
reduplicated; both halves are identical. The reduplicated verb root occurs in a
context where Stem1 is called for. In (b), however, where the future prefix a-
calls for a Stem2 base, the first copy of the root assumes its Stem2 shape, while
the second is Stem1 (Crowley 1991:198):
In the cases seen thus far, root allomorphy is generally phonologically trans-
parent. In Nāti, for example, Stem2 can easily be derived phonologically from
Stem1 through the addition of an initial nasal element, which in turn stops the
initial portion of a following continuant. Different analyses have been given
to phonologically analyzable mutations of this kind in the literature; see, e.g.,
the extensive literature on consonant mutation in Celtic and in West African
languages (Fula [Arnott 1970], Mende [Innes 1971; Conteh, Cowper & Rice
1985], Sereer [McLaughlin 2000], and many others). A morphological analy-
sis would attribute the nasal to a semantically bleached morpheme occurring in
the selected set of morphological contexts; alternatively, a more phonological
approach would posit a stem-initial nasalization rule triggered in the same set
of contexts. This issue need not be resolved here, since languages like Nāti do
not provide a test case for MDT in any event. Since the modification is derived
by the grammar, any theory of reduplication could give a cyclic account of the
mutation effects, on the assumption that the mutation-triggering prefix com-
bines with the output of verb root reduplication. As with any other stem in
a mutation context, only the consonant immediately following the prefix is
affected.
Although the languages in (33) do not distinguish MDT from other theories,
the languages that do are only a small step away. Recall that the mutations which
are apparently productive and clearly phonologically general in the languages
in (33) derive historically from prefix-root fusion. Any language in which the
reflexes of that fusion have been muddied by reanalysis or sound change will
not yield to a cyclic anlysis in which reduplication logically precedes mutation.
When a reduplicated verb root occurs in a mutation context, such languages
will require the direct selection, from the lexicon, of different lexically listed
allomorphs of the verb root in question.
52 Evidence for morphological doubling
The first such case to be examined here is Sye, which exhibits two-fold
allomorphy but has lost the phonological generality of the mutation process.
We then turn to another set of Vanuatu languages, Paamese and Southeast
Ambrym, whose more complex verb root allomorphy also precludes a simple
phonological account.
2.3.1.2.1 Sye Sye, spoken on the island of Erromango, resembles other lan-
guages of Southern and Central Vanuatu in exhibiting both verb root allomorphy
and reduplication. The discussion of Sye in this section draws heavily on the
detailed description and insightful analysis of Crowley 1998; 2002d. Verb root
reduplication, which is total, indicates that “an action takes place in a vari-
ety of locations at once, e.g. avan‘walk’, avan-avan ‘walk all over’” (Crowley
2002d:714). Sye has two series of verb allomorphs, termed here Stem1 and
Stem2. Stem2 forms appear in a collection of seemingly unrelated morpholog-
ical environments (e.g. in verbs inflected for present or future tense, or bearing
conditional or past habitual prefixes; Crowley 2002d:704). Stem1 forms appear
elsewhere and can be understood as the default stem type. The distribution of
stem alternants is illustrated in (37) for the verb ‘provoke’. Its Stem1 alternant,
arinova, is given in (a); in examples (b) and (c), ‘provoke’ co-occurs with future
tense prefixes, and thus appears in its Stem2 form, narinowa. Data are from
Crowley 2002d: 711:
(37) a. etw-arinowa-g
2sg.imp.neg-provoke1- 1sg ‘Don’t provoke me!’
b. co-narinowa-nt
3sg.fut-provoke2- 1pl.incl ‘(S)he will provoke us’
c. kokwo-narinowa-nd
1inc.du.fut-provoke2- 3pl ‘We will provoke them’
The verb ‘provoke’ is an example of what Crowley calls a weak verb. A weak
verb root exhibits a completely transparent relationship between its Stem1 and
Stem2 allomorphs: Stem2 consists of Stem1 with the addition of an initial nasal.
Data are taken from Crowley 2002d:
Strong verbs, on the other hand, exhibit a much less transparent relationship
between their allomorphs. While historically the Stem2 verb alternants appear
Synonym and antonym constructions 53
Comparison of evcah → ampcah and evtit → avtit, for example, shows a lack
of complete phonological regularity in the mapping between root allomorphs.
Even where the mapping is systematic, e.g. replacement of absolute initial o
with a, but prefixation of am to consonant-initial words, there is no unitary
process that can be appealed to of the sort that makes a phonological account
of weak roots possible. At least some Sye root alternants must be lexically
listed; we make the simplifying assumption here, adopting Zuraw’s (2000)
approach to patterned exceptions, that the root alternants are all listed, although
the argument would not change if the phonology of Sye were allowed to derive
some of the allomorphy. The lexicon provides Stem1 and Stem2 alternants of
each root; affixal constructions specify the stem type they combine with. As
seen in (39), future tense prefixes select for a stem of type Stem2; the second
person singular negative imperative prefix selects for a stem of type Stem1
(or simply does not specify a stem type, Stem1 being the default).
We turn next to reduplication, an arena in which Sye patterns much like the
other Vanuatu languages seen thus far. In those morphological contexts calling
for Stem1, a reduplicated verb consists of two copies of Stem1. In contexts
54 Evidence for morphological doubling
As these Sye data show, reduplicants are not always phonological copies of
their bases, as phonological theories of reduplication would require. What is
instead occurring in (37) is semantic agreement between suppletive allomorphs:
it is morphological doubling, not phonological copying.
(41)
Class Stem1 Stem2 Stem3 Stem4 Stem5
I t- t- d-
II x- x- g-
III Ø- x- m- v- g-
IV h- h- m- v- g-
V h- x- m- v- g-
VI h- h- m- v- b-
VII h- v- m- v- b-
VIII v- v- b-
Synonym and antonym constructions 55
Nothing in the overt segmental phonology of the prefixes could explain the
root allomorphy. Class III, whose Stem1 forms are apparently vowel-initial,
suggests a morphological account on which the basic pattern is to prefix x- to
Stem1 to form Stem2, prefix m- to form Stem3, prefix v- to form Stem4, and
prefix g- to form Stem5. However, the existence of four different patterns for
h-initial roots (classes IV-VIII) makes it impossible to derive Stem2 by any
regular means from Stem1.
Though Crowley does not provide any examples of reduplication, he does
state that Stem1, the default, is used “as the second of a pair of reduplicated
syllables,” strongly suggesting that Ambrym behaves like Sye and the other
languages discussed above in potentially using different allomorphs of the same
verb root in reduplication, the first conforming to the requirements of the higher
affix and the second consisting of the default stem allomorph. Parker (1968)
mentions reduplication only briefly, indicating that reduplication copies the first
CV of the stem (e.g. ka-kal ‘scratch,’ pi-pili ‘be red,’ mu-mun ‘drink,’ etc.). He
does not explicitly address the question of what happens when a reduplicated
verb occurs in a mutation environment (which, if our understanding is correct, he
calls “Aorist”). However, Parker does provide one suggestive example, namely
the reduplicated verb go-xoles ‘he exchanges.’ The first (truncated to CV) copy
of the root begins with g, while the second begins with x. This alternation would
put ‘exchange’ into Stem class V in the table in (41).
For Paamese, which has a similar system, we are fortunate to have more data.
The following discussion is based on Crowley 1982; 1991. The description in
Crowley 1991, covering all dialects, revises that of Crowley 1982, primarily
covering a southern dialect, in a number of minor respects, including the number
of verb stem classes posited. Presented below is the classification of verbs from
Crowley 1991:190, based on the types of allomorphy they show across different
56 Evidence for morphological doubling
A B C D
(N/C/S) (N/C=S)
I t- t- r- r-/d-/d- r-/d-
II t- t- r- mur-/d-/d- mur-/d-
III r- r- r- mur-/d-/d- mur-/r-
IV k- k- k- k-/g-/g- k-/g-
V Ø- k- k- k-/g-/g- k-/g-
VI Ø- k- k- k-/g-/ŋ- k-/ŋ-
VII Ø- k- k- muk-/g-/ŋ- k-/ŋ-
VIII h- h- v- v- h-
IX Ø- Ø- mu- mu- mu-/Ø-
X C- C- C- muC-/C-/C- C-
XI C- C- C- C- C-
its Primary, or default, form, i.e. Stem1. Examples taken from Crowley 1982
(C82) and Crowley 1991 (C91) illustrate these generalizations, below:
construction stipulates only that the stem type of the first daughter determines
the stem type of the construction as a whole:
The presumed reason that Stem4, rather than Stem1, appears as the second
daughter is that it is phonotactically superior, in possessing an onset. Crowley
(1982) points out (p. 126) that were a form like uasi to reduplicate as u-uasi,
“the regular phonological rules of the language would eliminate all trace of
[reduplication] having taken place.” The fact that Onset can outrank the pref-
erence for Stem1 supports the claim that the usual use of Stem1 form in the
second daughter position derives from its status as the default stem. In the terms
of Optimality Theory, the grammar would state a general preference for Stem1
allomorphs, which is overridable (a) by the specific requirements of higher
affixes or constructions or (b) by phonotactic constraints such as onset, as
illustrated schematically below:
Synonym and antonym constructions 59
2.3.1.3 Wrapup
This section has verified one of the most striking and unique predictions of
MDT, namely that reduplication constructions are able to use distinct allo-
morphs of the same morpheme in the two daughters. This prediction follows
from the thesis of MDT that the two copies in reduplication have independent
inputs. We turn in the next section to a larger-scale instantiation of this same
general phenomenon.
Abbi (1991:24) provides similar examples in which the first word is native and
the second comes from Urdu.
Synonym constructions are quite pervasive (for general discussion see Abbi
1991:24 and, especially, Ourn & Haiman 2000). To cite just two examples
outside of Hindi, Li and Thompson (1981:68ff.) describe Chinese as having
verbal compounds in which “[t]he two verbs that constitute a parallel verb
either are synonymous or signal the same type of predicative notions,” e.g. gòu
măi ‘buy-buy = buy,’ měi-lı̀ ‘beautiful-beautiful = beautiful.’ Ourn and Haiman
(2000) discuss a similar construction in Vietnamese, examples of which, taken
from Nguyen 1997, are provided below:
Foreshadowing the theory of MDT, Singh argues for treating both constructions
as reduplication:
What Singh is suggesting here appears to be the same idea that MDT is
built on: reduplication can (or according to MDT must) double a morpho-
logically abstract entity, one defined essentially by semantic properties. The
Synonym and antonym constructions 61
MDT schemas for the two structurally similar constructions in (48) and (50)
are given below:
(51)
Hindi synonym construction (= (48)) Hindi echo word construction (= (50))
⎡ Syntax = N ⎤ ⎡ Syntax = N ⎤
⎢Semantics = ‘x etc.’ ⎢Semantics = ‘x etc.’⎥
⎣ ⎦ ⎣ ⎦
2.3.3.1 Khmer
Khmer is rife with coordinate or symmetrical compounds, including what
Ourn and Haiman call “near-synonym” compounds like the following (Ourn &
Haiman 2000:485, 500–502) (see also Gorgoniev 1976:318). With respect to
semantics, Ourn and Haiman state that, “They seem to be simply semantically
redundant and rhythmically elaborated ways of saying what could be said by
a single word” (p. 491). Ourn and Haiman clearly characterize these synonym
and near-synonym constructions as reduplication.
(53) a. cah + tum ‘village elder’ 485
‘old’ ‘mature’
b chap + rɔhah ‘fast’ 485
‘quick’ ‘fast’
c. clooh + prɑkaek ‘quarrel’ 485
‘squabble’ ‘argue’
d. ʔaar + kɑmbaŋ ‘secret’ 500
‘secret’ ‘secret’
e. cbah + prakɑt ‘exact’ 500
‘exact’ ‘exact’
f. kee + mɔrdɔk ‘legacy’ 501
‘heritage’ ‘heritage’
Synonym and antonym constructions 63
Ourn and Haiman write that “[s]o strong is this tendency to compound near-
synonyms that sometimes a verb will be conjoined with a doublet” (p. 485).
The example in (54a), below, is one in which the two nouns are semantically
identical but have different etymological origins, one coming from Sanskrit
and the other from Pali. The example in (54b) exhibits not etymological but
morphological divergence; the second daughter is a simple verb meaning ‘step,’
while the first is a morphological construction, termed by Ourn and Haiman a
“cognate accusative,” literally meaning ‘take a step.’
Khmer also has the next point on the scale, constructions in which both
daughters are what Ourn and Haiman call “typical or representative members
of a group,” as follows:
Ourn and Haiman note that some of the (near)-synonym compounds are
semantically opaque, meaning (somewhat) exocentric (pp. 485, 503).
2.3.3.2 Acehnese
Acehnese provides phonological evidence for the structural parallel claimed
in MDT to hold between reduplication and synonym/antonym constructions.
Examples of total reduplication, which confers emphasis, include the following,
from Durie 1985:
The two parts are usually identical in form but may vary slightly: sometimes
the second copy differs in its vowel or final consonant, and sometimes unstressed
64 Evidence for morphological doubling
syllables can differ (ibid. p. 43). Reduplication can also be partial, targeting the
final syllable (ibid. p. 42):
Like Sye, Acehnese permits different, suppletive allomorphs of the same mor-
pheme to be used in what appears to be the same construction (p. 43):
Clearly, one would want to say about Acehnese that there is a meta-
construction whose daughters are either synonyms or antonyms, and which
assigns stress to each daughter. Treating double stress as the result of base-
reduplicant identity would obscure the relationship between reduplication and
juxtaposition of opposites; the allomorphy cases show that reduplication is not
a matter of BR identity to start with.
Comparison of MDT with OO correspondence 65
2.3.4 Wrapup
Clearly there is a cline of semantic similarity on which total identity is an
endpoint. As attested by the fact that descriptive grammarians standardly use
the term “reduplication” for the kinds of constructions we have been looking
at, reduplication includes cases in which the essential relation between the two
daughters is one of semantic identity, similarity, or opposition.
2.5 Conclusion
This chapter has assembled support for two of the essential claims of MDT,
repeated below:
(61) The Thesis of Morphological Targets: reduplication doubles
morphological constituents (affix, root, stem, word), not phonological
constituents (mora, syllable, foot)
67
68 Reduplicative phonology: daughters
support the view that reduplicative phonology is not qualitatively different from
nonreduplicative phonology and, specifically, that reduplicative phonology is
not identity-driven. Rather, true insight into reduplicative phonology comes
from looking at reduplication in its full morphological context.
These two chapters are organized around several predictions about reduplica-
tive phonology. All follow from the basic architecture of MDT, specifically from
the MDT claim that each node in a reduplication construction – first daughter,
second daughter, mother – is associated with a cophonology, or subgrammar.
This architecture is explicated in §3.1.
The first phonological prediction of MDT, sketched below, is that the phono-
logical effects that one finds within individual daughters in reduplication con-
structions will be the same kinds of effects one finds applying outside of
reduplication:
(1) Generalized Phonology Prediction:
The set of phonological effects found applying within reduplication is
equivalent to the set of morphologically conditioned phonological
effects found outside of reduplication. There is nothing unique about the
phonology of reduplication constructions.
3.1 Cophonologies
MDT uses cophonologies, or phonological grammars associated with particular
morphological constructions, to model morphologically conditioned phonology
in general. Cophonologies within a language have been motivated indepen-
dently of morphologically conditioned phonology to handle variation (Anttila
1997; Itô & Mester 1997; Anttila & Cho 1998) and what Zuraw calls pat-
terned lexical exceptions (Zuraw 2000, also Itô & Mester 1995a; b). The use
of cophonologies for the phonological patterns associated with different mor-
phological constructions was pioneered in Orgun 1994; 1996; Inkelas, Orgun
and Zoll 1997 and has been developed in much subsequent work (Itô & Mester
1996b; Orgun 1997; Inkelas 1998; Inkelas & Orgun 1998; Orgun 1999; Yu
2000; 2003).
The cophonology approach is a natural evolution of ideas put forward in the
theory of Lexical Morphology and Phonology (LMP; see, for example Pesetsky
1979; Kiparsky 1982a; b; Kaisse & Shaw 1985; Mohanan 1986; Pulleyblank
1986; Inkelas 1990), which handles morphologically conditioned phonology by
sorting the morphological constructions of a language into sequentially ordered
groups, each associated with its own distinct phonological grammar. In LMP,
phonological rules themselves are, in principle, quite general; it is the grouping
together of rules and morphological constructions within morphological levels
that produces morphological conditioning of phonology. Much research has
shown LMP in its original form to be both too strong and too weak. It is too
Cophonologies 71
strong in claiming that levels are strictly ordered relative to one another, a
point argued in, for example, Mohanan 1986; Hargus 1988; Inkelas and Orgun
1995; 1998. It is too weak in the sense that it omits from consideration many
phonological effects which are too morphologically specific to merit positing an
entire level, and thus requires building very specific morphological information
back into some phonological rules or constraints (Inkelas 1998). For example,
stress shift is associated with Level 1 morphology in English (e.g. Kiparsky
1982b), yet only a subset of Level 1 suffixes actually induce stress shift. Level 1
suffixes not inducing stress shift have to be associated with an extrametricality
rule (e.g. Hayes 1981) that is stipulated to apply to some but not all Level 1
suffixes. This very specific kind of stipulation falls outside the scope of what
level ordering can do.
Cophonology theory shares with LMP the ambition of making phonologi-
cal alternations themselves fully general. Every morphological construction –
each compounding, affixation, zero-derivation, truncation construction, and so
on – is associated with a cophonology, which may potentially differ from the
cophonologies of other constructions. In cophonology theory phonological rules
or constraints are never themselves indexed to particular morphological con-
texts; it is the entire bundle of rules or the specific ranking of constraints consti-
tuting the cophonology itself which is morphologically indexed. Cophonology
theory differs from LMP in abandoning the failed claim that morphological
conditioning boils down to dividing the lexicon into a few ordered components
each with fully general phonology.
Building on Anttila 1997 and suggestions in Inkelas and Orgun 1998 and
Stump 1998, we assume that the cophonologies of any given language are
constrained by what we have called in other work (Inkelas & Zoll 2003) the
“Master Ranking,” a partial ranking of constraints that every cophonology in
the language conforms to. The Master Ranking is in some ways similar to
proposals such the Stratum Domain or Strong Domain hypotheses of LMP,
which were attempts to constrain the degree to which phonological strata in
the same language could differ. These last hypotheses, however, presuppose
that strata are extrinsically ordered; the arguments against stratum ordering
are voluminous (see, for example, Mohanan 1986; Hargus 1988; Hualde 1988;
Inkelas & Orgun 1995; Inkelas 1998; Inkelas & Orgun 1998). Cophonologies
are intrinsically unordered. Their relative ordering follows from the intrinsic
or extrinsic ordering of the morphological constructions with which they are
associated.
Below are schematic representations of compounding and affixation, illus-
trating the association of cophonologies with morphological mother nodes. As
72 Reduplicative phonology: daughters
b. Affixation schema:
⎡ Syntax = g(Synx) ⎤
⎢ Semantics = f(Semx) ⎥ φj”
Cophonology = “φ
⎢ ⎥
⎣ Phonology = φj(Px, Py)⎦
⎡ Syntax = Synx ⎤
⎢Semantics = Semx⎥
⎢ ⎥ [Py]Y
⎣ Phonology = Px ⎦ X
What is important for purposes of this chapter is simply the fact that distinct
cophonologies can be associated with the mother node of each distinct mor-
phological construction. It is when cophonologies differ across morphologi-
cal constructions that we say that phonology is morphologically conditioned.
We illustrate the use of cophonologies to model morphologically conditioned
phonology with the example of Turkish velar deletion, below.
Stem-final velar deletion is a typical morphologically conditioned phonolog-
ical alternation. Some suffixes trigger it and others do not. (On velar deletion,
see, for example, Zimmer & Abbott 1978; Sezer 1981; Inkelas & Orgun 1995;
Inkelas 2000.)
Affixation constructions triggering velar deletion, like the dative, are associ-
ated with Cophonology 1 ; affixation constructions not triggering velar dele-
tion, like the aorist, are associated with Cophonology 2 .
(7)
Dative suffix (triggers velar deletion) Example:
⎡ Syntax = N ⎤ ⎡ Syntax = Nx ⎤
⎢ ⎥ ⎢ ⎥
⎢Semantics = Semx⎥ [/E/]Y ⎢ Semantics = ‘baby’ ⎥ [/E/]Y
⎣ Phonology = Px ⎦ X ⎣ Phonology = /bebek/⎦ X
⎡ Syntax = Nx ⎤ ⎡ Syntax = Nx ⎤
⎢ ⎥ ⎢ ⎥
⎢Semantics = Semx⎥ [/r/]Y ⎢Semantics = ‘look’⎥ [/r/]Y
⎣ Phonology = Px ⎦ X ⎣Phonology = /bak/⎦ X
of accent (if any) is predictable: accent falls on the syllable containing the
penultimate mora of the verb stem (8b) (Poser 1984:51–52, citing McCawley
1968). The data below are taken from Smith 1997:7–8, citing Haraguchi 1977
and Poser 1984:
(8) a. Nouns b. Verbs
(as discussed in Chapter 2); the mother is the reduplication construction itself,
which puts those two stems together and associates the result with a par-
ticular meaning. Each constituent in the construction is associated with a
cophonology:3
The three cophonologies may potentially differ from one another. Any of them
may also differ from the cophonologies of other constructions in the language,
resulting in the appearance of reduplication-specific phonological effects.
Consider, by way of illustration, intensive reduplication in Sanskrit, as dis-
cussed in Steriade 1988:108 (forms are prefinal, lacking the application of vowel
coalescence rules):4
The two copies are differentiated in two phonological respects: the first copy is
truncated down to a single syllable, and its initial consonant cluster (if any) is
truncated to its least sonorant consonant (kr → k, sv → s, etc.).
In MDT these modifications are performed by the cophonology of the first
daughter (Cophonology X). In Cophonology X, markedness constraints (e.g.
*Complex, which bans complex onsets) rank higher, relative to faithfulness,
than they do in the cophonology of the second daughter (Cophonology Y) or
the mother (Cophonology Z). The contrast in rankings is shown below.
with /m/ (a) (Bruening 1997, based on a manuscript by Bert Vaux). If the word
already begins with /m/, however, /č’/ is used instead (b):
Similar effects are found in cognate constructions throughout Asia (see Abbi
1991 for an overview), as well as in the emphatic adjective partial reduplication
construction, discussed in Chapter 2, found throughout Turkic (e.g. Johanson
& Csato 1998) and in Armenian (Vaux 1998).
Yip (1997; 1998) characterizes such cases in reduplication-specific terms.
Using a BRCT-like constraint, Repeat, to drive reduplicative identity, she
invokes a higher-ranking constraint, *Repeat, to drive (minimal) dissimila-
tion between base and reduplicant.
Of course, however, dissimilation that takes place across base and reduplicant
can generally be described equally well as dissimilation that takes place between
input and output, for one of the daughters in reduplication. Instead of assuming
that the reduplicant-initial m-prefix in Abkhaz is dissimilating (to č’-) with
respect to the initial m- of the base, we could equally well assume that it is
dissimilating with respect to the initial input consonant of the reduplicant:
⇐ BR
maák’ ’-aák’ maák’ ’-aák’
⇐ IO ⇐ IO ⇐ IO
/maák’/ /maák’/
/maák’/
(21) a. if base has penultimate long vowel, length shifts to preceding open
syllable, else vanishes
gu:lay ‘vegetables’ gulay-an ‘vegetable garden’
ta:goʔ ‘hide’ taguʔ-an ‘hiding place’
hala:man ‘plant’ ha:laman-an ‘garden’
lanso:nes ‘sp. of fruit’ lansones-an ‘place for growing lansones’
b. if base has penultimate short vowel, it lengthens
hiram ‘borrow’ hi:ra:m-an ‘place for borrowing’
ʔaklat ‘book’ ʔakla:t-an ‘library’
kumpisal ‘confess’ kumpi(:)sa:l-an ‘confessional’
taraŋkah ‘lock’ ta:raŋka:h-an ‘gate’
Length dissimilation is only part of the story; when the stem-final vowel
lengthens, as in (21b), so does the stem-initial vowel, as well, optionally, as
other stem-medial vowels in open syllables. Schachter and Otanes report sim-
ilar behavior for several other suffixes. According to Schachter and Otanes
1972, all syllables with long vowels are stressed; it is thus conceivable that
the alternations above are ultimately stress-related, but the pattern is clearly
dissimilatory, regardless of the precise prosodic parameter along which the
dissimilation occurs.
Given that input–output dissimilation is necessary in order to describe
nonreduplicative phonological effects, Occam’s Razor suggests it is also the
right analysis of reduplicative dissimilation as well.
The conclusion reached here, namely that none of the phonological modifica-
tions typically found in reduplication is reduplication-specific, finds interesting
echoes not only in the Native Identity approach of Steriade (1988) but also
in the following statement of Alderete et al. (1999), who argue for a Coerced
Identity approach to reduplication:
3.3.1 Hua
As seen in Chapter 2, Hua intensifying reduplication (Haiman 1980) involves
total reduplication in which each copy is subject to final vowel modification. In
the first copy, the final vowel is replaced with u; in the second copy, the final
vowel is replaced with e. The data are repeated in (24); recall that hu is a support
verb.
The base of these constructions is in most cases bound, not appearing (even
with a vowel suffix) as a free word; however, Newman notes a few forms
like gabzà ‘heap up a lot of something,’ presumably historically the base of
gabje jè ‘bulky’ and gabza -gàbzà ‘huge (people)’ (p. 76).8
A similar construction is reported by Newman (1989b:251); Niepokuj
(1997:56) and Downing (2000a) offer discussion and additional data. Neither
copy is tonally faithful to the isolation form:
adjectives in (26) calls for daughters of different types, in that the first requires
a cophonology assigning High tone, while the second requires a cophonology
assigning Low tone. Both daughters impose -a stem-finally.
(27) Daughter #1 cophonology: H tone imposed (and -a: suffixed)
Daughter #2 cophonology: L tone imposed (and -a suffixed)
The interest of the Tarok facts lies in the divergent modification exhibited
by the two copies of a monosyllabic base. Neither copy is faithful to the input,
and each shows neutralization along a different dimension. As shown in (29),
reduplication of monosyllables manifests tonal reduction in the second copy,
and truncation and vowel neutralization in the first copy. In the first copy, the
final consonant deletes and all vowels raise to [+high]. In the second copy, tone
(which in Tarok can be High, Low, or Mid) is neutralized to Mid.
(29) ı̀-jáŋ ı̀-jı́-ja:ŋ ‘his twin’ R206
a:-sáŋ a:-sə́-sa:ŋ ‘his rope’ R203
ı̀-tòk ı̀-tù-tok ‘his chair’ S203
ı̀-gy el ı̀-gi-gy el ‘his/her chin’ N115
[tù] [tok]
X: Vowel raising and truncation ⇒ | | ⇐ Y: Reduction to Mid tone
/tòk/ /tòk/
light syllable, and *V[-high], which neutralizes the height of the reduplicant
vowel to the unmarked value, [+high]. Both effects are observed in tù-tok (31).
Failure to truncate causes a fatal violation of the templatic constraint (31a, b),
and failure to raise the vowel crucially violates the vowel markedness constraint
(31c). The optimal outcome is a light syllable with a high vowel (31d).
(31)
Cophonology X /tòk/ Output=CV *V[-high] IO-Faith
a. tòk *! *
b. tùk *! *
c. tò *! *
) d. tù **
a. tòk *!
) b. tok * *
The problem for the Basic Model of BRCT presented by Tarok arises with
tonal neutralization in the second copy, which, because it is not truncated, would
be analyzed as the base. With respect to tone, the reduplicant is more faithful
to the input than the base is. Accounting for this while maintaining that IO-
Faith is responsible for base faithfulness results in a ranking paradox.10 We
know independently that IO-Faith must outrank the tonal markedness con-
straints (*High, *Low) since High, Low, and Mid all occur in nonreduplicated
forms:
(33)
tòk IO-Faith-Tone *High, *Low
) a. tòk *
b. tok *!
(34)
Red-tòk IO-Faith-Tone *High, *Low BR-Faith-Tone
()) a. tù-tok *! * *
b. tu-tok *!
c. tù-tòk **!
0 d. tu-tòk * *
(37)
Step 1 truncation
mà kubà → mà kubà
Step 2 reduplication
mà kubà → mà kubà mà kubà
In MDT, parallel modification occurs as a result of the fact that in the redupli-
cation construction in question, both daughters impose the same cophonology.
Recall that the daughters in reduplication are themselves constructions that take
a morpheme or collection of morphemes as input and map them, via a specific
cophonology, to an output which serves as one of the two inputs to the redu-
plication construction. If we assume, as in Chapter 2, that every morphological
construction in the language is identified by some sort of index, which we will
call type (following the type hierarchy literature), then to model a reduplication
construction in which both daughters are the product of the same construction,
we specify that the daughters agree not only semantically but also in type.
Type features are nonsemantic diacritic features of the sort that are needed any-
way to represent things like conjugation or declension class, or nonsemantic
gender.
Each daughter of the Hausa construction illustrated in (35) is the product of
the same stem-forming construction, shown below, which modifies noun stems
by shortening the final vowel and produces stems of type #47:
⎢ Syntax: N ⎥ ⎢ Syntax: N ⎥
⎣Phonology: truncate last vowel of Y⎦ ⎣ Phonology: bùhu ⎦
| |
⎡ Semantics: X⎤ ⎡ Semantics: ‘sack’ ⎤
⎢ Syntax: N ⎥ ⎢ Syntax: N ⎥
⎢ ⎥ ⎢ ⎥
⎣Phonology: Y⎦ ⎣ Phonology: /bùhu / ⎦
88 Reduplicative phonology: daughters
While nonderivational, this maps closely to the idea of “first truncate, then
copy.”
Parallel application of reduplication-specific phonological modification
presents a challenge for standard BRCT. To illustrate this prediction, we again
use Hausa adjective formation, which shortens final long vowels in both copies
in reduplication (mà kubà → mà kubà-mà kubà). In order for the final long
vowel to shorten in the base, it has to be the case that the ban on final long
Divergent modification 89
(41)
long-Red BR-Faith *V ] IO-Faith
a. long-short * *
b. long-long **
()) c. short-short *
d. short-long * * *
The problem is that Hausa does not shorten final long vowels generally (not
even in other reduplication patterns), and, therefore, the ranking *V ] » IO-
Faith cannot be correct. The only way for BRCT to describe the Hausa
pattern would be to adopt cophonologies, such that for adjective formation,
*V ] » IO-Faith, but for other constructions, IO-Faith » *V ]. This move would
bring BRCT closer to MDT but would vitiate the central tenet of BRCT, which
is that it is the relative ranking of BR-Faith that distinguishes reduplicative
phonology.12
Interestingly, BRCT is capable of generating a superficially similar paral-
lel modification effect. McCarthy and Prince (1999a) credit René Kager and
Philip Hamilton for independently observing what has come to be known as
the “Kager–Hamilton problem”: given base-reduplicant correspondence (as in
BRCT), it is logically possible for a truncated reduplicant to cause a base to
become truncated as well. This can happen in case BR-Faith is ranked so
high that a reduplicant-specific constraint on prosodic size (Red = X) is indi-
rectly imposed on the base as well. Thus, for example, the ranking Red =
, BR-Faith » IO-Faith would produce a situation where bases and redu-
plicants are both truncated to light syllables. The belief that such phenomena
do not occur inspired the development of Generalized Template Theory (GTT;
McCarthy & Prince 1994c; Urbanczyk 1996), a serious modification to BRCT
which prohibits the use of direct prosodic constraints on reduplicant size. How-
ever, the existence of doubled truncated nicknames like JoJo or CoCo (from
Josephine or Collette) suggests, as observed by Downing (2000a), that in fact
there may be no empirical basis to the “Hamilton–Kager” problem; any time
truncation and reduplication are in a morphological feeding relationship, the
appearance of double truncation can be generated, with or without GTT, and
GTT is therefore unnecessary.13
90 Reduplicative phonology: daughters
Newman notes (p. 196) that the form each copy takes in frequentative redu-
plication is identical to the stative form of the same verb, e.g. sàssà e ‘carved.’
However, Newman argues that the frequentative and stative constructions are
not synchronically related, either functionally or semantically. What this shows
is that Hausa has a stem-forming construction producing L*H verbs ending
in -e:
(43) Construction: Example:
(44)
Double truncation outside of reduplication
a. Japanese clipped compound loanwords (Itô 1990:220)
waado purosessaa → waa puro ‘word processor’
paasonaru koNpyuutaa → paso koN ‘personal computer’
paNtii sutokkiNgu → paN suto ‘panty stockings’
sukeeto boodo → suke boo ‘skate board’
uhuke uhuke
However, our survey of the literature shows that the evidence for base-
dependent reduplicant shape is very slim, if not nonexistent. The prediction
of MDT that base-dependence should not occur appears to be correct.
Since the infixation account works neatly for all the data, and since CV
infixing reduplication is common not only in the world’s languages in gen-
eral but in Oceanic languages in particular, we may safely conclude with
McCarthy and Prince (1999b) that Orokaiva does not present evidence for base
dependence.16
We have not found other robust examples of the kind of CV ∼ VC reduplica-
tive allomorphy, conditioned by whether the following base begins with a C or a
V, that would signal base dependence.17 There are certainly cases, like Roviana
(Corston-Oliver 2000), where phonological alternations applying at the mother
node level give rise to phonologically conditioned CV ∼ VC allomorphy. How-
ever, Roviana is not a case of base-dependence; all of the surface reduplicative
allomorphs can be derived from the same basic (C)VC reduplicant structure.
In Roviana, the general method of forming imperfectives is to reduplicate the
first two syllables of the verb root. As shown below, when the first two syllables
are both CV, the second V is often deleted from the reduplicant (49a, b). If
V-deletion creates a geminate, simplification occurs (b):
In the case of a vowel-initial verb, the reduplicant would be VCV. The final
vowel of the reduplicant fuses with the initial vowel of the base, giving the
appearance of VC reduplication.
The data in (52) suggest that the target output shape for the reduplicant is
not CV but CVCV, which affects the interpretation of the data in (51). The data
with apparent VC reduplicants may very well instead be instantiating VCV
reduplication and experiencing a V-V → V simplification like that of Roviana.
Indeed, there is independent evidence for vowel elision in Tawala. Ezard (1997)
describes a regular process by which the initial vowel of the derivational pre-
fix om- is elided following a vowel-final prefix, thus, for example, /hi-om-
hoe/ → himhoe ‘they left’ (p. 36). Ezard also describes a process by which
morphemes containing a lexical vowel sequence undergo the loss of the first
vowel when another morpheme follows in the same word or phrase: i-mae
‘3sg-stay = he stayed,’ vs. i-me duma ‘3sg-stay very = he stayed a long time’
(p. 37).
If we accept vowel elision in (52), then the data in (51b) and (52) fall together.
In each case the output of the reduplication is two moras. What requires expla-
nation is not the VC allomorph in (52) but the CV allomorph in (51a). Although
we cannot be sure, Ezard’s observation about VV reduction in non-final mor-
phemes may well be relevant here. If indeed VV → V simplification is taking
place at Cophonology Z (or even at the word level or phrasally), the output of
the reduplicant may in these cases be bimoraic (CVV) as well.
96 Reduplicative phonology: daughters
3.5 Conclusion
In this chapter we have documented a number of cases in which both daughters
in reduplication are modified, and we have provided analyses making use of
the principle in MDT that requires only semantic identity between the two
daughters in reduplication and allows the daughters to diverge phonologically
and morphotactically.
By accounting for reduplicative phonology using the same method –
cophonologies – which we use to account for nonreduplicative phonology,
we correctly predict that all of the types of phonological effects that occur in
reduplication occur outside of reduplication as well. Morphological Doubling
Theory is economical, in not using any reduplication-specific phonological
rules or constraints; it also makes the right predictions about the phonology
of reduplication. Theories that focus solely on reduplication fail to capture the
fundamental parallels between reduplicative and nonreduplicative phonology.
4 Morphologically conditioned
phonology in reduplication:
the mother node
98
General approach to junctural phonology 99
1982; Sietsema 1988; Patterson 1990).5 The data in (5) illustrate the pattern for
vowel final roots; the reduplicant is boldfaced (Shaw 1985:184):
If the first coronal in a cluster of coronals is {t, č, n, d}, it becomes dorsal,
but this happens only if the cluster straddles the juncture between reduplicant
and base (Shaw 1985:184):
[ ] [ ] [ ] [ ]
(12)
/phet – nákpakpa/ IO-Ident(Place) *Cor-Cor
b. pheg – nákpakpa *!
(13)
/sut-Red-a/ IO-Ident(Place) IR-Ident(Place) *Cor-Cor
0 a. sut-sút-a *
()) b. suk-sút-a *!
c. suk-súk-a *!
It is clear that the solution for Dakota lies in having a cophonology specific
to the reduplication construction, as we propose above.8
(17)
[RED-buku] BR-MetricalFaith Culminativity
) a. búku-búku *
b. bùku-búku *!
the stems neta ‘I see’ and yopnuha ‘he wards off there,’ serve as illustration.
For any given stem, some suffixes arbitrarily trigger deletion of the final vowel
(20a), while others do not (20b):
(23)
/Red-bunı́ta/ BR-Ident(Stress) Penult
) b. underapplication bunı́ta-ta *
(25)
b. underapplication sá-sa-ga * *!
Stress shift and subordination under affixation decrease identity between the
two copies. Kenstowicz (1995) and Cohn and McCarthy (1998) independently
argue that this is a kind of TETU effect; once identity is disrupted for other rea-
sons, unmarkedness emerges.13 As modeled in (28), the unmarkedness effect
in this case is stress reduction; the disruption in identity is caused by stress
shift upon suffixation. As seen, the addition of a suffix to a reduplicated form
forces the main stress on the second copy to shift rightward (by high-ranking
Penult). This shift induces non-identity between the two copies, since it
results in differently stressed syllables in each. Given that perfect identity is
impossible, the effects of Culminativity emerge, triggering stress subor-
dination in the first copy in typical TETU fashion:14
(28)
(30)
pás ‘try’ di [pàs-pás]kan ‘tried on, repeatedly’
kərá ‘monkey’ kərá-kərá ‘monkeys’ [kərà-kərá]an ‘toy monkey’
kəcı́l ‘small’ kəcı́l-kəcı́l ‘small (dist.)’ məŋ[əcı̀l-ŋəcı́l]kan ‘to belittle s.t.’
Reduplication-specific non-alternation 111
(31)
[RED-pas-kan] BR-MetricalFaith culminativity
a. pàs-pás-kan *!
0 b. pás-pás-kan *
Cohn and McCarthy solve this problem by proposing that although stress does
not shift, metrical structure changes under suffixation by virtue of enlarging the
second metrical foot to two syllables. Footing differences are argued to trigger
likewise a violation of BR-MetricalFaith; the decision among candidates
is thus again passed down to culminativity, with the result that only the
second stress retains its primary status:
(32)
[RED-pas-kan] BR-MetricalFaith culminativity
) a. (pàs)-(pás-kan) *
b. (pás)-(pás-kan) * *!
(33)
/hand/ *complex IO-Max-C NoCoda
a. hand *!
b. han * *!
0 c. ha *
In the Indonesian stress case, this problem is exacerbated further by the fact
that, in a fully developed analysis, the single metrical BR-MetricalFaith
constraint would have to be broken down into a family of metrical faithfulness
constraints, as is standard in any analysis of stress (see, for example, Kager
1999 for an overview). In that case, we would not expect to see the TETU
effect at all. Since suffixed output candidates violate foot identity regardless of
whether or not stress subordination occurs, the candidate that at least preserves
the same degree of stress would always be optimal:
(34)
[RED-pas-kan] Br-Ident BR-Ident
(stress degree) (foot membership)
a. (pàs)-(pás-kan) *! *
) b. (pás)-(pás-kan) *
Even if it were possible to work out the technical details of Cohn and
McCarthy’s Indonesian analysis, the deeper issue is whether or not the intuition
Cohn and McCarthy, and Kenstowicz, pursue for Indonesian is the right one,
cross-linguistically. The intuition behind their analysis is that if a language has
a phonological alternation that fails to apply in reduplication, the inhibition is
due to BR identity; only when perfect identity is impossible for independent
reasons is the alternation predicted to apply, in the manner of TETU.
Our investigations, detailed below, fail to support this intuition. Lack of appli-
cation of a phonological alternation in reduplication is due to its absence from
the cophonology associated with that reduplication construction; application of
the alternation when the construction is embedded in further morphology is due
to the presence of the alternation in the cophonology associated with the higher
morphological constructions. Interestingly, this is the analysis originally given
to Indonesian by Cohn (1989); we have found its counterpart to be the correct
analysis in numerous other cases as well.
Reduplication-specific non-alternation 113
4.3.5 Klamath
We illustrate our argument against the equation of phonological underapplica-
tion with maintenance of base-reduplicant identity with Klamath intensive redu-
plication, a well-known case for which that equation has been made (McCarthy
& Prince 1995a).15 The relevant phonological alternation in Klamath is vowel
reduction/syncope. Both prefixes and root reduplication trigger vowel reduction
in the second syllable of the derived stem (Barker 1963; 1964).16 If the second
syllable is closed, its vowel reduces to schwa (35a, b). If the second syllable
would be open, the vowel deletes (35c, d) altogether. Henceforth, page numbers
for data taken from Barker (1963) are prefixed with “B63”; those for data taken
from Barker (1964) are prefixed with “B64.”17
the set of constraints that motivate vowel reduction and deletion; it is outranked
by IO-Faith- 1 , which protects the vowel in the initial syllable from
reduction/deletion, with the result that only the second syllable is normally
affected (as in (35)). In the Intensive form shown in (37), normal second syllable
reduction, which would affect only the base (37a), is blocked because it would
render the base and the reduplicant non-identical. Reducing both copies, which
would also maintain identity, fatally violates the high-ranking IO-Faith- 1
(37b) that protects the initial syllable from reduction. The optimal solution is
to reduce neither (37c):
(37)
..............................................
/INT-Wič-l’i/ IO-Faith- 1 BR-Faith Reduce
a. W[ə]č-W[ə]č-l’i *!
b. Wič-W[ə]č-l’i *! *
) c. Wič-Wič-l’i **
If this is the right analysis, then the failure of reduction in Intensive redu-
plication requires some version of BR faithfulness, and would be incompatible
with MDT. However, the backcopying analysis has two major drawbacks. First,
it does not take into account the fact that vowel reduction also fails to apply in
certain nonreduplicative morphological contexts; second, it does not take into
account the fact that reduction does apply in some reduplicative contexts. The
BR identity analysis presupposes an implicational connection between redupli-
cation and lack of vowel reduction which simply does not exist, a point made
by Zoll (2002), on which this discussion draws; Park (2000) reaches similar
conclusions as well. To support this view, we present some background on
Klamath morphology.
The verb in Klamath consists of a number of prefix classes, a root, and a
variety of suffixes (Barker 1964; Delancey 1991; 1999):
Bare roots and root-suffix sequences are contexts where BR identity is not at
stake; thus some other mechanism is needed to prevent vowel reduction in these
domains, while allowing it in prefixed stems like those in (35). MDT assumes,
as for all morphologically conditioned phonology, a cophonological differ-
ence. Stems, consisting of root plus suffixes, are associated with a cophonol-
ogy that does not enforce vowel reduction (IO-Faith » Reduce). By con-
trast, prefixes like those in (35) are associated with a cophonology that does
enforce vowel deletion in noninitial syllables (IO-Faith-Initial » Reduce »
IO-Faith).
The same cophonological differences found outside of reduplication also
obtain among reduplication constructions. Vowel reduction fails to apply in the
Intensive reduplication construction (36), as we have seen; however, it does
apply in another reduplication construction: the Distributive (40).
The prediction that reduction will be backcopied to the base, yielding the
starred forms in (41), is illustrated below. Only Reduce violations for the
second syllable are shown:
(42)
a. se-Leq’-Leq’-a *!
()) b. se-L[ə]q’-Leq’-a *!
0 c. se-L[ə]q’-L[ə]q’-a *
McCarthy and Prince (1995a) cite a different form which they interpret as
showing that prefixed Intensive forms do show reduction in both base and redu-
plicant: /sw’V-Red-ciq’-a/ → sw’i-c[ə]q-c[Ø]q’a ‘shake the head’ (1995a:349),
which has reduction in the reduplicant and deletion in the base. This form is
assumed to be representative of prefixed reduplicated forms in Klamath gen-
erally. (McCarthy & Prince observe that the form is problematic in that the
reduplicant and base reduce differently: schwa in the reduplicant, and deletion
in the base.)
An exhaustive search of Barker (1964) reveals, however, that this form is
not representative and that double reduction of prefixed intensive stems is not
the norm. Crucially, there are no examples in which an unprefixed intensive
Reduplication-specific non-alternation 117
Under any account, forms like č[ə]q-č[Ø]q’-a, which exhibit double reduc-
tion even when unprefixed, are lexical exceptions and must be listed as such.
Given this, the patterning of the rest of the data reflects normal application of
vowel reduction.
In summary, the failure of the Intensive to trigger reduction is not directly
attributable to BR identity. Instead, it must be attributed to the association of
the Intensive construction with a non-reducing cophonology. The side-by-side
structures in (44) of an Intensive verb and a prefixed Intensive verb show how the
differential application of reduction in the two cases follows directly from the
association of the appropriate cophonologies with the constructions involved.
Each arrow represents the effect of the cophonology associated with the mother
node just above:
that reduction does not apply stem-internally. This analysis accords with the
insights of Barker (1964) and White (1973), who treat Intensive reduplication
essentially as root-root compounding.
By contrast, the Distributive, a late morphological process, patterns with
stem-external prefixes in triggering vowel reduction. The association of clusters
of morphological constructions, or zones in the word, with similar cophonolo-
gies is the kind of pattern that stratal ordering was originally developed to
handle; it can be captured in cophonology theory through meta constructions;
see, for example, Inkelas and Orgun 1998; Stump 1998; Anttila 2002 for
discussion.
In summary, the failure of vowel reduction in Intensive reduplication in
Klamath is a case of normal non-application of Reduction/Deletion stem-
internally, not backcopying underapplication. The proposed cophonology anal-
ysis is capable not only of distinguishing constructions triggering reduction
from those which do not, but also of correctly predicting the outcome in forms
containing both kinds of constructions. In the latter respect it is more descrip-
tively adequate than the backcopying analysis argued for in McCarthy and
Prince 1995a. Our reanalysis thus eliminates Klamath as support for BR cor-
respondence, while also making the more general point that morphological
investigation is essential to the understanding of the non-application of a phono-
logical alternation.19
4.4 ∃-Faith
The MDT view that reduplicative phonology is simply a subcase of morpho-
logically conditioned phonology, modeled in exactly the same way, contrasts
sharply with the BRCT view, stated most strongly by Spaelti (1997):
all deviations from complete identity between the two parts of the reduplication
are due to [TETU]. In contrast to this any unexpected identity between the redu-
plicant and the base [is] the result of what I will call Identity Induced
Failure of Alternation (IIFA). These two concepts constitute the
entirety of the theory of reduplication. (p. 43)
The difficulty with this view, as we have seen, is that it does not extend
to the broader range of morphologically conditioned phonology found across
languages, nor does it fully account for the phonology of reduplication itself.
One proposal within the BRCT framework that has made some inroads into
the latter problem is existential faithfulness (∃-Faith), a proposal by Struijke
(1998; 2000a; 2000b), building on earlier work by Raimy and Idsardi 1997;
∃-Faith 119
Spaelti 1997; Fitzgerald 2000 (see also Rose 1999) which addresses the issue
of ranking paradoxes arising when alternations which are not fully general
in the language target the base of reduplication. One such paradox arises in
Dakota (§4.2), where coronal dissimilation applies across the internal juncture
only in reduplication; Tarok, in Chapter 3, represents another ranking paradox
in which reduplication-specific neutralization applies to the base. Klamath, in
which vowel reduction applies to the base in some cases of reduplication but
not generally in the language, is yet another such example. In each case, the
paradox is the same. For the alternation in question to apply to the base of
reduplication, it must be the case that the relevant markedness constraint (set)
outranks IO-Faith. That predicts, however, that the alternation will also occur
outside reduplication, which is not true.
Perhaps the best-known example in the literature of a reduplication-induced
ranking paradox comes from diminutive reduplication in Lushootseed, dis-
cussed earlier in this chapter, and one of the languages highlighted in the work
of Struijke (2000a; b). Lushootseed exhibits a process of unstressed vowel
syncope/reduction. Earlier work on Lushootsheed (Hess 1977; Hess & Hilbert
1978; Broselow 1983; Bates 1986; Hess 1993; 1995) observes that reduction fre-
quently occurs between voiceless obstruents; Urbanczyk (1996) demonstrates
that the alternation targets unstressed vowels, primarily the low vowel /a/. An
overview of the morphological conditioning of syncope, oversimplified in a cru-
cial way to be clarified shortly, is as follows. In unreduplicated words, reduction
is optional, leading to free variation (a), while in reduplicated words, reduction
is obligatory (b). Which vowel reduces – reduplicant or base – depends on
the location of stress, as discussed in Urbanczyk 1996 and references therein.
Whether an unstressed /a/ syncopates or reduces is predictable from segmental
environment; we subsume both processes under the term “reduction.” The data
below are taken from Urbanczyk 1996:
(45)
a. caq’(a) ‘spear, jab’ cá-cq’ ‘act of spearing big game on water’ 167
w w
šaX -il ‘grass, hay’ sá-ʔsX -il ‘(short) grass, lawn’ 167
wális ‘type of frog’ wá-w’lis ‘Little Frog (wife of p’ic’ikw )’ 167
laq-il ‘late’ lá-ʔlq-il ‘be a little late’ 167
q’aXa-cut ‘uncover’ q’ə-ʔq’áXa-cut ‘show self a little’ 169
b. sčúsad ∼ sčúsəd ‘(established) star’ 166
hı́qab ∼ hı́qəb ‘too, excessively’ 166
qacágw=ac ∼ qəcágw=ac ∼ qcágw=ac ‘ironwood, ocean spray, spiraea’ 166
Xá’=al=ap ∼ Xə’=ál=ap ‘steer a canoe with a paddle held 166
over the stern (like a rudder)’
120 Reduplicative phonology: mothers
The ranking paradox lies in the fact that the constraint rankings driving the
pattern in bases of reduplication are clearly different from those driving the
pattern in unreduplicated bases. In particular, the ranking that entails obliga-
tory reduction in diminutive reduplication, Reduce » IO-Faith, incorrectly
predicts obligatory reduction outside of reduplication as well.20
The cophonology approach would resolve this paradox by positing two
cophonologies, one associated with unreduplicated stems, and one associated
with the reduplication construction.
(48)
/sčúsad/ ∃-Faith Reduce
) a. no reduction sčúsad *
b. reduction sčúsəd *!
/Red-cáq’/
c. no reduction cá-caq’ *!
) d. reduction cá-cq’
If ∃-Faith can not only resolve the ranking paradox which MDT resolves
using different cophonologies but also predict which construction – redupli-
cation or nonreduplication – will be associated with which pattern, ∃-Faith
would be an appealing alternative to cophonologies.
To evaluate the potential of ∃-Faith to obviate cophonologies, we turn
now to an in-depth exploration of the intuitions underlying ∃-Faith and the
predictions it makes. We ultimately conclude that ∃-Faith is neither nec-
essary nor sufficient in accounting for reduplicative phonology, nor can it
extend, as cophonologies do, to nonreduplicative morphologically conditioned
phonology.
In this section we explore four aspects of ∃-Faith that lead to the conclusion
we have just stated.
(51)
/ bùhu/ ∃-Faith *Final-V:
) bùhu
bùhu *!
.....................................................
/Red-bùhu/ ∃-Faith *Final-V: BR-Faith
possible a. bùhu-bùhu **
impossible c. bùhu-bùhu *!
But this move undermines the primary motivation behind ∃-Faith, which
was to resolve ranking paradoxes without resorting to reranking. Once reranking
is introduced as a possibility, there is no longer any need for ∃-Faith; we can
simply use cophonologies, as MDT does, which utilize markedness constraints
and standard IO-Faith to generate the right outcomes. The cophonological
approach to Hausa is shown below:
The problem for ∃-Faith is that if ∃-Faith is substituted for IO-Faith, the
distinction between these two patterns collapses. ∃-Faith predicts reduction
in reduplication regardless of where it is ranked with respect to Reduce.
(61)
...................................
/Red-Xa’-il/ ∃-Faith (V) Reduce
0 b. Xá -X’-il
A tableau showing the correct result for a non-reducing stem is given below
(compare to the failed tableau, above):
(63)
/Red-Xa’-il/ ∃-Faith (V) BR-Faith Reduce
) a. Xá -Xa’-il *
b. Xá -X’-il *!
The only way to save the ∃-Faith approach from descriptive inadequacy is
to permit reranking of BR-Faith and Reduce across different reduplication
constructions, as shown below.
(64) Reducing reduplicative construction: ∃-Faith, Reduce » BR Faith
Non-reducing reduplication construction: ∃-Faith, BR Faith » Reduce
other reduplication
Nonreduplication
constructions
constructions
phonology z
phonology y
Nonreduplication
constructions
phonology x phonology y
constructions
∃-Faith 129
The facts we discuss are of a familiar type; the novelty in our discussion lies
only in pointing out their implications for ∃-Faith.
(72)
a. RED 'ham bo-'ham botu-ana27 ‘sit-sit-nml = chair’ 469
CPD 'etu-'lotu ‘house-pray = church’ 469
b. RED ham bo-ham botu-ana ∼ ‘chair’ 470
ham-ham botu-ana
peka-peka ∼ pek-peka ‘dance-dance = dancing’ 470
CPD etu-lotu ∼ et-lotu ‘church’ 470
c. RED heγe-heγere ∼ heγ-heγere ∼ he-heγere ‘laugh-laugh’ 470
ra-raro (< rar-raro < raro-raro) ‘cook’ 470
pino-pino ∼ pim-pino (< pin-pino) ‘star’ 470
CPD <no examples given>
Even though vowel and consonant reduction are neutralization effects that
could, in reduplication, be attributed to ∃-Faith, an ∃-Faith account would
do nothing to capture the parallelism between reduplication and compounding
with respect to these effects.
Parallelism, in particular languages, between compounding and reduplication
constructions has often been noticed; a common response has been to propose
that reduplication and compounding both operate on prosodic words. While this
may well be the right approach to Roviana reduplication, it still does not elim-
inate the need for cophonologies. Vowel deletion is not a general phenomenon
at the end of prosodic words in Roviana; thus, it is still necessary to specify,
cophonologically, its occurrence in reduplication and compounding.
A similar constellation of reduplication and compounding is manifested in
Burmese. As is typical of languages in the Tibeto-Burman family, Burmese
imposes strict constraints on syllable structure. Complex onsets are allowed
only in the “main,” or “full” syllable of the word; a preceding “minor” syllable
is permitted only a simplex onset. Green (2002) analyzes “full” syllables as
feet and treats complex onsets as being licensed only foot-initially. Synchronic
alternations show that this distributional generalization is actively enforced.
As analyzed in VanBik 2003, Burmese has a process of compounding marked
phonologically by voicing of the initial obstruent of the second member (e.g. pè
‘peanut’ + pouʔ ‘rot/putrid’ = pè-bouʔ ‘fermented soybean’). Compounds are
subject to reduction, whereby the rime of the first member reduces to [ə], and
its onset simplifies and, if the second member begins with a voiced obstruent,
voices:28
Even though the onset reduction in reduplication might normally call out for
an ∃-Faith account, the clear parallelism between compounding and redupli-
cation shows that ∃-Faith is irrelevant in this reduction process.
4.4.6 Wrapup
Our discussion of ∃-Faith has revealed that even in a theory with ∃-Faith
constraints, it is still necessary to appeal to cophonologies to capture the full
range of reduplicative phonology. Once cophonologies are invoked, ∃-Faith
becomes superfluous. ∃-Faith is neither necessary nor sufficient in the anal-
ysis of reduplicative phonology.
The main problem with ∃-Faith lies with its twin presuppositions that
reduplicative phonology is different from nonreduplicative phonology and that
the difference emanates from base-reduplicant identity. Both presuppositions,
however, derive from an overly narrow focus on reduplication. By situating
reduplication in the larger context of morphologically conditioned phonology,
we have found that the same cophonological approach needed outside redupli-
cation is necessary, and sufficient, to account for reduplicative phonology as
well.
Part of the appeal of ∃-Faith lies in its evocation of a commonly held view
that contrast preservation is a desideratum of grammar. Reduplication, offering
two chances to preserve each input contrast, might therefore be expected to
behave differently from other constructions. Syntagmatically, certainly, we have
seen that ∃-Faith is not substantiated. The paradigmatic function of contrast
preservation is harder to evaluate, and may have some truth. It is possible
that functional pressures to recover input information have created a situation
in which reduction is more likely, statistically, within reduplication; we do
not know, and it would take a massive diachronic and synchronic study to
be able to test this hypothesis. In any case, if one looks only at the types
of reductions that are possible, one finds that reduplicants have no special
status.
134 Reduplicative phonology: mothers
4.5 Conclusion
Reduplication constructions, as wholes, are often associated with phonologi-
cal effects not observed generally in the language. We have argued at length
that this phenomenon is exactly parallel to the phenomenon of morphologi-
cally conditioned phonology outside of reduplication. Theories that approach
reduplication out of morphological context and focus exclusively on effects
that appear related to the phonological identity between bases and reduplicant
miss the larger picture and unnecessarily burden the theory with reduplication-
specific analytical tools.
5 Morphologically driven opacity
in reduplication
135
136 Morphologically driven opacity in reduplication
[medja] [medja-ne]
Although the data in (3) are neutral between the two analyses in (4), abundant
evidence within Javanese supports the opacity-by-truncation analysis.
When causative and locative stems are reduplicated, identical stem vowels
occur in the two copies, parallel to the phenomenon manifested in the suffixed
reduplicated stems in §5.1.1. In the case of causative and locative suffixation
there is positive evidence pointing to the source of this effect. As shown in
(6), the consonantal causative and locative stem formers surface on both copies
when the root in question is vowel-final (see, for example, Sumukti 1971:97,
Horne 1961:223, Dudas 1976:31). Double underlining identifies those vowels
and consonants in the reduplicant for whose quality (or existence) the suffix is
responsible:
Why do only the causative and locative stem-forming consonants, rather than
the entire causative and locative suffix complexes, surface on the first copy?
One possibility is morphological: perhaps only the causative and locative stem
formers are present in the input to reduplication, with the extensions being added
to the entire reduplicated stem. The other hypothesis, which we adopt here, is
phonological: the reduplicant, i.e. the output of Cophonology X, is subject to
a restriction that its syllable count cannot exceed that of the input root. We
formalize this by positing a constituent, the Proot (Prt), which contains all of
the syllables of the morphological root (Rt), plus any surrounding consonants
which can syllabify into the Proot without increasing its syllable count (see
(8) for formalization). Cophonology X truncates its input down to the Proot,
which consists of the material in the morphological root plus any incorporated
affixal consonants. In the schema below, representing the derivation of ‘berate’
in (6d), each daughter takes as input the Causative stem {uneq}ake, based on
the input /uni-q-ake/. Cophonology X truncates its input down to the Proot,
{uneq}; Cophonology Y is faithful.
/uniRt-q-ake/
{unεq} {unεq}ake
Cophon. X: truncation to Proot ⇒ | | ⇐ Cophon. Y: Identity
{uneq}ake {uneq}ake
on the root. Our analysis of Javanese is firmly rooted within this tradition of
mismatches between morphological and prosodic constituents.
The Root-Proot (RP) correspondence constraints involved in truncating the
output to a Proot which is based on the input morphological Root but which
incorporates an adjacent non-Root consonant are given below:
(8) RP-Max-seg: All root segments should have correspondents in the Proot
RP-Max-syl: All root syllables should have correspondents in the Proot
RP-Dep-syl: All Proot syllables should have correspondents in the root
RP-Dep-seg: All Proot segments should have correspondents in the root
IP-Max-seg: All input segments should have correspondents in the Proot
In Javanese, RP-Max constraints are always satisfied; the Proot always includes
the Mroot. Of interest is the potential for the Proot to contain affix material as
well. The ranking illustrated in (9), RP-Dep-syl » IP-Max-seg » RP-Dep-seg,
results in inclusion of non-root input segments in the Proot when doing so does
not add syllables to the Proot which lack correspondents in the root:
a. {unε}qake qake!
) c. {unεq}ake ake q
d. {u}nεqake niqake
When vowel-final stems, i.e. those taking the /-n/ allomorph of the nomi-
nalizer, are reduplicated, the suffixal /-n/ surfaces, as expected under the MDT
analysis, in both copies; not surprisingly, the ablaut patterns triggered by /-n/
appear in both copies as well:5
When, however, the root is consonant-final, /-an/ surfaces only following the
second copy. As predicted in the MDT account, the ablaut effects associated
with the nominalizer are nonetheless found on both copies:
The analysis already developed for reduplicated causative and locative roots
extends straightforwardly to the nominalizer. Cophonology X truncates the suf-
fixed, nominalized stem input down to its Proot, which consists of the input mor-
phological root plus any adjacent material that can syllabify into the Proot with-
out increasing its syllable count. This analysis correctly predicts that only the /n/
allomorph of the nominalizer will survive truncation; vowel ablaut effects, how-
ever, will surface in the output of Cophonology X regardless of whether suffixal
material does. Example (13) shows nominalizer stem formation for vowel-final
and consonant-final roots, and (14) illustrates the fate of Nominalized stems in
reduplication:
/medja Rt -ku/
The question arises at this point as to why the k of -ku cannot syllabify into
the Proot, yielding *medjak-medjaku, in the same manner as the -q Causative
stem former (as in (6)) or the -n allomorph of the Nominalizer (as in (11)).
We hypothesize that a principle of Morpheme Integrity, along the lines of that
proposed for Kinande by Mutaka and Hyman 1990, prevents the partial pars-
ing of Javanese affixes into the Proot. Either the whole suffix (if consonantal)
incorporates into the Proot, or none of it does. What distinguishes the Causative
-q and Nominalizer -n suffix allomorphs is that they are monoconsonantal, able
to syllabify in their entirety into a preceding vowel-final Proot.
144 Morphologically driven opacity in reduplication
b. Demonstrative
(20) {bəd
. a}e Cophonology deletes intervocalic /h/
bəd.ahRI -e/
The MDT analysis developed for Javanese reduplication handles these cases
straightforwardly. The suffix triggering /h/-deletion is present in the input to
Daughter-based opacity in Javanese 147
both copies in reduplication; the first copy undergoes truncation to its Proot,
preserving the effects of /h/-deletion but obscuring its suffixal trigger. This is
illustrated below for the reduplicated demonstrative form of ‘broken’ (21a). The
inputs to reduplication are both {bəd.a}e, itself the output of the Demonstrative
stem-forming construction whose input is /bəd.ah-e/:
(22) bəda-b
. ədae
.
bəda . bədae
. Daughters of reduplication
| |
{bəda}e
. {bəda}e
. Demonstrative stems
| |
/bədah-e/
. /bədah-e/
.
As with /a/-raising and nasal prefix fusion, /h/-deletion is opaque in the first copy
because the environment which conditions its application within the suffixed
stem is obscured by morphological truncation in reduplication.
(26) {murid}-e
/muritRt -e/
Neither Laxing nor Consonant Devoicing are conditioned in this stem; both
transparently fail to apply. The appearance of underapplication in reduplication
is a result of two factors: (a) the truncation, in Cophonology X, of the suffix
Daughter-based opacity in Javanese 149
which, on the stem cycle, renders the stem-final syllable open, permitting the
vowel to remain tense and the consonant to remain voiced, and (b) the fact that
Laxing and Devoicing do not reapply in Cophonology Z.
(27) murid-muride
murid muride
| |
{murid}e {murid}e
MDT does not predict that Laxing and Consonant Devoicing have to be
opaque in the output of reduplicated suffixed stems. It would be possible, for
example, within MDT for Laxing and Consonant Devoicing to apply again in
Cophonology X, producing an output in which both copies – the first, trun-
cated copy, and the second, intact copy – exhibit transparent phonology: murit-
murid-e. The fact that they do not apply cyclically is an arbitrary fact about
Javanese. What MDT does clearly predict, however, is that the opacity within
each daughter is an effect of daughter-internal phonology; this is a prediction
in which MDT differs sharply from BRCT or other Coerced Identity theories.
In a BRCT account of Laxing, underapplication in murid-muride would follow
from the fact that Laxing would disrupt BR identity if it applied in the first copy
(where it is conditioned); therefore it is coerced to apply in neither. The problem
is that identity would be maintained equally well if Laxing overapplied, yielding
murid-muride with transparent application in the first copy by opaque applica-
tion in the second copy. In BRCT what determines whether underapplication
or overapplication of Laxing will be the outcome is relative markedness. If the
constraint against a tense vowel in a closed syllable (*Tense/Closed) outranks
the ban on a lax vowel in an open syllable (*Lax/Open), Laxing will overapply;
if the ranking is reversed, Laxing will underapply. In MDT, by contrast, there
is no means of deriving Laxing overapplication. Laxing is conditioned neither
in the input nor in the output of the second copy in forms like murid-muride.
If Laxing were a word-level alternation, rather than a stem-level alternation,
MDT could describe the situation in which Laxing applied in the first copy but
not in the second (the hypothetical murit-muride), but it could never generate
murit-muride.
In summary, in every case of opacity we have surveyed in Javanese, the
alternation is transparent in its domain of application (the stem); the opacity is
generated when the stem in question is embedded in a reduplication construction
which truncates that part of the stem orginally determining the applicability
of the alternation. MDT not only describes this opacity effortlessly; it also
150 Morphologically driven opacity in reduplication
correctly predicts whether the opacity will take the form of underapplication or
overapplication.
5.1.7 Summary
In surveying opaque reduplicative phonology in Javanese, we have concluded
that BR identity does not play a role. If anything, reduplication obscures identity,
by virtue of the concomitant truncation. It is not the case in Javanese that
reduplicative phonology creates or preserves identity that would not otherwise
have existed.
In finding that reduplication operates on morphologically complex stems, we
concur with the original insight of Dudas (1976), who concludes (p. 209) that
reduplication is ordered after the phonological alternations in question.
A strong prediction that our analysis makes is that alternations internal to
the daughters of reduplication may be rendered opaque by reduplication, but
that word-level phonology, which applies to the output of reduplication, cannot
be. This prediction appears to be correct. Glide Insertion (Dudas 1976:135–36)
applies between two non-identical vowels when the first is tense and the second
is lower or fronter than the first. In the examples below, Glide Insertion breaks
up vowel hiatus resulting from h-deletion. Notice that it applies transparently,
not opaquely: only the second copy in reduplication, not the first, is followed
by an inserted glide:
5.2.1 Chamorro
In Chamorro, opacity results from the interaction between stem-level stress
assignment and subsequent infixing reduplication (Topping 1973). Recall from
our discussion of Chamorro in Chapter 4 that stress is normally penultimate,
shifting rightward under suffixation:
The MDT analysis of Chamorro parallels that developed for Javanese. Penul-
timate stress assignment applies transparently in the stems which are input to
reduplication, as shown below:
Reduplication calls for two copies of each stem, the first of which is truncated
by Cophonology X to (in the case of stressed CV reduplication) the initial CV
portion of its stressed syllable. The second copy in reduplication, associated
with Cophonology Y, preserves the stem intact. Cophonology Z infixes the
output of Cophonology X into the output of Cophonology Y:
152 Morphologically driven opacity in reduplication
(32) hugá-ga-ndo
(33)
‘dim-go/be like’ /soN-kaa/ sangkakaa 77
‘rec-move-I’ /ko-puu-ku/ kopupuuku 34
‘ass.col-ignore-someone-rf’ /pog-baya-an/ pogbabayaan 60
‘af-du.rec-aug-obey’ /m-pi-ku-bojo/ mikubobojo 107
‘des-lie down’ /si-kili/ sikikili 110
‘comp-n.inten-un.dist-spouse’ /N-o-piN-savo/ nopinsasavo 92
‘af-cont-lift’ /m-iN-kakat/ mingkakakat 105
‘af-mid-poss-go around’ /m-pog-ki-liput/ mogililiput 87
‘exas-arrive/reach’ /to-rikot/ toririkot 72
‘comp, mul.rec-be side by side’ /-in-, poi-ganding/ pinoigaganding 109
‘af, come’ /-um-, ravung/ rurumavung 102
‘catch an illness-uf-you’ /ruvang-o-ko/ ruruvangoko 112
Mother-based opacity: infixation 153
(34)
‘af-du.rec-n.ser-paddle a boat’ /m-pi-siN-alud/ misingangalud 107
‘comp-n.inten-cont-eat’ /no-ko-iN-aran/ nokoingangaran 80
‘af-diss-sleep’ /m-posiN-odop/ mosingongodop 60
‘dim-hold in hand’ /soN-onggom/ songongonggom 77
The MDT account of the data in (33) and (34) is that Eastern Kadazan
reduplication is infixing. As in Javanese, the Proot plays a crucial role. In Eastern
Kadazan, reduplication, a late morphological process, truncates the first copy
of the word down to the initial CV of the Proot and infixes the result directly
preceding the Proot in the second copy. The Proot, as in Javanese, consists of
those syllables projected from the morphological root. In the case of consonant-
initial roots, as in (33), the morphological root and the Proot are identical, and
reduplication reproduces the initial syllable. In the case of vowel-initial roots,
as in (34), the Proot can include a consonant from a preceding prefix if the
morphological root is vowel-initial; in such a case, reduplication reproduces
the syllable composed of that consonant and the root-initial vowel.
(37)
indad-an-po Proot-onset Align(Mroot,Proot)
) a. in{dad}anpo i
b. {indad}anpo *!
{da} in{dad}anpo
| |
in{dad}anpo in{dad}anpo
Further support for the infixation analysis is evidence that, completely inde-
pendent of onset overcopying issues, the input to reduplication can be a mor-
phologically complex stem. This assumption is crucial to the claim that redu-
plication is a late process. The data in (39) show that the Proot can contain the
completive infix -in- (a) as well as what appear to be denominal verb stems
(b, c):
(39) a. -in- + vaal /v-in-aal/ vi+vinaal ‘It was made up’ 101
comp-make
b. in-sada /m-pog-iN- mo+gi+ginsadano ‘He catches fish 63
vbl-fish sada-no/ regularly’
c. to-ruol15 /m-posiN-to- mosin+to+toruol ‘She is pretending 60
?-? ruol/ to have an illness’
The infix -in- is one of the outermost affixes (Hurlbut 1988:23); its redu-
plicability confirms that reduplication is a late morphological process, looking
potentially deep into the word for the Proot.
In sum, what appears initially to be onset overcopying is actually internal
reduplication of a prosodic constituent (Proot), a phenomenon for which there
is strong precedent in the form of internal reduplication of stressed syllables (as
in Chamorro; see Yu 2003 for a recent survey). We have provided independent
evidence for the two crucial parts of the analysis: reduplication is infixing, and
reduplication operates on morphologically complex constituents.
The semantics of Eastern Kadazan reduplication are also consistent with
an analysis in which reduplication is a relatively late process. According to
Hurlbut (1988:100), reduplication is basically semantically iterative, though
Mother-based opacity: infixation 155
its meaning can vary according to what other affixes are present in the word.
Together with the m-prefix, reduplication marks progressive iterative aspect;
together with completive aspect, reduplication gives the sense that the action
of the verb was undertaken without purpose (or, apparently, in bad faith). The
fact that the semantics of reduplication depends on the other affixes in the word
is consistent with its being a late morphological process.
Further support for this conclusion comes from two examples illustrating
whole-word reduplication (Hurlbut 1988). With what appears to be a purely
iterative meaning, reduplication targets all three syllables of the complex stems
mingguli in (40a) and pointaamon in (40b):
In its details this analysis is almost exactly parallel to the analysis of Javanese
laxing. What can be thought of as an æ → ε rule seems to underapply in the
output of truncation, but that is simply because the output is faithful, in its vowel
quality, to the input, where the rule is transparently not conditioned. A similar
example involving place of articulation is Gram [græm]; for those speaker who
pronounce the word Grandma as [græmma], the place of articulation in the final
consonant of Gram can be explained only by assimilation to the following -ma
of Grandma, its morphological base. Numerous other examples can be found
in Weeda 1992; see especially pp. 78ff., where Weeda discusses cases of vowel
tensing in Madurese and New York English that closely parallel Benua’s data
and involve clearly productive alternations.
-al- ∼ -ar-, which follows the first consonant, renders nasal harmony opaque.
Harmony appears to overapply to the right of the infix, where it is not condi-
tioned on the surface; the final liquid of the infix is the type of consonant that
blocks harmony.
(43)
[ŋãruliat]
⇐ Plural stem cophonology: Nasal harmony
[ŋuliat]
| ⇐ Singular stem cophonology: Nasal harmony
/-al-/ /ŋuliat/
(44)
red-apeq-an BR-Faith Ablaut IO-Faith IR-Faith
a. apeq-apeq-an *!
b. apeq-apiq-an *! *
) c. apiq-apiq-an * *
d. apiq-apeq-an *! * *
(46)
Red-murid-e *Tense/Closed BR-Ident *Lax IO-Faith
) a. mu.rid-mu.ri.d-e
b. mu.rid.-mu.ri.d-e *! * *
c. mu.rid.-mu.ri.d-e *!* *
d. mu.rid-mu.ri.d-e *! *
BRCT and MDT might appear to work equally well for these Javanese exam-
ples of over- and underapplication – although, as argued in §5.5.3, MDT makes
stronger predictions than BRCT regarding which alternations will show over-
application and which will show underapplication.
We add to this argument in the next several sections of this chapter, showing
other ways in which MDT provides a better account of reduplicative opacity
than does BRCT in general. We make three points:
Not all reduplicative opacity increases BR identity; therefore BRCT
is not sufficient
BRCT predicts kinds of identity effects that don’t occur; thus BRCT
overgenerates
MDT can handle the apparent identity effects that do occur; thus BRCT
is not necessary
What is opaque about the reduplicated form is the location of stress, which is
normally penultimate in Chamorro. But this “displacement” of stress does not
result in the base and reduplicant being more identical than they otherwise would
be; if stress were penultimate in the reduplicated form, giving us hugagándo,
base and reduplicant would be no less similar.
MDT can handle both the effects in Chamorro, in which identity clearly plays
no role, and those in Javanese, in which identity could be said to play a role, in
the same manner. By contrast, BRCT has no light to shed on Chamorro. BRCT
would presumably also have to appeal to cyclicity in some guise and would
therefore derive the opacity the way MDT does; BR correspondence would
play no role.
(48) Scopal Opacity Prediction (SOP): cophonologies can render only the
phonology of their inputs opaque
Mother ← can opacify inputs
The SOP does not follow from BRCT. As a result, BRCT makes dramatically
different predictions regarding phonological opacity in reduplication. Below we
survey three types of phonological alternations which in BRCT are predicted
to participate in over- or underapplication. We will show that only one of these
types of effects actually does participate, namely stem-internal phonology, pre-
cisely as MDT predicts.
We have already seen how MDT and BRCT handle such cases; (a) is discussed
in the section on Javanese h-deletion (§5.1.5), and (b) is discussed in the section
on Javanese laxing (§5.1.6).
At the internal base-reduplicant juncture, the nasal ending the first copy assim-
ilates in place to the consonant beginning the second copy; this alternation is
then reflected in the second copy.
MDT cannot describe this effect. There is no cophonology that could assim-
ilate the final segment of the second copy to the first segment of itself or of
the preceding copy. MDT predicts (50) not to occur. By contrast, BRCT can
easily describe it. Let us assume for the sake of exposition that in (50) the
second copy is the reduplicant. Letting “NPA” in (51) stand for the constraint(s)
responsible for requiring nasal-stop clusters to agree in place, the ranking NPA »
IO-Ident-place ensures that NPA will take place generally in the language,
and therefore in bases of reduplication as well. Satisfaction of IO-Ident-
place compels the reduplicant-final consonant to be identical to the base,
yielding overapplication of a BR-juncture effect:
..............................................
(51) /bihan-Red/ NPA IO-Ident BR-Faith
a. biham-bihan * *!
b. underapplication bihan-bihan *!
) c. overapplication biham-biham *
The BRCT analysis of this case is actually formally parallel to the BRCT
treatment of the actual opaque stem-internal Javanese alternations analyzed in
§5.4.2.1, above. The problem with this is that cases like bihan → biham-biham
do not exist. McCarthy and Prince (1995a), discussing parallel hypothetical
but unattested examples, are able to offer no formal explanation for the gap
(pp. 327–28).18
could produce this result. BRCT can easily describe the hypothetical situation
in (52b):
(53)
/[Red-tapan]-la/ BR-Faith assimilation IO-Faith
a. transparent tapan-tapal-la *! *
) b. overapplication tapal-tapal-la *
c. underapplication tapan-tapan-la *!
To our knowledge, effects like these do not occur. However, the analysis
BRCT offers of these hypothetical data is formally completely parallel to the
opaque stem-internal alternations above, predicting that they should be equally
common. BRCT misses a major cross-linguistic generalization in generat-
ing, with equal facility, overapplication effects for phonology applying stem-
internally (which occurs often), phonology applying across the BR-juncture,
and phonology applying across junctures external to reduplication (neither of
which occur reliably).
The statement that cases like those in (50) and (52) do not occur does not
mean that there are no data which superficially resemble these patterns, as seen
already in the discussion of Javanese in §5.1. To take another example, the data
from Tawala (Austronesian; Ezard 1997:38), below, might initially appear to
instantiate overapplication of an external junctural effect (as in (52)). All of
these words exhibit CVCV reduplication, which marks durative aspect. Data
from the reduplication of long roots shows that the first copy is the truncated one
(e.g. geleta ‘to arrive,’ gele-geleta (durative); Ezard 1997:41). As seen below,
suffix-induced vowel change in the second copy is replicated in the first copy
as well.
(54) a. dewa-dewa-ya /dewa-dewa-ya/ ‘. . . -3sg’
b. dewe-dewe-ya /dewa-dewa-e-ya/ ‘. . . trv-3sg’
c. dewi-dewi-yai /dewa-dewa-iyai/ ‘. . . -1pl.excl’
The two suffix types whose effect on a preceding root vowel is replicated in
reduplication are the transitivizing suffix “trv” shown in (54b), and the six
object agreement suffixes, one of which is illustrated in (54c). As a derivational
suffix, the transitivizer is expected to occur closer to the root, hierarchically
speaking, than inflection, which includes object agreement and aspect. Cross-
linguistic expectations about inflectional affix ordering, e.g. Bybee 1985, hold
that object agreement is generally closest to the verb root, followed by aspect;
tense and subject agreement are even farther from the verb root. Thus the null
hypothesis for Tawala would be that durative reduplication operates on a stem
including the verb root, any derivational morphology, and object agreement.
On this hypothesis the data in (54) are completely expected.19
Eastern Kadazan, seen earlier in §5.2.2, might also appear at first glance to
involve overapplication of an external junctural effect between prefix and redu-
plicant. As argued in §5.2.2, however, morphology again explains the pattern:
the opacity in Eastern Kadazan is due to infixation. The alternation in question –
adjunction of a prefix-final consonant into an otherwise vowel-initial Proot – is
stem-internal.
Two other examples like Eastern Kadazan, namely Tagalog and Chumash,
have in fact been analyzed in the BRCT literature as instantiating the overappli-
cation of an effect at the external juncture between prefix and reduplicant. It is
argued in Chapter 6 that the opacity in Tagalog and Chumash should be under-
stood in a parallel fashion to that of Eastern Kadazan: independent evidence
exists that both languages have internal reduplication, and that the juncture in
question is internal to the stem being reduplicated.
5.4.2.4 Summary
MDT makes restrictive predictions about what kinds of phonological effects
can over- or underapply in reduplication; BRCT predicts a much wider range of
reduplicative opacity. The predictions of MDT are borne out in the data, while
those of BRCT are not. Observations parallel to ours about the apparently false
predictions of BRCT are also made in Kiparsky (1997), in the context of a
discussion of Sanskrit reduplication.
(57) Word
|
Prefixed stem
Stem
(58) Word
|
Prefixed Rstem
Rstem
Stem Stem
This layered morphological structure also makes predictions about the redu-
plicability of the effects of phonological alternations conditioned within Stems
versus those conditioned solely in Rstems or Words. Stem-internal alternations
are predicted on this Morphological Doubling Theory analysis to be reduplica-
ble, in the sense that they apply within both daughters of the Rstem. However,
alternations applying only within Rstems or Words are not expected to be dou-
bled. These predictions are completely supported by the data.
168 Morphologically driven opacity in reduplication
Not all suffixes trigger these root-final effects; compare, for example, the exam-
ples in (61) to those in (60d). The Umlaut alternation is triggered by indepen-
dent indicative third person suffixes (Dahlstrom 1997:224); the examples in
(61), with second person (a) or conjunct paradigm (b) suffixes, do not show any
alternation in the root-final vowel:
(61) /ke-nepa:-pwa/ ke-nepa:-pwa ke-nepa-nepa:pwa 224–25
‘you (pl) sleep’
/na:kw a:-waki/ na:kw a:-wa:či na:kw a- na:kw a:wa:či 224
‘they leave; conjunct’
Stem Stem
Umlaut, truncation ⇒ ⇐ Umlaut, etc. (certain suffixes)
… Root Sfx Root Sfx
Alternations like Umlaut therefore apply internal to the stem that is doubled and
(in the first instance) truncated in reduplication. Insofar as reduplicative trun-
cation obscures the suffixal triggers for these processes, they apply opaquely.
As Dahlstrom puts it (p. 223), “bisyllabic reduplication copies the output of
the rule but not the environment which triggers the rule.” This is precisely the
description of daughter-internal opacity in MDT.
the initial consonant, rather than both copies. Equating Initial Change formally
with prefixation, Dahlstrom observes that it is outside, not inside, the scope of
reduplication. Initial Change turns initial /a/, /e/, /i/ to /e:/, and initial /o/ to /we/.
The examples below illustrate Initial Change in its function of marking conjunct
participle inflection in reduplicated words. As seen, Initial Change affects the
initial vowel in the word, resulting in phonological divergence between the two
copies of the root:
(63) /amw-/ e:mwa-h-amw-a:čihi ‘the ones whom they (repeatedly) eat’ 222
/ašam-/ e:ša-h-ašam-a:či ‘that which he (repeatedly) feeds him’ 222
/kanawi-/ ke:na-kanawi-ta ‘the one who gives speeches’ 222
(64) Word
|
Prefixed stem
⇐ Initial change (Prefix Cophonology)
Rstem
Stem Stem
Initial Change applies external to the stem and is therefore not present in
the input to reduplication; we correctly predict no “overapplication” in those
cases where Initial Change happens to affect the first of the two daughters in
the reduplication construction.
A second alternation applying at the Prefix-Rstem juncture is /t/-epenthesis,
triggered by VV sequences across the Prefix-Stem or Prefix-Rstem juncture:
(66) Word
|
Prefixed stem /t/-insertion (Prefix Cophonology)
Rstem
Stem Stem
(68) aškw at-amwa aškw a-h-aškw at-amwa ‘he has [food] left over’ 217
a:mi:-wa a:mi-h-a:mi:wa ‘he moves camp’ 218
opye:ni opye-h-opye:ni ‘slowly’ (preverb) 218
amw-e:wa amwe-h-amwe:wa ‘he eats him’ 219
ay-o:ya:ni ayo-h-ayo:ya:ni ‘I use it; conjunct’ 219
i-wa iwa-h-iwa ‘he says’ 219
Many prefixed reduplicated forms show both /t/- and /h/-epenthesis (see, for
example, (59) and (67); neither alternation over- or underapplies in the service
of greater base-reduplicant identity. This is exactly as MDT predicts; neither
inserted /t/ nor inserted /h/ is present in the input to reduplication, i.e. within
either daughter of the Rstem. Because it is a junctural alternation, /h/-insertion
has to be handled higher up, either at the Rstem or, as argued below, at the Word
cophonology.
[do] not provide an explanation for why reduplication should have this ten-
dency to late ordering while other morphological processes such as suffixation,
infixation, etc. typically apply before most if not all phonological rules. The
reason appears to be the “iconic” nature of reduplication. (p. 441)
Since the writing of this passage, of course, many cases have been docu-
mented of phonology applying to the inputs to affixation and other morpho-
logical processes; indeed the entire literature on cyclicity is of this kind. It is
difficult, however, to evaluate Kenstowicz’s claim that opacity is more com-
mon in reduplication than elsewhere. Even if this is true, it is still not obvious
that a grammatical BR-identity requirement is responsible. The prevalence of
174 Morphologically driven opacity in reduplication
junctures (a). NPA also applies at the reduplicant-base juncture, as in (b). Here,
we see that by virtue of the ranking BR-Faith, NPA » IO-Faith, assimilated
place features of the reduplicant-final nasal are backcopied to the corresponding
nasal in the base.
/m-pi-siN-alud/ misingangalud
The fact that the prefix-final consonant, “ng,” is doubled along with the
following root-initial vowel in this example could be described as a case of
backcopying overapplication. In fact, McCarthy and Prince (1995a) give pre-
cisely such an analysis to parallel phenomena in Chumash and Tagalog. Using
Eastern Kadazan data in place of Chumash or Tagalog, their analysis would sit-
uate the reduplicant immediately preceding the root and require the reduplicant
to be a light syllable, as below:
An autosegmental analysis would spread a root node (or all of the features it
dominates) to the suffixal consonant position. In Optimality Theory, a variety of
approaches exist to accomplish the same representational end. One possibility
is to rank Dep,the ban on feature insertion, higher than the ban on multiple
indexation of output features, which would favor feature sharing (copying) over
The question of backcopying 179
the insertion of a completely new set of consonant features. The other possibility
is to hand the task of copying over to the constraints compelling string-internal
correspondence (Walker 2000a; Hansson 2001; Rose & Walker 2001) between
segments meeting specified thresholds of similarity and proximity.23 Either
approach will result in a representation like that in (77), in which the suffix
consonant features are co-indexed with those of the root-final consonant:
5.7.3 Implications
Most of the examples of backcopying in morphological reduplication cited in
the BRCT literature turn out instead to be reanalyzable in one of two directions.
Either the phenomenon in question is actually close-range phonological assim-
ilation of a type attested outside of reduplication (and therefore not reducible
to BR identity), or the phenomenon in question is illusory, the result of mor-
phological misanalysis. Chapter 6 is devoted to demonstrating the latter for
two famous cases of apparent backcopying, and to arguing that for these, as
well as for the cases discussed in this chapter, morphological doubling, sans
BR-identity constraints, is the right analysis of morphological reduplication.
6 Case studies
6.1 Tagalog
Like many other Austronesian languages, Tagalog has partial reduplication
in the vicinity of the root, as well as a phonological process of nasal fusion
operating at prefix junctures. The interaction of these two phenomena creates
opacity in reduplication.
181
182 Case studies
McCarthy and Prince (1995a) cite the forms in (1) in support of BRCT’s
prediction of backcopying overapplication. Nasal fusion, conditioned by some
ŋ-final prefixes, converts a sequence of /ŋ/ + /p,k,t,s/ to a single nasal consonant
at the relevant place of articulation (/m,ng,n,n/); nasal fusion also optionally
collapses /ŋ-b/ to /m/ and /ŋ-ʔ/ to /ŋ/).1
McCarthy and Prince (1995a) take the position, consistent with descriptions
in the primary sources (e.g. Schachter & Otanes 1972; marked “SO” above)
that Tagalog has root reduplication. They assume morphological inputs of the
type in (2):
(2) /paŋ-Red-putul/
/naŋ-Red-isda/
Given these inputs, McCarthy and Prince are naturally concerned with the fact
that the prefix affects the shape not only of the reduplicant, which it immediately
precedes, but also the base, to which it is not adjacent. In (1a), the result of nasal
fusion at the reduplicant-base juncture is reflected in the base as well, and in
(1b), the final consonant of the prefix is actually doubled, serving as onset not
only to the reduplicant vowel but also to the base.
McCarthy and Prince’s (1995a:60) solution to the doubling problem is back-
copying: the reduplicant interacts transparently with the prefix and, as a result
of BR identity, the phonological results are mirrored in the base: /paŋ-Red-
putul/ → [pa-mu-mutul].
(3)
Input to reduplication: [pamutul] (< /paŋ-putul/) [naŋisda] (< /naŋ-isda/)
↓ ↓
Output of internal reduplication: [pa-mu-mutul] [na-ŋi-ŋisda]
These data suggest that the maŋ-stem is a subconstituent of the word con-
taining both maŋ- and reduplication, whereas the bare reduplicated stem is not.
The second, semantic, argument supports this conclusion. Although the
semantic evidence is not always cut-and-dried, in examples like those in (4)
the meaning of the word with both reduplication and maŋ- is based on the
meaning of the maŋ- stem in a way that supports treating the maŋ-stem as a
subconstituent of the word. The prefixed stem ma-mayan means ‘to reside in a
town’; ma-ma-mayan is the nominalization of that verb. The same relationship
holds between maŋibig (< /maŋ-ibig/) ‘to court, be a suitor’ and its nominalized
counterpart ma-ŋi-ŋibig ‘beau, suitor.’
The third argument in favor of infixation is phonological. Some stems
containing disyllabic prefixes, when subject to reduplication, show variation
between stem reduplication and second syllable reduplication (which happens
to target the prefix). The data below are taken from Schachter and Otanes
1972:370:5
(5)
Basic form Contemplated aspect
UR, no reduplication Stem reduplication Second syllable reduplication
its Proot. In Tagalog, the Proot consists of the morphological root plus a pre-
ceding prefix-final consonant to serve as a syllable onset, where needed. An
analysis of an apparent overcopying example, maŋiŋibig ‘suitor,’ is provided
below. This structure is composed of three independent constructions: prefixa-
tion, which produces maŋ-stems, truncation (which produces the truncated stem
type called for in the first daughter), and reduplication:
(6) ma{ŋi}{ŋibig}
← Infixation
{ŋi} ma{ŋibig}
Truncation → | |
ma{ŋibig} ma{ŋibig}
| | ← Prefixation
/maŋ -ibigRt/ /maŋ -ibigRt/
Once it is recognized that prefixes are present in the input to reduplication and
that the reduplicant is a truncated version of the Proot, which can include prefixal
material, the doubling of onsets and of nasal fusion in Tagalog reduplication
is no longer a mystery. Rather, it is normal application of phonology, rendered
opaque by truncation. No backcopying is required.
6.2 Chumash
Chumash, like Tagalog, has been described as exhibiting root reduplication
with opaque overcopying of prefix material to the left of the reduplicative
prefix (McCarthy & Prince 1995a). Our investigations have revealed a very
different picture: instead, as we found for Tagalog and, in Chapter 5, for Eastern
Kadazan, Chumash exhibits infixing reduplication which targets the Proot of a
morphologically complex word. Our conclusions echo those of Cole (1994) and
Downing (1998d), both of whom also recognize the role of the prosodic stem in
Chumash reduplication. Downing, in particular, observes that an infixing Pstem
analysis makes McCarthy and Prince’s backcopying analysis unnecessary.
Our sources for Chumash data are Applegate 1972; 1976, on the Ineseño
dialect, and Wash 1995, on the Barbareño dialect. We will refer to these data
sources in examples as A72, A76, and W95, respectively.6 Most discussions in
the literature are based on the Ineseño dialect; any examples from Barbareño
will be explicitly identified.
The data in (7) illustrate the apparently prefixing stem reduplication in
nouns and verbs. Reduplication pluralizes nouns and renders verbs repetitive,
186 Case studies
(7)
čh umaš ‘islander’ čh um+čh umaš ‘islanders’ A76:273
k-ni-č’eq 1subj-trans-tear kni+č’eq+č’eq ‘I’m tearing it up’ A76:282
k-wi-č’eq 1subj-by hitting-tear kwi+č’eq+č’eq ‘I pound it to pieces’ A72:387
Chumash stem reduplication has played a prominent role in the BRCT liter-
ature because of data like those in (8). McCarthy and Prince (1995a:308) cite
these forms, taken (apparently via Mester 1986) from Applegate (1976:279),
as manifesting backcopying overapplication in reduplication, a signature pre-
diction of BRCT:8
reduplication as well. Surface forms and glosses are taken from Wash 1995
(pp. 31–32, 167):
(10)
Plain stem Reduplicated
(13)
Input to reduplication: [si.šex.peč] (> /s-iš-expeč/) [k’anš] (> /k-ʔanš/)
↓ ↓
Output of internal [si-šex-šexpeč] [k’an-k’anš]
reduplication:
{ex} si{expe}
Truncation ⇒ | |
si{expe} {expe}
Stem-formation ⇒ | | ⇐ Stem-formation
/s-i-expe/i /s-i-expe/i
Chumash 189
(16)
Gloss Unreduplicated Reduplicated Source
(17)
No reduplication Reduplication
(18)
Without reduplication With reduplication Gloss Source
The same word can contain both Pstem-external and Pstem-internal Inner
prefixes. As expected, reduplication targets the Pstem, duplicating the Pstem-
internal, but not the Pstem-external, Inner prefixes. Inner prefixes are, as before,
in boldface in the unreduplicated forms:
(19)
Without reduplication With reduplication Gloss Source
The form in (19b) shows that the final consonant of a Pstem-external prefix will
incorporate into the Pstem, as expected, in case the Pstem would otherwise be
V-initial.
When more than one Inner prefix is present in a word, the Pstem contains the
leftmost Inner prefix and all following material. In (20), the root is preceded by
a string of up to three Inner prefixes, each independently known to be Pstem-
internal. The reduplicant (in bold) is the initial CVC portion of this Pstem.
The form in (20c) shows that Pstems which include Inner prefixes behave in
the same way as those based on roots. If the Pstem would be vowel-initial
but is preceded by a consonant-final prefix (subject to the dialectal conditions
discussed above), the consonant joins the Pstem. In the unreduplicated form,
Inner prefixes are shown in boldface and the Pstem is delimited with curly
brackets:
Chumash 193
(20)
Without reduplication With reduplication Gloss Source
The Prefixation hypothesis, with ordering as its only possible account of the
split behavior of Inner prefixes, makes a striking morphological prediction: all
reduplicating Inner prefixes should be ordered after (inside, closer to the stem
than) all nonreduplicating Inner prefixes. The Infixation hypothesis makes no
such prediction, as it draws no structural morphological distinction between
Pstem-internal and Pstem-external Inner prefixes.
The ordering prediction of the Prefixation account appears to be inconsistent
with the information provided by Applegate on the relative linear order of the
Inner prefixes. Applegate partitions Inner prefixes into position classes which
correlate roughly with syntactic function and semantic transparency. Although
there are not as many examples containing both types of Inner prefixes as
one would like, the forms that do exist give the clear impression that there
is no fixed linear ordering between reduplicating and nonreduplicating Inner
prefixes.
Example (19) illustrates words in which nonreduplicating Inner prefixes pre-
cede reduplicating Inner prefixes. These data are consistent with the Ordering
194 Case studies
hypothesis. But Inner prefixes also occur in the opposite order, contrary to
the Ordering hypothesis. The examples in (22) illustrate the ubiquitous, highly
productive Causative prefix su- seen earlier in (17), (20). A Pstem-internal,
reduplicating prefix (see, for example, (17)), su- can precede other prefixes
which Applegate classifies as nonreduplicating or, in the terms used here, Pstem-
external. In (22), reduplicating (Pstem-internal) prefixes are subscripted as “R”;
nonreduplicating (Pstem-external) prefixes (on which see (18)) are subscripted
as “N”. One or two of the Inner prefixes in (22) are unclassified, due to a lack
of sufficient data.
(22)
a. /maqR -suR -niN -apayRt / maqsunapay ‘to raise a line, string a bow’ A72:379
/wi-suR -niN -apayRt / wisunapay ‘[sea] to cast ashore’ A72:383
b. /k=suR -watiN -lok’inRt / ksuwatilok’in ‘I cut it in passing A72:368
(I’ll be back for it later)’
c. /suR -tiN -wyRt / sutiwy ‘to sing a charm, spell’ A72:324
/suR -wašR -tiN -aq-peyRt / šuwešteqpey ‘to mend, fix’ A72:383
When verbs like these are subject to reduplication, the leftmost Pstem-internal
prefixes define the left edge of the Pstem, which includes all following material,
including Inner prefixes that would otherwise be Pstem-external. This can be
seen in example (23), in which the Pstem-internal prefix xul- precedes the
Pstem-external transitive prefix ni- (see (18e)). The reduplicant is xun, which
consists of the prefix xul plus the initial consonant of ni-, with which the final
l of the prefix fuses.14
The inescapable conclusion from examples like (22) and (23) is that the split
in reduplicability within Inner prefixes is not reflected in their ordering relations;
both reduplicating > nonreduplicating and nonreduplicating > reduplicating
orders are well-attested in Ineseño Chumash. Furthermore, a normally nonredu-
plicating prefix can be “promoted” to a reduplicating prefix if it is sandwiched
between a reduplicating prefix and the stem. This behavior follows naturally
from the Pstem analysis; it is contrary to the predictions of the Prefixation
account, and therefore supports the Infixation account of reduplication.15
Although evidence from semantics is not strong, it tends to support the claim
of the Infixation account that reduplication is a late morphological process tar-
geting whole words, rather than having scope over only stems (as the Prefixation
Chumash 195
(24)
Plain Reduplicated Source
6.2.7 Summary
In Chumash, as argued above, the whole word is subject to reduplication; the
first copy is truncated to the initial CVC portion of its Pstem, which is then
infixed before the Pstem of the second copy. The Pstem contains the stem and
any Pstem-internal prefixes (as well as other prefixes sandwiched in between).
Because of consonant fusion and onset requirements, Pstems can also contain
the final consonant of a preceding prefix which is otherwise Pstem-external.
The potential mismatch between Pstem and morphological constituent is what
gives rise to the appearance of onset overcopying, or overapplication of conso-
nant fusion. However, both effects are normal application within the MDT
analysis, for which we have provided independent support from Chumash
morphology.
196 Case studies
197
198 Final issues
trigger, not (for example) the farthest. Recent work in Optimality Theory has
cast assimilation in terms of correspondence; for Walker 2000a; b; Hansson
2001; Walker to appear, assimilation occurs when a grammar imposes identity
requirements on corresponding segments; a key component of the theory is that
segment-to-segment correspondence is established in the first place only among
segments that are sufficiently similar and sufficiently proximal in the input. In
this approach, as in the autosegmental approach, assimilation can occur at a dis-
tance only if it also occurs locally; assimilation is therefore always proximal. In
Turkish vowel harmony, for example, a harmonic suffix agrees with the closest
root vowel, not the farthest, as can be demonstrated with disharmonic roots:
thus anne ‘mother’ is anne-ler and elma is elma-lar. There is no language with
vowel harmony in which these plurals could be anne-lar and elma-ler (unless
dissimilation is involved, but then the identity of the initial vowel would be
irrelevant).
The view of phonological segment copying adopted in this book is that phono-
logical copying is an extreme form of phonological assimilation. It conforms to
the same generalizations about proximity, and it is handled with the same the-
oretical mechanisms. What distinguishes phonological copying from ordinary
epenthesis of a default segment is the phonological correspondence established
between the introduced segment and its nearest comparable neighbor. Zuraw
(2002) has observed that phonological correspondence itself can be a desidera-
tum; where faithfulness to input features is not an issue, an epenthetic segment
which corresponds to another is, from that perspective, more desirable than one
which does not.
To illustrate the ways in which phonological segment copying patterns with
phonological assimilation, rather than with morphological reduplication, we
may fruitfully contrast Hausa noun pluralization (2a) and Ponapean prefixation
(2b) with Madurese noun pluralization (2c):
The Hausa requirement that syllables begin with consonantal onsets leads to
consonant epenthesis in the plural suffix.1 As discussed in Chapter 1, phonologi-
cal copying derives from the need for this consonant to be featurally specified; as
The morphological purpose of reduplication 199
expected in phonological assimilation, it draws all of its features from the nearest
consonant. In a similarly cross-linguistically common pattern, Ponapean inserts
an epenthetic copy vowel when “an impermissible consonant cluster is cre-
ated through the process of affixation” (in these examples, involving the prefix
ak-, meaning ‘to make a demonstration of’) and the following syllable is heavy
(Rehg 1981:70, 92). The epenthetic vowel takes on the quality of the vowel in
the immediately following syllable. Proximity of this kind is typical of phono-
logical copying generally. It is not, however, a requirement of morphological
reduplication, as illustrated by the “opposite-edge” morphological reduplica-
tion occurring in Madurese (Stevens 1985, via McCarthy & Prince 1995a:278),
where, as mentioned in Chapter 5, the first copy in plural noun reduplication
is truncated to its last – not its initial – syllable. Although opposite-side effects
are a minority pattern in reduplication, the important point is that they exist at
all; opposite-edge effects never occur in phonological assimilation.2
In summary, phonological copying serves a phonological purpose; it involves
single segments, and when copying is involved, the copying is proximal. In all
of these ways, phonological copying is distinct from morphological copying,
which involves morphological constituents – often long strings, not single seg-
ments – is potentially nonproximal, and serves a morphological purpose.
Despite being different – or perhaps because they are different – phonolog-
ical copying and morphological reduplication are not mutually exclusive. For
example, Hausa pluractional formation (discussed in Chapters 1 and 4) involves
both phenomena. Pluractional verbs are formed in Hausa by doubling the verb
stem and truncating the first copy to CVC. This is morphological reduplication.
The final consonant of the truncated first copy, however, is subject to a form of
phonological copying: it assimilates totally to the following consonant, under
appropriate conditions discussed in Chapter 4. This is phonological copying.
[ ]F [ ]F
7.4 CV reduplication
Although for the majority of examples it is clear whether phonology or mor-
phology is driving the observed duplication, it is inevitable that there will be
cases where the two analyses are similarly compelling, without a good way to
choose between them. The area of greatest potential ambiguity involves dupli-
cation of strings small enough to be manageble phonologically but large enough
to be plausible as truncated morphological constituents – in particular, CV redu-
plication. CV reduplication has generally been analyzed as morphological in
202 Final issues
this work (see, for example, Paamese, in Chapter 2, Roviana and Tawala, in
Chapter 3; Tohono O’odham and Chamorro, in Chapter 4; Eastern Kadazan,
in Chapter 5, Tagalog, in Chapter 6, and others). In some of these cases, e.g.
Paamese, there is evidence from allomorphy that the process is clearly morpho-
logical reduplication; in other cases, we have simply made the assumption that
morphological reduplication is taking place, in the absence of evidence to the
contrary.
It is theoretically possible that, in the unusual event that vowel and con-
sonant epenthesis are conditioned simultaneously and copy epenthesis is pre-
ferred to default segment epenthesis, CV duplication could be derived phono-
logically. In Miya, for example, pluractional stems are formed in a variety
of ways which Bissell (2002) unifies under the analysis of mora prefixation.
As described by Schuh (1998), CaC roots form the pluractional via vowel
lengthening, e.g. tlakə → tláakə ‘scrape’ (p. 176), while for all other CVC
roots the pluractional is formed through Ca- prefixation, where the C is a
phonological copy of the root-initial consonant, e.g. pa-pə́rà ‘cut’ (p. 175),
kwa-kwı́yà ‘catch’ (p. 176). The lengthening evident in tláakə could be seen as
copy vowel epenthesis, while the prefix consonant in pa-pə́rà could be seen as
copy consonant epenthesis. If both happened to co-occur in the same syllable,
purely phonological processes could add up to what appears to be CV redu-
plication. A similar phenomenon occurs in the English construction Yu (2003)
terms “Homeric infixation,” in which the infix -ma- follows a metrical foot:
saxophone → saxa-ma-phone, secretary → secre-ma-tary. The infix imposes
another condition, however, which is that at least one syllable must follow. Thus
in cases such as music or tuba, which consist of only one foot, consonant dupli-
cation and schwa (spelled “a”) insertion take place to satisfy the demands of the
infix: mu-sa-ma-sic, tu-ba-ma-ba. This instantiates the phonological insertion
of a CV syllable; it is not exactly CV duplication, since the vowel is fixed, as
in Miya, but, like Miya, it comes close.
The potential for CV duplication to occur for phonological reasons is con-
sistent with the essential bifurcation we have argued for between phonological
duplication and morphological reduplication. The ambiguous nature of CV
duplication is also consistent with evidence from language change. Niepokuj
(1997) has argued for four stages in the diachronic evolution of reduplication.
Stage 1 is total; in Stage 2, one copy is reduced. In Stage 3, reduplication
is affixal (= partial), and by Stage 4, the reduplicant is so reduced as to be
realized as gemination. Although Niepokuj does not have evidence from recon-
structions to support all aspects of this schema, she does cite two cases that
strongly support the diachronic path from Stage 3 CV prefixing reduplication
The question of rhyme 203
Pacoh nickname is formed by choosing two words which are commonly used
together . . . the second of which rhymes with the given name of the person
being nicknamed. The appropriate sex-title, ku ‘male’ or kán ‘female’ is then
chosen to precede the paired words and the given name.”
This example, while compelling in its illustration of the aesthetic role rhyme
plays for humans, does not pass the substitution test: it would be very sur-
prising to find a pattern which phonologically resembled the Pacoh nickname
construction but instead formed, for example, agentive compounds. Our take on
the Pacoh construction is based on an observation made by Ourn and Haiman
2000, namely that in some cases, instances of a particular grammatical con-
struction that happen to rhyme are preferred stylistically to ones that do not.
Apte (1968), for example, describes a productive construction in Marathi
which couples words whose meanings are the opposite of each other and which
rhyme, e.g. jewha-tewha ‘every now and then, lit. when-then’ (p. 54). Alliter-
ation is common in synonym compounds, as well, e.g. in the Khmer examples
in (9) (from Ourn & Haiman 2000, discussed in Chapter 2); alliteration is not,
however, an absolute requirement of the construction.
In (10a), the modified noun ‘hardwood palm’ ends in the consonant n; the
agreeing adjective takes an n suffix as well. Were it modifying a noun ending
in a different consonant, it would end in the corresponding consonant. In (10b),
the head noun ‘days’ ends in h; the concord element in the preceding adjective
ends in the consonant h.
The concord elements in (10) are assigned numbers in morpheme glosses.
These numbers represent noun classes. Dobrin makes the important point that
the concord in Arapeshan is mediated, to a very high degree, by noun class,
as analyzed by Aronoff (1992; 1994). Following Fortune (1942), Aronoff and
Dobrin posit thirteen noun classes in Arapeshan. Masculine and feminine human
nouns are assigned to fixed classes (7 and 4, respectively), regardless of the final
consonant they end in. Most other nouns have class membership consistent with
their final consonant.
s-agreement. The “C” position in words agreeing with s-final nouns is instan-
tiated with s.
example is the ‘with’ suffix /-lI/: yemek-oda-lı ‘endowed with a dining room,’
not *yemek oda-sı-lı. What these facts show is that the constraint barring *yemek
oda-sı-sı cannot be a prohibition on adjacent identical morphs; it is something
more arbitrary and unrelated to phonological form. Given that languages can
ban arbitrary sequences of morphemes (as position class languages attest in the
extreme), careful statistical analysis would have to be undertaken to show that
the RMC is not an illusion, the result of linguists paying attention to morpheme
co-occurrence constraints when the morphemes are identical, and not paying
as much attention otherwise. Another window onto the psychological reality
of the RMC could be reanalysis; if languages that used to allow sequences of
identical morphs start banning them, it could be that the RMC has reared its
head. According to Yoquelet (2004), however, synchronic instances of what
appears to be the RMC tend strongly, if not universally, to derive historically
from morphemes that have acquired multiple functions but retain the distribu-
tional properties of their shared ancestor. If Yoquelet is right, then the RMC is
not a real grammatical constraint even diachronically.
Anti-identity effects have been claimed to hold elsewhere in morphology
as well. Kurisu (2001) argues that much nonconcatenative morphology can be
reduced to an anti-identity requirement holding between words, where one word
is the morphological base of the other. If no overt affixation marks the derived
word, anti-identity requires some other modification to take place. Kurisu pro-
poses that the ranking of anti-identity requirements in the markedness hierarchy
determines what modification will be performed: truncation, ablaut, metathesis,
stress shift, etc. Even if we set aside the cases in which homophony does occur
in paradigms, however, Kurisu’s approach cannot account for cases in which
truncation, ablaut and other effects co-occur with overt affixation which itself is
sufficient to ensure non-homophony between related words. Further, Kurisu’s
approach incorrectly predicts that words in which anti-identity forces modifica-
tion should differ minimally, in being less marked along some single dimension,
from their nonderived counterparts. In any event, all of Kurisu’s data can be
handled by input–output dissimilation; it is universally recognized in generative
phonology (including Optimality Theory) that there is phonological correspon-
dence between inputs and outputs.
It is beyond the scope of this chapter to review all of the claims in the
literature to the effect that the synchronic grammar acts to avoid identity across
related words, a topic that has recently come into focus (see, for example,
Crosswhite 1997; 1999; Suzuki 1999). We can only suggest that such claims
be strongly scrutinized.5 Opening the door to phonological correspondence,
whether used to enforce or to prohibit identity, endows the theory with a great
212 Final issues
deal more descriptive power, much of which is clearly unnecessary, e.g. the
ability to describe overapplication of junctural effects (see Chapter 5, §5.5.2),
backcopying (see Chapter 5, §5.8), and other unattested phenomena.
1 Introduction
1. Weber (1989) characterizes the meaning of adverbial clause reduplication as “rep-
etition of the event referred to by the reduplicated element” (p. 323).
2. The Hausa data are taken from Newman 2000 (p. 424); the Amele data are from
Roberts 1991 (p. 135), and the Warlpiri data are based on Nash 1986 (p. 130).
3. On the quality of the prefixal vowel, see Akinlabi 1993; Pulleyblank 1998; Alderete
et al. 1999.
4. Other languages cited by Landau as having similar verb copying effects include
Haitian, Vata, Yoruba, Brazilian Portuguese, Yiddish, and Russian.
5. See Raimy 2000; Frampton 2003 for a phonological approach to reduplication that
does not invoke a Red morpheme.
6. One exception is the literature on a-templatic reduplication, which offers criteria
for distinguishing templatic from a-templatic morphological reduplication. See, for
example, Gafos 1998b; Urbanczyk 1998; Hendricks 1999, as well as Zuraw 2002.
We return to this issue in Chapter 7.
7. Following McCarthy and Prince 1995a, this conviction has been bolstered by
renewed attention to apparent identity effects described by Wilbur 1973; Moravcsik
1978; Marantz 1982; Clements 1985; Kiparsky 1986; Mester 1986; Steriade 1988;
McCarthy and Prince 1993; 1995a; 1999b, and elsewhere. These will be discussed
at length in subsequent chapters.
8. Chechen data are presented in the practical orthography developed by Johanna
Nichols; for more information, see http://socrates.berkeley.edu/∼chechen.
9. See Chapter 2 for a fuller discussion of Sye and other cases of divergent allomorphy.
The same general phenomenon (in Sanskrit and other languages) is also discussed
in Saperstein 1997.
10. Blust (2001) argues, with examples from Thao (Formosan), that triplication often
does have an iconic function of intensifying whatever meaning is associated with
ordinary reduplication.
11. For recent overviews of Optimality Theory, see Kager 1999; McCarthy 2002b. It is
important to note that the use of Optimality Theory in this work differs from standard
implementations in two ways: (a) some practitioners of Optimality Theory reject the
use of cophonologies, though all admit morphologically conditioned phonology in
some guise, and (b) the Base-Reduplicant Correspondence Theory (BRCT) devel-
oped for Optimality Theory by McCarthy and Prince 1995a is not used here; MDT
213
214 Notes to pages 21–42
13. See Saperstein 1997 for an earlier proposal which allows the two daughters in
reduplication to differ in stem type.
14. Steriade (1988:76), citing Wiesemann 1972, describes a similar process in Kaingang,
where pluralization can be marked by g-insertion, raising a penultimate vowel,
and/or reduplication.
15. -eʔ and -oʔ are infinitive suffixes; -doʔ is a combination of object marker (-do-) and
infinitive suffix. See Roberts 1987:272, 281ff. for discussion of the generalizations
over which vowels will be replaced by which other vowels.
16. Tier replacement is the analysis given in Inkelas 1998 of “consonant extraction”
in Modern Hebrew (see, for example, Bat-El 1994 and references therein) and is
also arguably involved in the well-known Javanese habitual-repetitive construction,
about which Yip has written extensively (see, for example, Yip 1997; 1998), and
in which a particular vowel in one of the copies is replaced; thus eliŋ → elaŋ-eliŋ
‘remember,’ tuku → tuka-tuku ‘buy,’ udan → udan-uden ‘rain’ (Yip 1997).
17. For an analysis in this same spirit of Sanskrit reduplication in terms of different
stem allomorphs, see Saperstein 1997.
18. Crowley notes (p. 199) that verb roots beginning with t and k exhibit allomorphy
only in the active, not in the stative.
19. We are grateful to Terry Crowley for discussing the Sye data with us.
20. Crowley (1982) notes (p. 155) that for verbs which participate in both CV- and
CVCV- reduplication, it is typical for CV- reduplication to be associated with
habitual/random or habitual/uninterrupted semantics, and for the CVCV- variant to
be associated with detransitivizing or habitual/interrupted meanings. However, this
association does not hold for verbs which reduplicate in only one way.
21. Singh calls these “partial reduplication”; however, as no truncation appears to be
associated with the process, we use the more standard term of “echo reduplica-
tion.” This particular pattern of echo reduplication, in which the onset of the sec-
ond member is replaced by a labial, occurs throughout Asia; see Abbi 1991 for
a typological overview, and §2.2.4 for discussion of echo formation as Melodic
Overwriting.
22. See also Malkiel’s (1959) discussion of the semantics of irreversible binomials.
detract from our basic observation that different morphological constructions can
be associated with different cophonologies.
3. Some reduplication constructions combine bare morphemes, not derived stems, and
thus do not have cophonologies associated with the daughters; see especially the
discussion of affix reduplication in Chapter 2. The construction in (11) should be
understood as the maximally complex reduplication construction.
4. Only full-grade forms are shown here. Zero-grade allomorphs of the root may
exhibit other differences between the two copies, which Steriade (1988) argues to
be a consequence of differences in the syllabification of full vs. zero grades. See
also Saperstein 1997 for an analysis more in line with MDT’s double stem selection
approach.
5. Transcriptions are based on Weeda 1992: 162–63. “HBC” stands for the Hooper
Bay/Chevak dialect; other forms are from the General Central Alaskan Yupik com-
plex of dialects.
6. Alderete (1999; 2001) invokes input–output anti-identity constraints to drive accen-
tual dominance effects, but the phenomenon he is analyzing is one of neutralization
(accent deletion), rather than dissimilation proper.
7. Hausa data are presented in Hausa orthography, with three exceptions: vowel length
is marked with a colon rather than a macron; following Newman 2000, tapped /r̃/
is marked with a tilde to distinguish it from retroflex /r/; following Hausa linguistic
tradition and Newman 2000, Low tone is marked with a grave accent (High is
unmarked).
8. The coronals /z/ and /d/ are related via coronal palatalization, which applies before
front vowels.
9. Truncation and vowel raising do not occur in polysyllabic bases. Niepokuj
(1997:24ff.) takes this as evidence that monosyllabic and polysyllabic reduplication
are different morphological constructions (prefixing and suffixing, respectively).
10. A possible alternative analysis, after Downing 2004, might be to analyze the Tarok
pattern as the result of the base melody being mapped, via the regular left-to-right
association algorithm of the language, to the prosodic word which contains both
base and reduplicant. However, for monosyllabic Low-toned bases the tone pattern
in reduplication is Low-Mid; according to Sibomana 1980:201, Low-Mid is not a
legal result of regular tone distribution.
11. In response to similar ranking paradoxes in reduplication in other languages (e.g.
Kwakwala and Lushootseed), Struijke (2000a; 2000b) has proposed Existential
Faithfulness (∃-Faith), which differs from standard input–output faithfulness in
being satisfied as long as input structures have correspondents anywhere in the
output; they could be in the reduplicant or in the base. While ∃-Faith would permit
BRCT to describe the Tarok facts, it is argued on other grounds in Chapter 4 that
∃-Faith is not a generally viable approach to reduplicative phonology.
12. The adoption of reduplicant-specific markedness constraints, e.g. *V]red , might
seem to be another alternative, but it is also essentially identical to introducing
cophonologies and just as incompatible with the foundational assumptions of BRCT.
13. In any case, nothing in BRCT, even with GTT, could rule out the reduplication of,
for example, a truncated nickname (e.g. Jonathan → Jon → Jon-Jon), which is
Notes to pages 91–101 217
7. Since dissimilation occurs only at the juncture, a full account of this effect in any
framework must include a mechanism to restrict the alternation to derived environ-
ments. On derived environment effects, see Mascaró 1976; 1982b; 1993 and, more
recently, Burzio 1997; Lubowicz 1999; Inkelas 2000; Anttila in press; Cho in press.
8. A potential alternative to this ranking paradox, ∃-Faith, is discussed and rejected
in §4.4.
9. Cohn and McCarthy’s (1998) more detailed account of Indonesian stress utilizes
the following constraint on the relationship between two PrWds (after Chomsky &
Halle 1968) to force stress subordination in compounds:
Nuc-Str
In PrWd1 PrWd2 , PrWd2 is more prominent
A general Culminativity constraint is sufficient for the argument here, but see
their paper for justification of Nuc-Str.
10. Kenstowicz (1995) and Cohn and McCarthy (1998) use a comprehensive BR-Max
constraint.
11. One might wish to account for the seeming typological oddity of the opposite pattern,
whereby stress demotion would occur in reduplication but not in compounds. At
present, however, its existence is predicted possible by any theory, including BRCT,
which could derive it as a standard TETU effect by changing the relative ranking of
BR-Faith and IO-Faith with respect to markedness. Ruling it out would require
as yet undiscovered meta-constraints on constraint indexation/reranking.
12. Another potential contributing factor to underapplication is opaque interactions
between daughter and mother cophonologies; we discuss a case from Javanese in
Chapter 5.
13. “TETU” stands for “The Emergence of the Unmarked,” the phenomenon whereby
a low-ranking markedness constraint M can have an effect when a higher-ranking
constraint, obedience to which normally prevents satisfaction of M, is itself nec-
essarily violated in some context, permitting satisfaction of M. See McCarthy and
Prince 1994a; Alderete et al. 1999.
14. The candidate bukú-bukú-ña, where stress shifts in both Red and Base, is ruled out
by a constraint favoring initial stress. See Kenstowicz 1995 and Cohn and McCarthy
1998 for details.
15. Not all cases previously described as underapplication will yield to the same analysis
as Klamath. See Chapter 5 for cases of underapplication in Javanese which have a
different origin.
16. This process, described in detail by Barker 1963; Barker 1964, has been discussed
extensively in the linguistics literature; see, for example, Kisseberth 1972; Kean
1973; White 1973; Thomas 1975; Feinstein and Vago 1981; Clements and Keyser
1983, inter alia.
17. Transcriptions of Klamath throughout this discussion follow Barker, except for
phonetically reduced symbols in brackets; uppercase letters represent voiceless
sonorants.
18. The “Intensive” has a variety of meanings. Barker (1964: 120) states that it can
signify repeated action, such as “twinkling, flickering, trembling,” or “emotional
and bodily conditions which persist: e.g., being nauseated, feeling bad, burning (in
taste), etc.”
Notes to pages 118–37 219
19. See Raimy 2000 for discussion of Southern Paiute, cited in McCarthy and
Prince (1995a) as exhibiting identity-induced underapplication. As Raimy and,
independently, Gurevich (2000) demonstrate, consideration of additional data from
Sapir 1930 shows the effect to be nonexistent.
20. “Reduce” stands for the constraint(s) driving reduction; see Urbanczyk 1996;
Struijke 2000b for a detailed Optimality Theory account of the specifics of vowel
reduction.
21. Hausa data are presented in Hausa orthography, with three exceptions: vowel length
is marked with a colon rather than a macron; following Newman 2000, tapped /r̃/
is marked with a tilde to distinguish it from retroflex /r/; following Hausa linguistic
tradition and Newman 2000, Low tone is marked with a grave accent (High is
unmarked).
22. See the detailed discussion of Lushootseed in Park 2000 as well.
23. On derived environment effects, see Mascaró 1976; 1982b; 1993 and, more recently,
Burzio 1997; Lubowicz 1999; Inkelas 2000; Anttila in press; Cho in press.
24. Fitzgerald (2002), a response to Yu 2000, focuses on theoretical objections to
cophonologies; for discussion of the issues Fitzgerald raises, see Chapter 3 and
Inkelas and Zoll 2003.
25. CVCV reduplication is attested in nouns, adjectives, and verbs; in the case of verbs,
it is intensifying (Corston-Oliver 2002:470, 483).
26. Prefixes also attract stress. Corston-Oliver states that roots of three or more
syllables are stressed on the first and second syllables, an observation which
suggest that “stress” in Roviana may either consist of, or be accompanied by,
tone.
27. The suffix -ana derives instrumental or locational nouns from verbs; it is always
accompanied by verb root reduplication (Corston-Oliver 2002:472). Otherwise, verb
root reduplication “indicates intensive affect” (p. 480). See Chapter 7 for further
discussion.
28. For discussion of compound reduction in another Tibeto-Burman language, see
Nagaraja 1979 on Khasi.
29. In pluractional CVG verbal reduplication (G = gemination of following conso-
nant), the reduplicant agrees in tone with the following syllable; however, in CVG-
reduplication deriving adjectives of sensory quality, the reduplicant is uniformly L
toned, e.g. zurfi ‘heat’ → zùz-zurfa ‘very deep’ (Newman 2000:511). Both patterns
are non-tone-integrating.
13. Klein (1997) proposes a BRCT analysis of final CV reduplication in which the
opaque antepenultimate stress pattern is attributed to BR identity; penultimate stress,
on Klein’s analysis, would make base and reduplicant metrically unequal. As dis-
cussed in Chapter 4, however, Klein’s analysis founders once the facts of stressed
CV reduplication are considered.
14. Lexical items are taken from sentences in Hurlbut 1988, who provides interlinear
glosses for morphemes but not for words; therefore word glosses are not attempted
here.
15. Hurlbut 1988 does not gloss the prefix to-; however, she mentions the existence
of a root ruol (p. 60) and cites, in the glossary, the forms o-ruol ‘be painful’ and
poko-ruol ‘curse, lit. cause pain’ (p. 133).
16. The counterexamples to this generalization all involve single-segment reduplication,
which, as argued in Chapter 7, falls under the category of phonological duplication,
rather than morphological reduplication.
17. See Kiparsky 2000:9 for critical discussion of Benua’s analysis of this example;
Kiparsky’s objections, however, do not affect the point made here.
18. There are two reported examples, to our knowledge, of overapplication of a BR-
juncture effect. One, in Chaha, is reanalyzed in §5.7 as phonological agreement,
not necessarily reduplication-specific. The other is a case from Malay, discussed in
Kenstowicz 1981; McCarthy and Prince 1995a. In Malay, nasality spreads rightward
from a nasal consonant to any following vowel or glide; it is blocked by nonglides.
In forms like hamã → hãmã-hãma, nasal harmony applies across the BR juncture
(hamã-hamã → hamã-hãmã) and is then apparently reflected to the first vowel
of the first copy (→ hãmã-hãmã), even though it is not transparently conditioned
there. This dramatic example of overapplication has attracted much attention in the
literature. To our knowledge Onn 1976 is the only source of this data, which has
not been replicated in any other publication.
19. For a parallel example, see the discussion of Warlpiri in note 3.
20. As Richard Rhodes has pointed out to us, the first form in (55c) exhibits double
reduplication; the initial ne of nenehkememewa is an instance of monosyllabic
reduplication, a less productive pattern than the disyllabic reduplication discussed
here. On double reduplication in Fox, see Dahlstrom 1997.
21. The /e∼i/ alternation in the roots in (59a) and the suffixes following the null roots in
(59b) is due to “Initial Change” discussed in §5.5.2.1. The /e/ vowel is underlying;
/i/ is derived.
22. An alternative view is offered by Burkhardt 2002, who proposes output-output
faithfulness as a means of handling the overapplication of /e/-raising.
23. Zuraw (2002) develops what is in many ways a similar theory, though gives a
base-reduplicant analysis to the corresponding strings. Her view is that learners
reanalyze words containing similar segments or sequences as reduplicative, which
then leads them to make the strings in question even more similar (hence the name
for her theory, namely “Aggressive Reduplication”). However, since the effect of
the reanalysis (increased similarity) is comparable to what is yielded by the purely
phonological correspondence analyses of Hansson 2001 and Rose and Walker 2001,
it seems unnecessary to posit a reduplicative morpheme as part of the process.
222 Notes to pages 182–94
6 Case studies
1. These data are taken from McCarthy and Prince 1995a. The rest of the data cited in
our discussion come from Bloomfield 1933; Schachter and Otanes 1972; English
1986; French 1988. Data are orthographic, except for ‘ng,’ which is converted to
[ŋ]. Stress and vowel length, not represented in orthography, are not represented
here either, despite being marked in some of the sources.
2. This form, whose gloss comes from Schachter and Otanes 1972:364, is the imper-
fective of maŋ-isda ‘go fishing.’ According to Schachter and Otanes, imperfectives
of verbs beginning with m-initial prefixes are formed by a two-part operation: redu-
plication and replacement of m with n.
3. Another alternative is offered by Saperstein (1997), who suggests a possible process
of reanalysis by which stems such as putul have developed nasal-initial allomorphs
(e.g. mutul), for which reduplication selects.
4. On the predictable d ∼ r alternation illustrated in (4e), see Schachter and Otanes
1972.
5. In some cases, e.g. magpa-kain → mag-pa-pakain, second syllable reduplication is
the only option.
6. Both Applegate and Wash use as their data source the field notes of J. P. Harrington;
Wash, in addition, draws from tape recordings made by Madison Beeler.
7. Applegate does not typically provide morpheme glosses. Applegate 1972 con-
tains extensive discussion of Chumash affixational morphology, and with work
one can usually identify the affixes in a given verb. However, there is enough
homophony among the affixes to make this tricky in some cases. We have supplied
morpheme glosses here and there, but have not yet attempted to do this for every
form.
8. The glosses in (8) are taken directly from Applegate 1976; some minor typographical
data errors in Mester 1986:204, carried over in McCarthy and Prince (1995a:308,
313), are also corrected here.
9. Wilbur (1973:17) says that geminate aspiration applies transparently in Chumash,
citing the form /k-kut-kuti/ → kh utkuti ‘to look.’ We have not been able to confirm
this form in Applegate’s work.
10. Note that this analysis requires the prefix-final consonant to switch morphemic
allegiance from prefix to reduplicant, a transformational process which could not
occur in MDT.
11. A CV or CVʔ stem will have a CVh reduplicant.
12. For only a minority of cases is supporting evidence given in the dissertation. How-
ever, there are examples to back up all of the crucial arguments made here.
13. The prefix in (18a) does reduplicate in Barbareño; see (17d). In (18f) we correct an
apparent typographical error by Applegate, whose surface form omits the vowel in
the first syllable.
14. As elsewhere, Applegate does not gloss these individual morphemes. There is a
prefix qili-/qulu- ‘of seeing, vision’ (p. 362) that could be the source of xul-. The
consonants /q/ and /x/ alternate freely in Chumash, and the final /i/ of qili-/qulu-
could be epenthetic (see p. 327), rather than underlying.
Notes to pages 194–201 223
15. In an affix-ordering chart, Applegate (1972:380) indicates that ni- precedes su-, a
claim consistent with such examples as
/ni-su-wal-tun/ [nisuwatun] ‘to put something over something else’ A72:332
/ni-su-tap/ ‘to put into; to stuff (e.g. a doll)’ A72:332
The prefixes ni- and su- thus occur in both orders. Several explanations are possible –
free variation, different orderings with different meanings, two different su- prefixes,
or two different ni- prefixes – but it is impossible to decide among these on the basis
of the available data. A similar discrepancy involves su- and wati-, occurring below
in the opposite order from what is exhibited in (22):
/wati-su-axsil-š/ [watišaxšilš] ‘to go fishing every now and then’ A72:383
16. The surface form of ‘my arm is broken’ is constructed from Applegate’s underlying
representation.
7 Final issues
1. On Hausa, see Newman 2000. Another recently discovered example of onset-driven
phonological copying occurs in American English “Homeric” infixation examples,
discussed by Yu 2003.
2. Proximal assimilation includes the possibility of onset-to-onset assimilation (pos-
sibly across an intervening coda) or coda-to-coda assimilation (possibly across an
intervening onset); Temiar, recently the subject of several insightful studies by Gafos
(1998a; 1998b), appears to be a case of the latter type.
3. Two other such cases occur in Kinande (Mutaka & Hyman 1990) and Nancowry
(Radhakrishnan 1981). The Kinande facts are amply discussed by Mutaka and
Hyman, and we will not repeat them here. Nancowry has been described by
Hendricks 1999 as a system in which reduplication of monosyllabic roots occurs
“to create a stem that is usable for other processes, such as inflection” (p. 251),
though in fact only one affix (the causative, which is derivational) selects for
reduplicated roots. There is no general requirement that the base of affixa-
tion must be disyllabic. In his grammar of Nancowry, Radhakrishnan 1981 dis-
cusses five different affixes, three of which have two suppletive allomorphs,
making a total of eight lexically listed affixes. Five of these eight affixes are
able to combine directly with monosyllabic roots; in fact the possessive -u
(e.g. kan-u ‘possessing [married to] a woman’; p. 65), the objective -a (e.g. wiʔ-a
‘a thing made’; p. 66), the causative ha- (e.g. ha-káh ‘to cause to know’; p. 54),
and the instrumental -an- (e.g. s-an-ák ‘spear’; p. 61) combine only with mono-
syllabic roots. Two of the affixes, causative -um (e.g. p-um-ʔ´y ‘to cause to have
bad smell,’ cf. paʔ´y ‘smell’; p. 54) and instrumental -in- (e.g. t-in-kuác ‘tracer,’
cf. takuác ‘to have a trace’; p. 62), combine only with lexically disyllabic roots;
reduplication of a monosyllabic root is not an option for these. But it is for
one prefix, the /ma-/ allomorph of the agentive. This prefix combines only with
reduplicated monosyllabic roots (p. 58), thus /ʔáp/ ‘to be closed’ → m-up-ʔáp
224 Notes to pages 210–11
‘one that is closed,’ where up-ʔáp is the reduplicated root. (On the phonology of
Nancowry reduplication, see Steriade 1988; Alderete et al. 1999; Hendricks 1999.)
4. Following standard practice in the literature on Turkish, we use uppercase letters (e.g.
“I”) for vowels whose specifications for [back] and [round] are predictable from the
context.
5. For example, Alderete (1999; 2001) invokes anti-identity constraints in analyzing
accentual dominance effects; anti-identity is used only to accomplish tone deletion,
not actually to toggle the values for tones or to insert tones into toneless bases.
Simple tone deletion (as proposed for the same data in Inkelas 1998) might be a more
straightforward analysis. Crosswhite (1997; 1999) invokes anti-identity constraints
to prohibit homophony within inflectional paradigms in Bulgarian and Russian. For
example, the suffix vowel in the Russian verb /stávj -at/ ‘place-3pl’ reduces to /ə/
(stáv j ət), rather than the expected /i/, ostensibly in order to avoid homophony with
the third singular form stáv j it. As Alan Timberlake (personal communication) points
out, however, other suffix /a/ vowels also reduce to /ə/ in the same phonological
context even when homophony is not at issue; the target of reduction appears to
be morphologically conditioned, making anti-homophony unnecessary as a causal
factor.
References
Abbi, Anvita. 1991. Reduplication in South Asian languages: an areal, typological and
historical study. New Delhi: Allied Publishers Limited.
Akinlabi, Akinbiyi. 1985. Tonal underspecification and Yoruba tone. PhD thesis,
University of Ibadan.
Akinlabi, Akinbiyi. 1993. Underspecification and the phonology of Yoruba /r/. Linguistic
Inquiry 24:139–60.
Alderete, John. 1999. Morphologically governed accent in Optimality Theory. PhD
thesis, University of Massachussets, Amherst.
Alderete, John. 2001. Dominance effects as transderivational anti-faithfulness. Phonol-
ogy 18:201–53.
Alderete, John, Jill Beckman, Laura Benua, Amalia Gnanadesikan, John McCarthy,
and Suzanne Urbanczyk. 1999. Reduplication with fixed segmentism. Linguistic
Inquiry 30:327–64.
Anderson, Stephen R. 1974. The organization of phonology. San Diego: Academic Press.
Anderson, Stephen R. 1992. A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
Anttila, Arto. 1997. Deriving variation from grammar. In Frans Hinskens, Roeland
Van Hout, and W. Leo Wetzels (eds.), Variation, change and phonological theory.
Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 35–68.
Anttila, Arto. 2002. Morphologically conditioned phonological alternations. Natural
Language and Linguistic Theory 20:1–42.
Anttila, Arto. In press. Derived environment effects in Colloquial Helsinki Finnish. In
Kristin Hanson and Sharon Inkelas (eds.), The nature of the word: essays in honor
of Paul Kiparsky. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Anttila, Arto and Young-mee Yu Cho 1998. Variation and change in Optimality Theory.
Lingua 104:31–36.
Applegate, Richard. 1972. Ineseño Chumash grammar. PhD thesis, University of
California, Berkeley.
Applegate, Richard. 1976. Reduplication in Chumash. In Margaret Langdon and Shirley
Silver (eds.), Hokan Studies. The Hague: Mouton, 271–83.
Apte, Mahadeo. 1968. Reduplication, echo formation and onomatopoeia in Marathi.
Poona: Deccan College Post-Graduate and Research Institute.
Arnott, David W. 1970. The nominal and verbal systems of Fula. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
225
226 References
Aronoff, Mark. 1988. Head operations and strata in reduplication: a linear treatment.
Yearbook of Morphology 1:1–15.
Aronoff, Mark. 1992. Noun classes in Arapesh. Yearbook of Morphology 1991:21–32.
Aronoff, Mark. 1994. Morphology by itself: stems and inflectional classes. Cambridge:
MIT Press.
Austin, Peter. 1981. A grammar of Diyari. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bagemihl, Bruce. 1988. Alternate phonologies and morphologies. PhD thesis, University
of British Columbia.
Barker, Muhammad Abd-al-Rahman. 1963. Klamath Dictionary, University of Califor-
nia Publications in Linguistics, vol. 31. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of
California Press.
Barker, Muhammad Abd-al-Rahman. 1964. Klamath grammar. University of Califor-
nia Publications in Linguistics, vol. 32. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of
California Press.
Barnes, Jonathan. 2002. Positional neutralization: a phonologization approach to typo-
logical patterns. PhD thesis, University of California, Berkeley.
Bat-El, Outi. 1994. Stem modification and cluster transfer in Modern Hebrew. Natural
Language and Linguistic Theory 12:571–96.
Bates, Dawn. 1986. An analysis of Lushootseed diminutive reduplication. In N. Niki-
foridou, M. VanClay, M. Niepokuj, and D. Feder (eds.), Proceedings of the Berkeley
Linguistics Society 12. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society, 1–13.
Bates, Dawn and Barry F. Carlson. 1998. Spokane (Npoqi’nis&cn) syllable structure
and reduplication. In Ewa Czaykowska-Higgins and Dale Kinkade (eds.), Sal-
ish languages and linguistics: theoretical and descriptive perspectives. Trends in
Linguistics: Studies and Monographs 107. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, 99–123.
Bates, Dawn, Thom Hess, and Vi Hilbert. 1994. Lushootseed dictionary. Seattle:
University of Washington Press.
Benua, Laura. 1997. Transderivational identity: phonological relations between words.
PhD thesis, University of Massachusetts.
Bhaskararao, Peri. 1977. Reduplication and onomatopoeia in Telugu. Poona: Deccan
College.
Bissell, Teal. 2002. “Avoidance of the marked” in Miya pluractional allomorphy. In
Aniko Csirmaz, Zhiqiang Li, Andrew Nevins, Olga Vaysman, and Michael Wagner
(eds.), Phonological answers. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL, 1–42.
Black, Deirdre. 1996. The morphological and phonological structure of Spokane
lexemes. PhD thesis, University of Montana.
Blevins, James. 2003. Stems and paradigms. Language 79:737–67.
Blevins, Juliette. 1996. Mokilese reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry 27:523–30.
Blevins, Juliette. 1999. Untangling Leti infixation. Oceanic Linguistics 38:383–403.
Blevins, Juliette. 2004. Evolutionary phonology: the emergence of sound patterns.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. Language. New York: Henry Holt.
Blust, Robert. 2001. Thao Triplication. Oceanic Linguistics 40:324–36.
Boas, Franz and Ella Deloria. 1941. Dakota Grammar, vol. 23, part 2: Memoirs of the
National Academy of Sciences.
References 227
Crowley, Terry. 2002c. Southeast Ambrym. In John Lynch, Malcolm Ross, and Terry
Crowley (eds.), The Oceanic languages. Richmond: Curzon Press, 660–70.
Crowley, Terry. 2002d. Sye. In John Lynch, Malcolm Ross, and Terry Crowley (eds.),
The Oceanic languages. Richmond: Curzon Press, 694–722.
Dahlstrom, Amy. 1997. Fox (Mesquakie) reduplication. International Journal of Amer-
ican Linguistics 63:205–26.
DeLacy, Paul. 2002. The formal expression of markedness. PhD thesis, University of
Massachusetts, Amherst.
Delancey, Scott. 1991. Chronological strata of suffix classes in the Klamath verb. Inter-
national Journal of American Linguistics 57:426–45.
Delancey, Scott. 1999. Lexical prefixes and the bipartite stem construction in Klamath.
International Journal of American Linguistics 65:56–83.
Dixon, Robert. 1972. The Dyirbal language of North Queensland. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Dixon, Robert. 1988. A grammar of Boumaa Fijian. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Dobrin, Lise M. 1998. The morphosyntactic reality of phonological form. Yearbook of
Morphology 1997:59–81.
Dobrin, Lise M. 1999. Phonological form, morphological class, and syntactic gender:
the noun class systems of Papua New Guinea Arapeshan. PhD thesis, University
of Chicago.
Dolbey, Andrew. 1996. Output optimization and cyclic allomorph selection. In Brian
Agbayani and Sze-Wing Tang (eds.), Proceedings of the fifteenth West Coast
Conference on Formal Linguistics. Stanford: CSLI Publications, 97–112.
Downing, Laura. 1997. Correspondence effects in Siswati reduplication. Studies in the
Linguistic Sciences 25:81–95.
Downing, Laura. 1998a. Morphological correspondence constraints on Kikerewe redu-
plication. In Emily Curtis, James Lyle, and Gabriel Webster (eds.), Proceedings
of the sixteenth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Stanford: CSLI
Publications, 161–75.
Downing, Laura. 1998b. Onset motivated overcopy in reduplication. In E. van Gelderen
and V. Samiian (eds.), Proceedings of WECOL. Fresno, CA: California State
University Press, 81–96.
Downing, Laura. 1998c. On the prosodic misalignment of onsetless syllables. Natural
Language and Linguistic Theory 16:1–52.
Downing, Laura. 1998d. Prosodic misalignment and reduplication. Yearbook of
Morphology 1997:83–120.
Downing, Laura. 1998e. Prosodic stem = prosodic word in Bantu. In Tracy Hall and
Ursula Kleinhenz (eds.), Studies on the phonological word. Current Studies in
Linguistic Theory. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, 73–98.
Downing, Laura. 1999a. Morphological constraints on Bantu reduplication. Linguistic
Analysis 29:6–46.
Downing, Laura. 1999b. Verbal reduplication in three Bantu languages. In Harry van der
Hulst, René Kager, and Wim Zonneveld (eds.), The prosody-morphology interface.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 62–89.
230 References
Inkelas, Sharon and Cemil Orhan Orgun. 1995. Level ordering and economy in the
lexical phonology of Turkish. Language 71:763–93.
Inkelas, Sharon and Cemil Orhan Orgun. 1998. Level (non)ordering in recursive mor-
phology: evidence from Turkish. In Steven Lapointe, Diane Brentari, and Patrick
Farrell (eds.), Morphology and its relation to phonology and syntax. Stanford, CA:
CSLI Publications, 360–92.
Inkelas, Sharon, Cemil Orhan Orgun, and Cheryl Zoll. 1997. Implications of lexical
exceptions for the nature of grammar. In Iggy Roca (ed.), Constraints and deriva-
tions in phonology. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 393–418.
Inkelas, Sharon and Cheryl Zoll. 2003. Is grammar dependence real? Unpublished ms.,
UC Berkeley and MIT. Available online from the Rutgers Optimality Archive,
ROA# 587-0303.
Innes, G. 1971. A practical introduction to Mende. London: School of Oriental and
African Studies.
Itô, Junko. 1990. Prosodic minimality in Japanese. In Michael Ziolkowski, Manuela
Noske, and Karen Deaton (eds.), Papers from the twenty-sixth regional meeting of
the Chicago Linguistics Society, vol. II: The parasession on the syllable in phonetics
and phonology. Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society, 213–39.
Itô, Junko and Armin Mester. 1986. The phonology of voicing in Japanese: theoretical
consequences for morphological accessibility. Linguistic Inquiry 17:49–73.
Itô, Junko and Armin Mester. 1995a. The core-periphery structure of the lexicon
and constraints on reranking. University of Massachussetts Occasional Papers in
Linguistics 18. Amherst, MA: GLSA.
Itô, Junko and Armin Mester. 1995b. Japanese phonology: constraint domains and
structure preservation. In John Goldsmith (ed.), The handbook of phonological
theory. Blackwell Handbooks in Linguistics Series Cambridge, MA: Blackwell,
817–38.
Itô, Junko and Armin Mester. 1996a. Rendaku 1: constraint conjunction and the OCP.
Available online from the Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA# 144-0996.
Itô, Junko and Armin Mester. 1996b. Structural economy and OCP interactions in local
domains. Paper presented at the Western Conference on Linguistics (WECOL).
University of California, Santa Cruz.
Itô, Junko and Armin Mester. 1997. Correspondence and compositionality: the Ga-gyo
variation in Japanese phonology. In Iggy Roca (ed.), Derivations and constraints
in phonology. Oxford: Clarendon Press, 419–62.
Itô, Junko and Armin Mester. 1999. The phonological lexicon. In N. Tsujimura (ed.),
The handbook of Japanese linguistics. Malden, MA: Blackwell, 62–100.
Itô, Junko, Armin Mester, and Jaye Padgett. 1995. Licensing and underspecification in
Optimality Theory. Linguistic Inquiry 26:571–613.
Jeanne, LaVerne Masayesua. 1982. Some phonological rules of Hopi. IJAL 48:
245–70.
Johanson, Lars and Eva Csato, eds. 1998. The Turkic languages. New York: Routledge.
Jones, William. 1919. Ojibwa Texts, Part 2. New York: G. E. Stechert.
Kager, Rene. 1996. On affix allomorphy and syllable counting. In Ursula Kleinhenz
(ed.), Interfaces in phonology. Studia grammatica 41. Berlin: Akademie Verlag,
155–71.
234 References
Kiparsky, Paul. 1982c. Word-formation and the lexicon. In Frances Ingemann (ed.), 1982
Mid-America linguistics conference papers. Lawrence, KS: University of Kansas,
3–29.
Kiparsky, Paul. 1986. The phonology of reduplication. Unpublished ms., Stanford
University.
Kiparsky, Paul. 1993. Blocking in non-derived environments. In Sharon Hargus and
Ellen Kaisse (eds.), Phonetics and phonology 4: studies in lexical phonology. San
Diego: Academic Press, 277–313.
Kiparsky, Paul. 1994. Remarks on markedness. Paper presented at TREND, University
of California, Santa Cruz.
Kiparsky, Paul. 1997. Templatic morphology: Base-reduplication Identity constraints vs.
cyclicity. Class handout from Scandinavian Summer School in Generative Phonol-
ogy, Hvalfjardarströnd, Iceland, June 1997.
Kiparsky, Paul. 2000. Opacity and cyclicity. The Linguistic Review 17:351–67.
Kisseberth, Charles W. 1972, Cyclical rules in Klamath Phonology. Linguistic Inquiry
3:3–33.
Kiyomi, Setsuko. 1993. A typological study of reduplication as a morpho-semantic
process: evidence from five language families (Bantu, Australian, Papuan,
Austroasiatic and Malayo-Polynesian). PhD thesis, Indiana University.
Klein, Thomas B. 1997. Output constraints and prosodic correspondence in Chamorro
reduplication. Linguistic Inquiry 28:707–16.
Koenig, Jean-Pierre. 1992. Shared structure vs. constructional autonomy in construction
grammar. Proceedings of the fifteenth International Congress of Linguistics, 356–
59.
Koenig, Jean-Pierre and Daniel Jurafsky. 1995. Type underspecification and on-line type
construction in the lexicon. In Raul Aranovich, William Byrne, Susanne Preuss,
and Martha Senturia (eds.), Proceedings of the thirteenth West Coast Conference
on Formal Linguistics. Stanford: CSLI Publications, 270–85.
Kornfilt, Jaklin. 1997. Turkish. London: Routledge.
Krause, Scott. 1980. Topics in Chukchee Phonology and Morphology. PhD thesis,
University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign.
Kroeger, Paul. 1990. Discontinuous reduplication in vernacular Malay. Proceedings of
the Berkeley Linguistics Society 15. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society, 193–
202.
Kurisu, Kazutaka. 2001. The phonology of morpheme realization. PhD thesis, UC Santa
Cruz.
Landau, Idan. 2003. Modular recoverability: chain resolution in Hebrew VP-fronting.
Unpublished ms., Ben Gurion University of the Negev and Tel Aviv University.
Lathroum, Amanda. 1991. Resolving the disparity between linear and structural
order in words: a study of reduplication in Austronesian. PhD thesis, Harvard
University.
Lefebvre, Claire and A. M. Brousseau. 2002. A grammar of Fongbe. Berlin: Mouton de
Gruyter.
Levin, Juliette. 1985a. A metrical theory of syllabicity. PhD thesis, Massachusetts
Institute of Technology.
236 References
Mester, Armin and Junko Itô. 1995. The core-periphery structure of the lexicon and con-
straints on reranking: University of Massachussets Occasional Papers in Linguistics
18. Amherst, MA: GLSA.
Mintz, Malcolm W. 1994. A student’s grammar of Malay and Indonesian. Singapore:
EPB Publishers Pte Ltd.
Mithun, Marianne. 1984. The evolution of noun incorporation. Language 60:847–94.
Mithun, Marianne. 2000. Incorporation. In Geert Booij, Christian Lehmann, and Joachim
Mugdan (eds.), Morphologie – morphology. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 916–28.
Mohanan, K. P. 1986. Lexical phonology. Dordrecht: Kluwer.
Mohanan, K. P. 1995. The organization of the grammar. In John Goldsmith (ed.), The
handbook of phonological theory. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell, 24–69.
Moravcsik, Edith. 1978. Reduplicative constructions. In Joseph Greenberg (ed.),
Universals of human language. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 297–334.
Mtenje, Al. 1988. On tone and transfer in Chichewa reduplication. Linguistics 26:125–
55.
Mutaka, Ngessimo and Larry M. Hyman. 1990. Syllable and morpheme integrity in
Kinande reduplication. Phonology 7:73–120.
Nagaraja, K. S. 1979. Contraction of Khasi nouns in compounds. Indian Linguistics
40:18–23.
Nash, David. 1986. Topics in Warlpiri grammar. New York: Garland.
Nater, Henry. 1984. The Bella Coola language: Canadian Ethnology Service Paper 92.
Ottawa: Museum of Man.
Nater, Henry. 1990. A concise Nuxalk–English dictionary: Canadian Ethnology Paper
115. Ottawa: Museum of Civilization.
Nespor, Marina and Irene Vogel. 1986. Prosodic phonology. Dordrecht: Foris.
Newman, Paul. 1986. Tone and affixation in Hausa. Studies in African Linguistics
17:249–67.
Newman, Paul. 1989a. The historical change from suffixal to prefixal reduplication in
Hausa pluractional verbs. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 11:37–44.
Newman, Paul. 1989b. Reduplication and tone in Hausa ideophones. In Kira Hall,
Michael Meacham, and Richard Shapiro (eds.), Proceedings of the fifteenth annual
meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society,
248–55.
Newman, Paul. 2000. The Hausa language: an encyclopedic reference grammar. New
Haven: Yale University Press.
Newman, Stanley. 1971. Bella Coola reduplication. International Journal of American
Linguistics 37:34–38.
Nguyen, Dı̀nh-hoà. 1997. Vietnamese: Tieng Viet Khong Son Phan. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins.
Niepokuj, Mary Katherine. 1997. The development of verbal reduplication in Indo-
European. Washington: Institute for the Study of Man.
Noonan, Michael. 1992. A grammar of Lango. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
Nunberg, Geoffrey, Ivan A. Sag, and Thomas Wasow. 1994. Idioms. Language 70:491–
538.
Onn, Farid M. 1976. Aspects of Malay phonology and morphology: a generative
approach. PhD thesis, University of Illinois.
References 239
Orgun, Cemil Orhan. 1994. Monotonic cyclicity and Optimality Theory. In Mercè
Gonzàlez (ed.), Proceedings of the Northeastern Linguistic Society 24. Amherst,
MA: GLSA, 461–74.
Orgun, Cemil Orhan. 1996. Sign-based morphology and phonology: with special atten-
tion to Optimality Theory. PhD thesis, University of California, Berkeley.
Orgun, Cemil Orhan. 1997. Cyclic and noncyclic effects in a declarative grammar.
Yearbook of morphology 1997:179–218.
Orgun, Cemil Orhan. 1999. Sign-based morphology: a declarative theory of phonology-
morphology interleaving. In Ben Hermans and Marc van Oostendorp (eds.),
The derivational residue in phonological Optimality Theory. Amsterdam: John
Benjamins, 247–67.
Orgun, Cemil Orhan. 2002. Phonology-morphology interaction in a constraint-based
framework. Unpublished ms., University of California, Davis.
Orgun, Cemil Orhan and Sharon Inkelas. 2002. Reconsidering bracket erasure. Yearbook
of Morphology 2001:115–46.
Ourn, Noeurng and John Haiman. 2000. Symmetrical compounds in Khmer. Studies in
Language 24:483–514.
Park, Miae. 2000. Surface opacity and phonological issues in Klamath and Lushootseed.
PhD thesis, University of Wisconsin-Madison.
Parker, G. J. 1968. Southeast Ambrym verb inflection and morphophonemics. Papers
in Linguistics of Melanesia. Pacific Linguistics Series A-15:27–40.
Patterson, Trudi Alice. 1990. Theoretical aspects of Dakota morphology and phonology.
PhD thesis, University of Illinois.
Payne, David. 1981. The phonology and morphology of Axininca Campa. Arlington,
TX: Summer Institute of Linguistics.
Pesetsky, David. 1979. Russian morphology and lexical theory. Unpublished ms.,
Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Peterson, David. 2001. The Ingush clitic ʔa: the elusive type 5 clitic? Language 77:144–
55.
Piñon, Chris. 1991. Falling in paradise: verbs, preverbs and reduplication in Hungarian.
Handout from talk delivered at Syntax Workshop, 5/21/91, Stanford University.
Poser, William J. 1984. The phonetics and phonology of tone and intonation in Japanese.
PhD thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Poser, William J. 1989. The metrical foot in Diyari. Phonology 6:117–48.
Prince, Alan and Paul Smolensky. 1993. Optimality theory: constraint interaction in
generative grammar. Cognitive Science Center Technical report TR-2, Rutgers
University.
Pulleyblank, Douglas. 1986. Tone in lexical phonology. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
Pulleyblank, Douglas. 1988. Vocalic underspecification in Yoruba. Linguistic Inquiry
19:233–70.
Pulleyblank, Douglas. 1998. Yoruba vowel patterns: deriving asymmetries by the ten-
sion between opposing constraints. Available online from the Rutgers Optimality
Archive, ROA# 270-0798.
Pulleyblank, Douglas. To appear. Patterns of reduplication in Yoruba. In Kristin Hanson
and Sharon Inkelas (eds.), The nature of the word: essays in honor of Paul Kiparsky.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
240 References
Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1982. The syntax of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Sezer, Engin. 1981. The k/Ø alternation in Turkish. In George N. Clements (ed.),
Harvard Studies in phonology. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club,
354–82.
Shaw, Patricia A. 1980. Theoretical issues in Dakota phonology and morphology: Out-
standing dissertations in linguistics. New York: Garland.
Shaw, Patricia A. 1985. Modularisation and substantive constraints in Dakota Lexical
Phonology. Phonology 2:173–202.
Sherrard, Nicholas. 2001. Blending and reduplication. PhD thesis, University of Essex.
Sibanda, Galen. 2004. Issues in the phonology and morphology of Ndebele. PhD thesis,
University of California, Berkeley.
Sibomana, Leo. 1980. Grundzüge der Phonologie des Tarok (Yergam). Afrika und
Übsersee 63:199–206.
Sibomana, Leo. 1981. Tarok II: Das Nominalklassensystem. Afrika und Übersee 64:25–
34.
Sietsema, Brian M. 1988. Reduplications in Dakota. In Papers from the regional meeting,
Chicago Linguistic Society. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society, 337–52.
Singh, Rajendra. 1982. On some “redundant compounds” in Modern Hindi. Lingua
56:345–51.
Smith, Jennifer. 1997. Noun faithfulness: on the privileged status of nouns in phonology.
Available online from the Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA# 242-1098.
Smith, Jennifer. 1998. Noun faithfulness and word stress in Tuyuca. In Jennifer Austin
and Aaron Lawson (eds.), Proceedings of ESCOL 97. Ithaca: CLC Publications,
180–91.
Smolensky, Paul. 1993. Harmony, markedness and phonological activity. Rutgers
University: Rutgers Optimality Workshop 1.
Soltész, Katalin J. 1959. As ösi magyar igekötök [The ancient Hungarian preverbs].
Budapest: Akadémia Kiadó.
Sommer, Bruce. 1981, The shape of Kenjen syllables. In Didier L. Goyvaerts (ed.),
Phonology in the 1980s. Ghent: E. Story-Scientia, 231–44.
Spaelti, Philip. 1997. Dimensions of variation in multi-pattern reduplication. PhD thesis,
University of California, Santa Cruz.
Spencer, Andrew. 1998. Morphophonological operations. In Andrew Spencer and
Arnold Zwicky (eds.), Handbook of morphology. Oxford: Blackwell, 123–
43.
Spring, Cari. 1990. Implications of Axininca Campa for Prosodic Morphology and
reduplication. PhD thesis, University of Arizona.
Sproat, Richard. 1986. Malayalam compounding: a non-stratum ordered account. In
Mary Dalrymple, Jeffrey Goldberg, Kristin Hanson et al. (eds.), Proceedings of the
fifth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Stanford: Stanford Linguistics
Association, 268–88.
Sprouse, Ronald. 1997. A case for enriched inputs. Available online from the Rutgers
Optimality Archive, ROA# 193–0597.
Sprouse, Ronald. In preparation. Allomorphy in the lexicon: blocking, opacity, and
ungrammaticality. PhD thesis, University of California, Berkeley.
Srichampa, Sophana. 2002. Vietnamese verbal reduplication. In Robert S. Bauer (ed.),
Collected papers on Southeast Asian and Pacific languages. Canberra: Pacific
242 References
Yip, Moira. 1997. Repetition and its avoidance: the case of Javanese. In Keiichiro Suzuki
and D. Elzinga (eds.), Arizona phonology conference Proceedings, vol. V: Pro-
ceedings of the South Western Workshop on Optimality Theory (SWOT). Tucson:
University of Arizona, Department of Linguistics coyote Papers, 238–62. Available
electronically from the Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA# 82-00002.
Yip, Moira. 1996. Lexicon optimization in languages without alternations. In Jacques
Durand and Bernard Laks (eds.), Current trends in phonology: models and methods.
Salford: University of Salford Publications, 757–88.
Yip, Moira. 1998. Identity avoidance in phonology and morphology. In Steven LaPointe,
Diane Brentari, and Patrick Farrell (eds.), Morphology and its relations to phonol-
ogy and syntax. Stanford: CSLI Publications, 216–46.
Yip, Moira. 1999. Reduplication as alliteration and rhyme. GLOT International 4:1–7.
Yoquelet, Corey. 2004. Grammaticalization and the epiphenomenon of the so-called
repeated morpheme constraint. Unpublished ms., University of California, Berke-
ley.
Yu, Alan C.-L. 2000. Stress assignment in Tohono O’odham. Phonology 17:117–35.
Yu, Alan C.-L. 2003. The phonology and morphology of infixation. PhD thesis, Univer-
sity of California, Berkeley.
Zepeda, Ofelia. 1983. A Papago grammar. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.
Zepeda, Ofelia. 1984. Topics in Papago morphology. PhD thesis, University of Arizona.
Zigmond, Maurice L., Curtis G. Booth, and Pamela Munro. 1990. Kawaiisu: a grammar
and Dictionary with Texts. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Zimmer, Karl and Barbara Abbott. 1978. The k/Ø alternation in Turkish; some experi-
mental evidence for its productivity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 7:35–46.
Zoll, Cheryl. 2002. Vowel reduction and reduplication in Klamath. Linguistic Inquiry
33:520–27.
Zuraw, Kie Ross. 2000. Exceptions and regularities in phonology. PhD thesis, University
of California, Los Angeles.
Zuraw, Kie Ross. 2002. Aggressive reduplication. Phonology 19:395–439.
Index of languages
245
246 Index of languages
247
248 Index of names
Early, R. 15 Inkelas, S. 6, 10, 11, 12, 16, 31, 33, 34, 38, 39,
Egbokhare, F. 14 40–41, 43, 48, 65, 67, 70, 71, 72, 100, 105,
Elbert, S. 203 118, 132–33, 140, 156, 215, 218, 224
Emeneau, M. 42, 79 Innes, G. 51
English, L. 183 Isdardi, W. 118, 127
Ezard, B. 67, 94–95, 164 Isoroembo, A. 93
Itô, J. 34, 67, 70, 74, 91
Feinstein, M. 218
Fillmore, C. 16 Jeanne, L. 105
Fitzgerald, C. 119, 129, 130, 219 Johanson, L. 42, 80
Ford, A. 67 Jones, W. 124
Fortune, R. 206–07 Jurafsky, D. 45
Frampton, J. 159, 213
French, K. 182 Kager, R. 4, 34, 89, 112
Kaisse, E. 70
Gafos, D. 21, 213, 223 Kanerva, J. 33
Geraghty, P. 15 Kawu, A. 2–3
Gerdts, D. 61 Kay, P. 16
Goldberg, A. 16 Kean, M. 218
Good, J. 8–9 Kelepir, M. 79
Goodenough, W. 203 Kenstowicz, M. 65, 72, 74, 104, 105, 108,
Gorgoniev, Y. 62 109, 112, 173, 177–78
Green, A. 131 Keyser, S. 218
Gurevich, N. 219 Kim, E. 217
Kiparsky, P. 4, 17, 70, 71, 105, 124, 135, 136,
Haiman, J. 8, 43–44, 47, 59, 60, 61, 62–63, 150, 156, 165, 201, 204, 205, 209, 213, 215,
82, 205 221
Halle, M. 218 Kisseberth, C. 105, 218
Hamilton, P. 89 Kiyomi, S. 14
Hanson, K. 205 Klein, T. 107, 108, 221
Hansson, G. 21, 177, 178, 179, Koenig, J.-P. 16, 45
198 Kornfilt, J. 33
Haraguchi, S. 75 Krause, S. 22, 156, 201
Hardman, M. 37 Kroeger, P. 96
Hargus, S. 71, 78 Kurisu, K. 80, 211
Harris, B. 22, 156
Harrison, S. 96 Landau, I. 3, 5, 6
Hayes, B. 71 Lathroum, A. 182
Healey, A. 93 Lefebvre, C. 5
Hendricks, S. 21, 213, 214, 223 Levin, J. (= Blevins, Juliette) 22, 96, 174, 203
Hess, T. 98, 119 Lewis, G. 33, 42
Hilbert, V. 98, 119 Li, C. 60
Hill, J. 129 Lieber, R. 12, 13, 17, 140, 182, 184
Hockett, C. 32 Lombardi, L. 155
Holtman, A. 204 Longtau, S. 84
Horne, E. 137, 138, 139, 144–45 Lubowicz, A. 219
Horoi, R. 14 Lynch, J. 14, 15, 49
Hualde, J. 71
Hurlbut, H. 152, 154–55, 176 MacWhinney, B. 210–11
Hyman, L. 5, 6, 10, 11, 31, 33, 38, 39, 40–41, Malkiel, Y. 215
65, 143, 223 Marantz, A. 4, 27, 68, 100
Index of names 249
251
252 Index of subjects
iconicity in reduplication: see semantic Native Identity theories 18, 68, 77–78, 81,
functions of reduplication 104, 110
identity: see phonological identity, semantic ‘near-synonym constructions’ (see also
identity synonym constructions) 47, 61–65
Identity Principle (Wilbur) 68, 135, 158–59 nonderived environment blocking: see derived
ideophones 1 environment effects
idiomatic meaning 12, 13, 15
Independent Daughter Prediction 69, 82 opacity effects in phonology (see also opacity
independent inputs in reduplication 26 effects in reduplication) 82, 135,
infixation 93–94, 155–56, 193 156–58
as source of phonological opacity in opacity effects in reduplication (see also
reduplication 153–54, 157–58, 183–85, cyclicity/layering, Base-Reduplicant
187–89, 193–95 Correspondence Theory) 20, 24, 135,
internal reduplication (see also 136–38, 141–42, 144–46, 147–48, 151,
infixation) 156, 182 152, 168, 172, 181–82, 186–87
item-based morphology 6, 12, 18, 46, 73, overapplication 24, 123, 135, 146, 150,
91 159–60, 161, 162, 164, 182, 186
underapplication 24, 99, 104, 105–06, 135,
‘Kager–Hamilton problem’ 89, 214 137, 148, 150, 159–60, 161, 162
opposite-edge reduplication 22, 156, 199
layering: see also cyclicity 102 Optimality Theory 6, 20, 21, 79, 92, 99
level ordering theory 17, 71, 105, 189–90 allomorphy in 34, 58
Lexical Conservatism 6 epenthesis in 124, 125
Lexical Morphology and Phonology (see also Lexicon Optimization 34
level ordering theory) 70–71 markedness in 124
Index of subjects 253