0% found this document useful (0 votes)
3 views34 pages

Problem Solving - Wikipedia

Problem solving is the process of overcoming obstacles to achieve a goal, ranging from simple personal tasks to complex business issues. It involves various techniques and methods across disciplines, including psychology, computer science, and engineering, and can be classified into well-defined and ill-defined problems. Common barriers to effective problem solving include confirmation bias, mental set, and functional fixedness, which can impede the search for solutions.

Uploaded by

magembebaroli95
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
3 views34 pages

Problem Solving - Wikipedia

Problem solving is the process of overcoming obstacles to achieve a goal, ranging from simple personal tasks to complex business issues. It involves various techniques and methods across disciplines, including psychology, computer science, and engineering, and can be classified into well-defined and ill-defined problems. Common barriers to effective problem solving include confirmation bias, mental set, and functional fixedness, which can impede the search for solutions.

Uploaded by

magembebaroli95
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 34

Problem solving

Problem solving is the process of achieving a goal by overcoming obstacles, a frequent part of
most activities. Problems in need of solutions range from simple personal tasks (e.g. how to turn on
an appliance) to complex issues in business and technical fields. The former is an example of
simple problem solving (SPS) addressing one issue, whereas the latter is complex problem solving
(CPS) with multiple interrelated obstacles.[1] Another classification of problem-solving tasks is into
well-defined problems with specific obstacles and goals, and ill-defined problems in which the
current situation is troublesome but it is not clear what kind of resolution to aim for.[2] Similarly, one
may distinguish formal or fact-based problems requiring psychometric intelligence, versus socio-
emotional problems which depend on the changeable emotions of individuals or groups, such as
tactful behavior, fashion, or gift choices.[3]

Solutions require sufficient resources and knowledge to attain the goal. Professionals such as
lawyers, doctors, programmers, and consultants are largely problem solvers for issues that require
technical skills and knowledge beyond general competence. Many businesses have found profitable
markets by recognizing a problem and creating a solution: the more widespread and inconvenient
the problem, the greater the opportunity to develop a scalable solution.

There are many specialized problem-solving techniques and methods in fields such as science,
engineering, business, medicine, mathematics, computer science, philosophy, and social
organization. The mental techniques to identify, analyze, and solve problems are studied in
psychology and cognitive sciences. Also widely researched are the mental obstacles that prevent
people from finding solutions; problem-solving impediments include confirmation bias, mental set,
and functional fixedness.

Definition

The term problem solving has a slightly different meaning depending on the discipline. For instance,
it is a mental process in psychology and a computerized process in computer science. There are
two different types of problems: ill-defined and well-defined; different approaches are used for each.
Well-defined problems have specific end goals and clearly expected solutions, while ill-defined
problems do not. Well-defined problems allow for more initial planning than ill-defined problems.[2]
Solving problems sometimes involves dealing with pragmatics (the way that context contributes to
meaning) and semantics (the interpretation of the problem). The ability to understand what the end
goal of the problem is, and what rules could be applied, represents the key to solving the problem.
Sometimes a problem requires abstract thinking or coming up with a creative solution.
Problem solving has two major domains: mathematical problem solving and personal problem
solving. Each concerns some difficulty or barrier that is encountered.[4]

Psychology

Problem solving in psychology refers to the process of finding solutions to problems encountered in
life.[5] Solutions to these problems are usually situation- or context-specific. The process starts with
problem finding and problem shaping, in which the problem is discovered and simplified. The next
step is to generate possible solutions and evaluate them. Finally a solution is selected to be
implemented and verified. Problems have an end goal to be reached; how you get there depends
upon problem orientation (problem-solving coping style and skills) and systematic analysis.[6]

Mental health professionals study the human problem-solving processes using methods such as
introspection, behaviorism, simulation, computer modeling, and experiment. Social psychologists
look into the person-environment relationship aspect of the problem and independent and
interdependent problem-solving methods.[7] Problem solving has been defined as a higher-order
cognitive process and intellectual function that requires the modulation and control of more routine
or fundamental skills.[8]

Empirical research shows many different strategies and factors influence everyday problem
solving.[9] Rehabilitation psychologists studying people with frontal lobe injuries have found that
deficits in emotional control and reasoning can be re-mediated with effective rehabilitation and
could improve the capacity of injured persons to resolve everyday problems.[10] Interpersonal
everyday problem solving is dependent upon personal motivational and contextual components.
One such component is the emotional valence of "real-world" problems, which can either impede or
aid problem-solving performance. Researchers have focused on the role of emotions in problem
solving,[11] demonstrating that poor emotional control can disrupt focus on the target task, impede
problem resolution, and lead to negative outcomes such as fatigue, depression, and inertia.[12] In
conceptualization, human problem solving consists of two related processes: problem orientation,
and the motivational/attitudinal/affective approach to problematic situations and problem-solving
skills.[13] People's strategies cohere with their goals[14] and stem from the process of comparing
oneself with others.

Cognitive sciences

Among the first experimental psychologists to study problem solving were the Gestaltists in
Germany, such as Karl Duncker in The Psychology of Productive Thinking (1935).[15] Perhaps best
known is the work of Allen Newell and Herbert A. Simon.[16]

Experiments in the 1960s and early 1970s asked participants to solve relatively simple, well-defined,
but not previously seen laboratory tasks.[17][18] These simple problems, such as the Tower of Hanoi,
admitted optimal solutions that could be found quickly, allowing researchers to observe the full
problem-solving process. Researchers assumed that these model problems would elicit the
characteristic cognitive processes by which more complex "real world" problems are solved.

An outstanding problem-solving technique found by this research is the principle of


decomposition.[19]

Computer science

Much of computer science and artificial intelligence involves designing automated systems to solve
a specified type of problem: to accept input data and calculate a correct or adequate response,
reasonably quickly. Algorithms are recipes or instructions that direct such systems, written into
computer programs.

Steps for designing such systems include problem determination, heuristics, root cause analysis,
de-duplication, analysis, diagnosis, and repair. Analytic techniques include linear and nonlinear
programming, queuing systems, and simulation.[20] A large, perennial obstacle is to find and fix
errors in computer programs: debugging.

Logic

Formal logic concerns issues like validity, truth, inference, argumentation, and proof. In a problem-
solving context, it can be used to formally represent a problem as a theorem to be proved, and to
represent the knowledge needed to solve the problem as the premises to be used in a proof that the
problem has a solution.

The use of computers to prove mathematical theorems using formal logic emerged as the field of
automated theorem proving in the 1950s. It included the use of heuristic methods designed to
simulate human problem solving, as in the Logic Theory Machine, developed by Allen Newell,
Herbert A. Simon and J. C. Shaw, as well as algorithmic methods such as the resolution principle
developed by John Alan Robinson.

In addition to its use for finding proofs of mathematical theorems, automated theorem-proving has
also been used for program verification in computer science. In 1958, John McCarthy proposed the
advice taker, to represent information in formal logic and to derive answers to questions using
automated theorem-proving. An important step in this direction was made by Cordell Green in 1969,
who used a resolution theorem prover for question-answering and for such other applications in
artificial intelligence as robot planning.

The resolution theorem-prover used by Cordell Green bore little resemblance to human problem
solving methods. In response to criticism of that approach from researchers at MIT, Robert
Kowalski developed logic programming and SLD resolution,[21] which solves problems by problem
decomposition. He has advocated logic for both computer and human problem solving[22] and
computational logic to improve human thinking.[23]

Engineering

When products or processes fail, problem solving techniques can be used to develop corrective
actions that can be taken to prevent further failures. Such techniques can also be applied to a
product or process prior to an actual failure event—to predict, analyze, and mitigate a potential
problem in advance. Techniques such as failure mode and effects analysis can proactively reduce
the likelihood of problems.

In either the reactive or the proactive case, it is necessary to build a causal explanation through a
process of diagnosis. In deriving an explanation of effects in terms of causes, abduction generates
new ideas or hypotheses (asking "how?"); deduction evaluates and refines hypotheses based on
other plausible premises (asking "why?"); and induction justifies a hypothesis with empirical data
(asking "how much?").[24] The objective of abduction is to determine which hypothesis or
proposition to test, not which one to adopt or assert.[25] In the Peircean logical system, the logic of
abduction and deduction contribute to our conceptual understanding of a phenomenon, while the
logic of induction adds quantitative details (empirical substantiation) to our conceptual
knowledge.[26]

Forensic engineering is an important technique of failure analysis that involves tracing product
defects and flaws. Corrective action can then be taken to prevent further failures.

Reverse engineering attempts to discover the original problem-solving logic used in developing a
product by disassembling the product and developing a plausible pathway to creating and
assembling its parts.[27]
Military science

In military science, problem solving is linked to the concept of "end-states", the conditions or
situations which are the aims of the strategy.[28]: xiii, E-2 Ability to solve problems is important at any
military rank, but is essential at the command and control level. It results from deep qualitative and
quantitative understanding of possible scenarios. Effectiveness in this context is an evaluation of
results: to what extent the end states were accomplished.[28]: IV-24 Planning is the process of
determining how to effect those end states.[28]: IV-1

Processes

Some models of problem solving involve identifying a goal and then a sequence of subgoals
towards achieving this goal. Andersson, who introduced the ACT-R model of cognition, modelled
this collection of goals and subgoals as a goal stack in which the mind contains a stack of goals
and subgoals to be completed, and a single task being carried out at any time.[29]: 51

Knowledge of how to solve one problem can be applied to another problem, in a process known as
transfer.[29]: 56

Problem-solving strategies

Problem-solving strategies are steps to overcoming the obstacles to achieving a goal. The iteration
of such strategies over the course of solving a problem is the "problem-solving cycle".[30]

Common steps in this cycle include recognizing the problem, defining it, developing a strategy to fix
it, organizing knowledge and resources available, monitoring progress, and evaluating the
effectiveness of the solution. Once a solution is achieved, another problem usually arises, and the
cycle starts again.

Insight is the sudden aha! solution to a problem, the birth of a new idea to simplify a complex
situation. Solutions found through insight are often more incisive than those from step-by-step
analysis. A quick solution process requires insight to select productive moves at different stages of
the problem-solving cycle. Unlike Newell and Simon's formal definition of a move problem, there is
no consensus definition of an insight problem.[31]

Some problem-solving strategies include:[32]

Abstraction
solving the problem in a tractable model system to gain insight into the real system
Analogy
adapting the solution to a previous problem which has similar features or mechanisms
Brainstorming
(especially among groups of people) suggesting a large number of solutions or ideas and
combining and developing them until an optimum solution is found
Bypasses
transform the problem into another problem that is easier to solve, bypassing the barrier, then
transform that solution back to a solution to the original problem.
Critical thinking
analysis of available evidence and arguments to form a judgement via rational, skeptical, and
unbiased evaluation
Divide and conquer
breaking down a large, complex problem into smaller, solvable problems
Help-seeking
obtaining external assistance to deal with obstacles
Hypothesis testing
assuming a possible explanation to the problem and trying to prove (or, in some contexts,
disprove) the assumption
Lateral thinking
approaching solutions indirectly and creatively
Means-ends analysis
choosing an action at each step to move closer to the goal
Morphological analysis
assessing the output and interactions of an entire system
Observation / Question
in the natural sciences an observation is an act or instance of noticing or perceiving and the
acquisition of information from a primary source. A question is an utterance which serves as a
request for information.
Proof of impossibility
try to prove that the problem cannot be solved. The point where the proof fails will be the starting
point for solving it
Reduction
transforming the problem into another problem for which solutions exist
Research
employing existing ideas or adapting existing solutions to similar problems
Root cause analysis
identifying the cause of a problem
Trial-and-error
testing possible solutions until the right one is found

Problem-solving methods

A3 problem solving – Structured problem improvement approach

Design thinking – Processes by which design concepts are developed

Eight Disciplines Problem Solving – Eight disciplines of team-oriented problem solving method

GROW model – Method for goal setting and problem solving

Help-seeking – Theory in psychology

How to Solve It – Book by George Pólya

Lateral thinking – Manner of solving problems

OODA loop – Observe–orient–decide–act cycle

PDCA – Iterative design and management method

Root cause analysis – Method of identifying the fundamental causes of faults or problems

RPR problem diagnosis – problem diagnosis method designed to determine the root cause of IT
problems

TRIZ – Problem-solving tools

Scientific method – is an empirical method for acquiring knowledge that has characterized the
development of science.

Swarm intelligence – Collective behavior of decentralized, self-organized systems

System dynamics – Study of non-linear complex systems

Common barriers

Common barriers to problem solving include mental constructs that impede an efficient search for
solutions. Five of the most common identified by researchers are: confirmation bias, mental set,
functional fixedness, unnecessary constraints, and irrelevant information.
Confirmation bias

Confirmation bias is an unintentional tendency to collect and use data which favors preconceived
notions. Such notions may be incidental rather than motivated by important personal beliefs: the
desire to be right may be sufficient motivation.[33]

Scientific and technical professionals also experience confirmation bias. One online experiment, for
example, suggested that professionals within the field of psychological research are likely to view
scientific studies that agree with their preconceived notions more favorably than clashing
studies.[34] According to Raymond Nickerson, one can see the consequences of confirmation bias in
real-life situations, which range in severity from inefficient government policies to genocide.
Nickerson argued that those who killed people accused of witchcraft demonstrated confirmation
bias with motivation. Researcher Michael Allen found evidence for confirmation bias with
motivation in school children who worked to manipulate their science experiments to produce
favorable results.[35]

However, confirmation bias does not necessarily require motivation. In 1960, Peter Cathcart Wason
conducted an experiment in which participants first viewed three numbers and then created a
hypothesis in the form of a rule that could have been used to create that triplet of numbers. When
testing their hypotheses, participants tended to only create additional triplets of numbers that would
confirm their hypotheses, and tended not to create triplets that would negate or disprove their
hypotheses.[36]

Mental set

Mental set is the inclination to re-use a previously successful solution, rather than search for new
and better solutions. It is a reliance on habit.

It was first articulated by Abraham S. Luchins in the 1940s with his well-known water jug
experiments.[37] Participants were asked to fill one jug with a specific amount of water by using
other jugs with different maximum capacities. After Luchins gave a set of jug problems that could
all be solved by a single technique, he then introduced a problem that could be solved by the same
technique, but also by a novel and simpler method. His participants tended to use the accustomed
technique, oblivious of the simpler alternative.[38] This was again demonstrated in Norman Maier's
1931 experiment, which challenged participants to solve a problem by using a familiar tool (pliers)
in an unconventional manner. Participants were often unable to view the object in a way that strayed
from its typical use, a type of mental set known as functional fixedness (see the following section).
Rigidly clinging to a mental set is called fixation, which can deepen to an obsession or
preoccupation with attempted strategies that are repeatedly unsuccessful.[39] In the late 1990s,
researcher Jennifer Wiley found that professional expertise in a field can create a mental set,
perhaps leading to fixation.[39]

Groupthink, in which each individual takes on the mindset of the rest of the group, can produce and
exacerbate mental set.[40] Social pressure leads to everybody thinking the same thing and reaching
the same conclusions.

Functional fixedness

Functional fixedness is the tendency to view an object as having only one function, and to be unable
to conceive of any novel use, as in the Maier pliers experiment described above. Functional
fixedness is a specific form of mental set, and is one of the most common forms of cognitive bias
in daily life.

As an example, imagine a man wants to kill a bug in his house, but the only thing at hand is a can of
air freshener. He may start searching for something to kill the bug instead of squashing it with the
can, thinking only of its main function of deodorizing.

Tim German and Clark Barrett describe this barrier: "subjects become 'fixed' on the design function
of the objects, and problem solving suffers relative to control conditions in which the object's
function is not demonstrated."[41] Their research found that young children's limited knowledge of an
object's intended function reduces this barrier[42] Research has also discovered functional fixedness
in educational contexts, as an obstacle to understanding: "functional fixedness may be found in
learning concepts as well as in solving chemistry problems."[43]

There are several hypotheses in regards to how functional fixedness relates to problem solving.[44] It
may waste time, delaying or entirely preventing the correct use of a tool.

Unnecessary constraints

Unnecessary constraints are arbitrary boundaries imposed unconsciously on the task at hand,
which foreclose a productive avenue of solution. The solver may become fixated on only one type of
solution, as if it were an inevitable requirement of the problem. Typically, this combines with mental
set—clinging to a previously successful method.[45]
Visual problems can also produce mentally invented constraints.[46] A famous example is the dot
problem: nine dots arranged in a three-by-three grid pattern must be connected by drawing four
straight line segments, without lifting pen from paper or backtracking along a line. The subject
typically assumes the pen must stay within the outer square of dots, but the solution requires lines
continuing beyond this frame, and researchers have found a 0% solution rate within a brief allotted
time.[47]

This problem has produced the expression "think outside the box".[48] Such problems are typically
solved via a sudden insight which leaps over the mental barriers, often after long toil against
them.[49] This can be difficult depending on how the subject has structured the problem in their
mind, how they draw on past experiences, and how well they juggle this information in their working
memory. In the example, envisioning the dots connected outside the framing square requires
visualizing an unconventional arrangement, which is a strain on working memory.[48]

Irrelevant information

Irrelevant information is a specification or data presented in a problem that is unrelated to the


solution.[45] If the solver assumes that all information presented needs to be used, this often derails
the problem solving process, making relatively simple problems much harder.[50]

For example: "Fifteen percent of the people in Topeka have unlisted telephone numbers. You select
200 names at random from the Topeka phone book. How many of these people have unlisted phone
numbers?"[48] The "obvious" answer is 15%, but in fact none of the unlisted people would be listed
among the 200. This kind of "trick question" is often used in aptitude tests or cognitive
evaluations.[51] Though not inherently difficult, they require independent thinking that is not
necessarily common. Mathematical word problems often include irrelevant qualitative or numerical
information as an extra challenge.

Avoiding barriers by changing problem representation

The disruption caused by the above cognitive biases can depend on how the information is
represented:[51] visually, verbally, or mathematically. A classic example is the Buddhist monk
problem:

A Buddhist monk begins at dawn one day walking up a mountain, reaches the top at
sunset, meditates at the top for several days until one dawn when he begins to walk
back to the foot of the mountain, which he reaches at sunset. Making no
assumptions about his starting or stopping or about his pace during the trips, prove
that there is a place on the path which he occupies at the same hour of the day on
the two separate journeys.

The problem cannot be addressed in a verbal context, trying to describe the monk's progress on
each day. It becomes much easier when the paragraph is represented mathematically by a function:
one visualizes a graph whose horizontal axis is time of day, and whose vertical axis shows the
monk's position (or altitude) on the path at each time. Superimposing the two journey curves, which
traverse opposite diagonals of a rectangle, one sees they must cross each other somewhere. The
visual representation by graphing has resolved the difficulty.

Similar strategies can often improve problem solving on tests.[45][52]

Other barriers for individuals

People who are engaged in problem solving tend to overlook subtractive changes, even those that
are critical elements of efficient solutions. For example, a city planner may decide that the solution
to decrease traffic congestion would be to add another lane to a highway, rather than finding ways
to reduce the need for the highway in the first place. This tendency to solve by first, only, or mostly
creating or adding elements, rather than by subtracting elements or processes is shown to intensify
with higher cognitive loads such as information overload.[53]

Dreaming: problem solving without waking consciousness

People can also solve problems while they are asleep. There are many reports of scientists and
engineers who solved problems in their dreams. For example, Elias Howe, inventor of the sewing
machine, figured out the structure of the bobbin from a dream.[54]

The chemist August Kekulé was considering how benzene arranged its six carbon and hydrogen
atoms. Thinking about the problem, he dozed off, and dreamt of dancing atoms that fell into a
snakelike pattern, which led him to discover the benzene ring. As Kekulé wrote in his diary,

One of the snakes seized hold of its own tail, and the form whirled mockingly before
my eyes. As if by a flash of lightning I awoke; and this time also I spent the rest of
the night in working out the consequences of the hypothesis.[55]
There also are empirical studies of how people can think consciously about a problem before going
to sleep, and then solve the problem with a dream image. Dream researcher William C. Dement told
his undergraduate class of 500 students that he wanted them to think about an infinite series,
whose first elements were OTTFF, to see if they could deduce the principle behind it and to say what
the next elements of the series would be.[56] He asked them to think about this problem every night
for 15 minutes before going to sleep and to write down any dreams that they then had. They were
instructed to think about the problem again for 15 minutes when they awakened in the morning.

The sequence OTTFF is the first letters of the numbers: one, two, three, four, five. The next five
elements of the series are SSENT (six, seven, eight, nine, ten). Some of the students solved the
puzzle by reflecting on their dreams. One example was a student who reported the following
dream:[56]

I was standing in an art gallery, looking at the paintings on the wall. As I walked
down the hall, I began to count the paintings: one, two, three, four, five. As I came to
the sixth and seventh, the paintings had been ripped from their frames. I stared at
the empty frames with a peculiar feeling that some mystery was about to be solved.
Suddenly I realized that the sixth and seventh spaces were the solution to the
problem!

With more than 500 undergraduate students, 87 dreams were judged to be related to the problems
students were assigned (53 directly related and 34 indirectly related). Yet of the people who had
dreams that apparently solved the problem, only seven were actually able to consciously know the
solution. The rest (46 out of 53) thought they did not know the solution.

Albert Einstein believed that much problem solving goes on unconsciously, and the person must
then figure out and formulate consciously what the mindbrain has already solved. He believed this
was his process in formulating the theory of relativity: "The creator of the problem possesses the
solution."[57] Einstein said that he did his problem solving without words, mostly in images. "The
words or the language, as they are written or spoken, do not seem to play any role in my mechanism
of thought. The psychical entities which seem to serve as elements in thought are certain signs and
more or less clear images which can be 'voluntarily' reproduced and combined."[58]

Cognitive sciences: two schools

Problem-solving processes differ across knowledge domains and across levels of expertise.[59] For
this reason, cognitive sciences findings obtained in the laboratory cannot necessarily generalize to
problem-solving situations outside the laboratory. This has led to a research emphasis on real-world
problem solving, since the 1990s. This emphasis has been expressed quite differently in North
America and Europe, however. Whereas North American research has typically concentrated on
studying problem solving in separate, natural knowledge domains, much of the European research
has focused on novel, complex problems, and has been performed with computerized scenarios.[60]

Europe

In Europe, two main approaches have surfaced, one initiated by Donald Broadbent[61] in the United
Kingdom and the other one by Dietrich Dörner[62] in Germany. The two approaches share an
emphasis on relatively complex, semantically rich, computerized laboratory tasks, constructed to
resemble real-life problems. The approaches differ somewhat in their theoretical goals and
methodology. The tradition initiated by Broadbent emphasizes the distinction between cognitive
problem-solving processes that operate under awareness versus outside of awareness, and
typically employs mathematically well-defined computerized systems. The tradition initiated by
Dörner, on the other hand, has an interest in the interplay of the cognitive, motivational, and social
components of problem solving, and utilizes very complex computerized scenarios that contain up
to 2,000 highly interconnected variables.[63]

North America

In North America, initiated by the work of Herbert A. Simon on "learning by doing" in semantically
rich domains,[64] researchers began to investigate problem solving separately in different natural
knowledge domains—such as physics, writing, or chess playing—rather than attempt to extract a
global theory of problem solving.[65] These researchers have focused on the development of
problem solving within certain domains, that is on the development of expertise.[66]

Areas that have attracted rather intensive attention in North America include:

calculation[67]

computer skills[68]

game playing[69]

lawyers' reasoning[70]

managerial problem solving[71]

mathematical problem solving[72]

mechanical problem solving[73]


personal problem solving[74]

political decision making[75]

problem solving in electronics[76]

problem solving for innovations and inventions: TRIZ[77]

reading[78]

social problem solving[11]

writing[79]

Characteristics of complex problems

Complex problem solving (CPS) is distinguishable from simple problem solving (SPS). In SPS there
is a singular and simple obstacle. In CPS there may be multiple simultaneous obstacles. For
example, a surgeon at work has far more complex problems than an individual deciding what shoes
to wear. As elucidated by Dietrich Dörner, and later expanded upon by Joachim Funke, complex
problems have some typical characteristics, which include:[1]

complexity (large numbers of items, interrelations, and decisions)

enumerability

heterogeneity

connectivity (hierarchy relation, communication relation, allocation relation)

dynamics (time considerations)


temporal constraints

temporal sensitivity

phase effects

dynamic unpredictability

intransparency (lack of clarity of the situation)


commencement opacity

continuation opacity

polytely (multiple goals)[80]


inexpressivenes

opposition
transience

Collective problem solving

People solve problems on many different levels—from the individual to the civilizational. Collective
problem solving refers to problem solving performed collectively. Social issues and global issues
can typically only be solved collectively.

The complexity of contemporary problems exceeds the cognitive capacity of any individual and
requires different but complementary varieties of expertise and collective problem solving ability.[81]

Collective intelligence is shared or group intelligence that emerges from the collaboration, collective
efforts, and competition of many individuals.

In collaborative problem solving people work together to solve real-world problems. Members of
problem-solving groups share a common concern, a similar passion, and/or a commitment to their
work. Members can ask questions, wonder, and try to understand common issues. They share
expertise, experiences, tools, and methods.[82] Groups may be fluid based on need, may only occur
temporarily to finish an assigned task, or may be more permanent depending on the nature of the
problems.

For example, in the educational context, members of a group may all have input into the decision-
making process and a role in the learning process. Members may be responsible for the thinking,
teaching, and monitoring of all members in the group. Group work may be coordinated among
members so that each member makes an equal contribution to the whole work. Members can
identify and build on their individual strengths so that everyone can make a significant contribution
to the task.[83] Collaborative group work has the ability to promote critical thinking skills, problem
solving skills, social skills, and self-esteem. By using collaboration and communication, members
often learn from one another and construct meaningful knowledge that often leads to better
learning outcomes than individual work.[84]

Collaborative groups require joint intellectual efforts between the members and involve social
interactions to solve problems together. The knowledge shared during these interactions is acquired
during communication, negotiation, and production of materials.[85] Members actively seek
information from others by asking questions. The capacity to use questions to acquire new
information increases understanding and the ability to solve problems.[86]

In a 1962 research report, Douglas Engelbart linked collective intelligence to organizational


effectiveness, and predicted that proactively "augmenting human intellect" would yield a multiplier
effect in group problem solving: "Three people working together in this augmented mode [would]
seem to be more than three times as effective in solving a complex problem as is one augmented
person working alone".[87]

Henry Jenkins, a theorist of new media and media convergence, draws on the theory that collective
intelligence can be attributed to media convergence and participatory culture.[88] He criticizes
contemporary education for failing to incorporate online trends of collective problem solving into
the classroom, stating "whereas a collective intelligence community encourages ownership of work
as a group, schools grade individuals". Jenkins argues that interaction within a knowledge
community builds vital skills for young people, and teamwork through collective intelligence
communities contributes to the development of such skills.[89]

Collective impact is the commitment of a group of actors from different sectors to a common
agenda for solving a specific social problem, using a structured form of collaboration.

After World War II the UN, the Bretton Woods organization, and the WTO were created. Collective
problem solving on the international level crystallized around these three types of organization from
the 1980s onward. As these global institutions remain state-like or state-centric it is unsurprising
that they perpetuate state-like or state-centric approaches to collective problem solving rather than
alternative ones.[90]

Crowdsourcing is a process of accumulating ideas, thoughts, or information from many


independent participants, with aim of finding the best solution for a given challenge. Modern
information technologies allow for many people to be involved and facilitate managing their
suggestions in ways that provide good results.[91] The Internet allows for a new capacity of
collective (including planetary-scale) problem solving.[92]

See also

Actuarial science – Statistics applied to risk in insurance and other


Philosophy portal
financial products
Psychology portal
Analytical skill – Crucial skill in all different fields of work and life

Creative problem-solving – Mental process of problem solving

Collective intelligence – Group intelligence that emerges from collective efforts

Community of practice
Coworking – Practice of independent contractors or scientists sharing office space without
supervision

Crowdsolving – Sourcing services or funds from a group

Divergent thinking – A process of generating creative ideas

Grey problem – IT service problem where the causing technology is unknown or unconfirmed,
making the problem solving difficult to allocate

Innovation – Practical implementation of improvements

Instrumentalism – Position in the philosophy of science

Problem-posing education – Method of teaching coined by Paulo Freire

Problem statement – Description of an issue

Problem structuring methods

Shared intentionality – Ability to engage with others' psychological states

Structural fix – solving a problem or resolving a conflict by bringing about structural changes in
underlying structures that provoked or sustained these problems

Subgoal labeling – Cognitive process

Troubleshooting – Form of problem solving, often applied to repair failed products or processes

Wicked problem – Problem that is difficult or impossible to solve

Notes

1. Frensch, Peter A.; Funke, Joachim, eds. (2014-04-04). Complex Problem Solving. Psychology
Press. doi:10.4324/9781315806723 (https://doi.org/10.4324%2F9781315806723) .
ISBN 978-1-315-80672-3.

2. Schacter, D.L.; Gilbert, D.T.; Wegner, D.M. (2011). Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Worth
Publishers. p. 376.

3. Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). "Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental


perspective". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (1): 26–31. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
8721.2007.00469.x (https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-8721.2007.00469.x) .
S2CID 145645352 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:145645352) .

4. Zimmermann, Bernd (2004). On mathematical problem-solving processes and history of


mathematics (https://www.researchgate.net/publication/238733375) . ICME 10. Copenhagen.
5. Granvold, Donald K. (1997). "Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy with Adults". In Brandell, Jerrold R.
(ed.). Theory and Practice in Clinical Social Work. Simon and Schuster. pp. 189 (https://books.g
oogle.com/books?id=K9Hm0UuFGJ0C&pg=PA189) . ISBN 978-0-684-82765-0.

6. Robertson, S. Ian (2001). "Introduction to the study of problem solving". Problem Solving.
Psychology Press. ISBN 0-415-20300-7.

7. Rubin, M.; Watt, S. E.; Ramelli, M. (2012). "Immigrants' social integration as a function of
approach-avoidance orientation and problem-solving style". International Journal of Intercultural
Relations. 36 (4): 498–505. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.12.009 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.ijint
rel.2011.12.009) . hdl:1959.13/931119 (https://hdl.handle.net/1959.13%2F931119) .

8. Goldstein F. C.; Levin H. S. (1987). "Disorders of reasoning and problem-solving ability". In M.


Meier; A. Benton; L. Diller (eds.). Neuropsychological rehabilitation. London: Taylor & Francis
Group.

9. Vallacher, Robin; M. Wegner, Daniel (2012). "Action Identification Theory". Handbook of Theories
of Social Psychology. pp. 327–348. doi:10.4135/9781446249215.n17 (https://doi.org/10.413
5%2F9781446249215.n17) . ISBN 978-0-85702-960-7.

Margrett, J. A; Marsiske, M (2002). "Gender differences in older adults' everyday cognitive


collaboration" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2909137) . International
Journal of Behavioral Development. 26 (1): 45–59. doi:10.1080/01650250143000319 (https://d
oi.org/10.1080%2F01650250143000319) . PMC 2909137 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pm
c/articles/PMC2909137) . PMID 20657668 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20657668) .

Antonucci, T. C; Ajrouch, K. J; Birditt, K. S (2013). "The Convoy Model: Explaining Social


Relations From a Multidisciplinary Perspective" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/P
MC3894851) . The Gerontologist. 54 (1): 82–92. doi:10.1093/geront/gnt118 (https://doi.org/1
0.1093%2Fgeront%2Fgnt118) . PMC 3894851 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/P
MC3894851) . PMID 24142914 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24142914) .

10. Rath, Joseph F.; Simon, Dvorah; Langenbahn, Donna M.; Sherr, Rose Lynn; Diller, Leonard
(2003). "Group treatment of problem-solving deficits in outpatients with traumatic brain injury:
A randomised outcome study" (https://www.researchgate.net/publication/247514323) .
Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 13 (4): 461–488. doi:10.1080/09602010343000039 (https://
doi.org/10.1080%2F09602010343000039) . S2CID 143165070 (https://api.semanticscholar.o
rg/CorpusID:143165070) .
11. D'Zurilla, T. J.; Goldfried, M. R. (1971). "Problem solving and behavior modification". Journal of
Abnormal Psychology. 78 (1): 107–126. doi:10.1037/h0031360 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fh0
031360) . PMID 4938262 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/4938262) .

D'Zurilla, T. J.; Nezu, A. M. (1982). "Social problem solving in adults". In P. C. Kendall (ed.).
Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press.
pp. 201–274.

12. Rath, J. F.; Langenbahn, D. M.; Simon, D; Sherr, R. L.; Fletcher, J.; Diller, L. (2004). "The construct
of problem solving in higher level neuropsychological assessment and rehabilitation*1" (http
s://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.acn.2003.08.006) . Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 19 (5):
613–635. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2003.08.006 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.acn.2003.08.006) .
PMID 15271407 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15271407) .

13. Rath, Joseph F.; Hradil, Amy L.; Litke, David R.; Diller, Leonard (2011). "Clinical applications of
problem-solving research in neuropsychological rehabilitation: Addressing the subjective
experience of cognitive deficits in outpatients with acquired brain injury" (https://www.ncbi.nl
m.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9728040) . Rehabilitation Psychology. 56 (4): 320–328.
doi:10.1037/a0025817 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fa0025817) . ISSN 1939-1544 (https://sear
ch.worldcat.org/issn/1939-1544) . PMC 9728040 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/article
s/PMC9728040) . PMID 22121939 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22121939) .

14. Hoppmann, Christiane A.; Blanchard-Fields, Fredda (2010). "Goals and everyday problem
solving: Manipulating goal preferences in young and older adults". Developmental Psychology.
46 (6): 1433–1443. doi:10.1037/a0020676 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fa0020676) .
PMID 20873926 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20873926) .

15. Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The psychology of productive
thinking] (in German). Berlin: Julius Springer.

16. Newell, Allen; Simon, Herbert A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.:
Prentice-Hall.
17. For example:
X-ray problem, by Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The
psychology of productive thinking] (in German). Berlin: Julius Springer.

Disk problem, later known as Tower of Hanoi, by Ewert, P. H.; Lambert, J. F. (1932). "Part II:
The Effect of Verbal Instructions upon the Formation of a Concept" (https://www.tandfonli
ne.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880) . The Journal of General Psychology.
6 (2). Informa UK Limited: 400–413. doi:10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880 (https://doi.or
g/10.1080%2F00221309.1932.9711880) . ISSN 0022-1309 (https://search.worldcat.org/i
ssn/0022-1309) . Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20200806135752/https://www.
tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880) from the original on 2020-
08-06. Retrieved 2019-06-09.

18. Mayer, R. E. (1992). Thinking, problem solving, cognition (Second ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman
and Company.

19. Armstrong, J. Scott; Denniston, William B. Jr.; Gordon, Matt M. (1975). "The Use of the
Decomposition Principle in Making Judgments" (https://web.archive.org/web/2010062022171
3/http://marketing.wharton.upenn.edu/ideas/pdf/armstrong2/DecompositionPrinciple.pdf)
(PDF). Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 14 (2): 257–263. doi:10.1016/0030-
5073(75)90028-8 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2F0030-5073%2875%2990028-8) .
S2CID 122659209 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:122659209) . Archived from the
original (http://marketing.wharton.upenn.edu/ideas/pdf/armstrong2/DecompositionPrinciple.p
df) (PDF) on 2010-06-20.

20. Malakooti, Behnam (2013). Operations and Production Systems with Multiple Objectives. John
Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-1-118-58537-5.

21. Kowalski, Robert (1974). "Predicate Logic as a Programming Language" (https://www.doc.ic.a


c.uk/~rak/papers/IFIP%2074.pdf) (PDF). Information Processing. 74. Archived (https://web.ar
chive.org/web/20240119025430/https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/IFIP%2074.pdf)
(PDF) from the original on 2024-01-19. Retrieved 2023-09-20.

22. Kowalski, Robert (1979). Logic for Problem Solving (https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/Log


icForProblemSolving.pdf) (PDF). Artificial Intelligence Series. Vol. 7. Elsevier Science
Publishing. ISBN 0-444-00368-1. Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20231102032823/http
s://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/LogicForProblemSolving.pdf) (PDF) from the original on
2023-11-02. Retrieved 2023-09-20.
23. Kowalski, Robert (2011). Computational Logic and Human Thinking: How to be Artificially
Intelligent (https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~rak/papers/newbook.pdf) (PDF). Cambridge University
Press. Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20240601181910/https://www.doc.ic.ac.uk/~ra
k/papers/newbook.pdf) (PDF) from the original on 2024-06-01. Retrieved 2023-09-20.

24. Staat, Wim (1993). "On abduction, deduction, induction and the categories". Transactions of the
Charles S. Peirce Society. 29 (2): 225–237.

25. Sullivan, Patrick F. (1991). "On Falsificationist Interpretations of Peirce". Transactions of the
Charles S. Peirce Society. 27 (2): 197–219.

26. Ho, Yu Chong (1994). Abduction? Deduction? Induction? Is There a Logic of Exploratory Data
Analysis? (https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED376173.pdf) (PDF). Annual Meeting of the
American Educational Research Association. New Orleans, La. Archived (https://web.archive.or
g/web/20231102041717/https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED376173.pdf) (PDF) from the
original on 2023-11-02. Retrieved 2023-09-20.

27. Passuello, Luciano (2008-11-04). "Einstein's Secret to Amazing Problem Solving (and 10
Specific Ways You Can Use It)" (https://web.archive.org/web/20170621145314/https://litemin
d.com/problem-definition/) . Litemind. Archived from the original (https://litemind.com/proble
m-definition/) on 2017-06-21. Retrieved 2017-06-11.

28. "Commander's Handbook for Strategic Communication and Communication Strategy" (https://
web.archive.org/web/20110429051434/http://www.au.af.mil/au/awc/awcgate/jfcom/cc_hand
book_sc_27oct2009.pdf) (PDF). United States Joint Forces Command, Joint Warfighting
Center, Suffolk, Va. 27 October 2009. Archived from the original (http://www.au.af.mil/au/awc/
awcgate/jfcom/cc_handbook_sc_27oct2009.pdf) (PDF) on April 29, 2011. Retrieved
10 October 2016.

29. Robertson, S. Ian (2017). Problem solving: perspectives from cognition and neuroscience
(2nd ed.). London: Taylor & Francis. ISBN 978-1-317-49601-4. OCLC 962750529 (https://searc
h.worldcat.org/oclc/962750529) .

30. Bransford, J. D.; Stein, B. S (1993). The ideal problem solver: A guide for improving thinking,
learning, and creativity (2nd ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman.
31. Ash, Ivan K.; Jee, Benjamin D.; Wiley, Jennifer (2012). "Investigating Insight as Sudden
Learning" (https://doi.org/10.7771%2F1932-6246.1123) . The Journal of Problem Solving. 4
(2). doi:10.7771/1932-6246.1123 (https://doi.org/10.7771%2F1932-6246.1123) . ISSN 1932-
6246 (https://search.worldcat.org/issn/1932-6246) .

Chronicle, Edward P.; MacGregor, James N.; Ormerod, Thomas C. (2004). "What Makes an
Insight Problem? The Roles of Heuristics, Goal Conception, and Solution Recoding in
Knowledge-Lean Problems" (https://eprints.lancs.ac.uk/id/eprint/558/2/Chronicle_et_alJEP_L
M%26C_03.pdf) (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition.
30 (1): 14–27. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.14 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0278-7393.30.1.1
4) . ISSN 1939-1285 (https://search.worldcat.org/issn/1939-1285) . PMID 14736293 (http
s://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/14736293) . S2CID 15631498 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/
CorpusID:15631498) .

Chu, Yun; MacGregor, James N. (2011). "Human Performance on Insight Problem Solving: A
Review" (https://doi.org/10.7771%2F1932-6246.1094) . The Journal of Problem Solving. 3 (2).
doi:10.7771/1932-6246.1094 (https://doi.org/10.7771%2F1932-6246.1094) . ISSN 1932-6246
(https://search.worldcat.org/issn/1932-6246) .

32. Wang, Y.; Chiew, V. (2010). "On the cognitive process of human problem solving" (https://www.r
esearchgate.net/profile/Patricia_Ryser-Welch/post/Do_Machines_learn/attachment/59d6235b
79197b8077981b28/AS:306908018216960@1450183981555/download/61-Elsevier-CogSys-
ProblemSolving.pdf) (PDF). Cognitive Systems Research. 11 (1). Elsevier BV: 81–92.
doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2008.08.003 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.cogsys.2008.08.003) .
ISSN 1389-0417 (https://search.worldcat.org/issn/1389-0417) . S2CID 16238486 (https://api.
semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:16238486) .

33. Nickerson, Raymond S. (1998). "Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises".
Review of General Psychology. 2 (2): 176. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175 (https://doi.org/10.1
037%2F1089-2680.2.2.175) . S2CID 8508954 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:8508
954) .

34. Hergovich, Andreas; Schott, Reinhard; Burger, Christoph (2010). "Biased Evaluation of Abstracts
Depending on Topic and Conclusion: Further Evidence of a Confirmation Bias Within Scientific
Psychology". Current Psychology. 29 (3). Springer Science and Business Media LLC: 188–209.
doi:10.1007/s12144-010-9087-5 (https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs12144-010-9087-5) .
ISSN 1046-1310 (https://search.worldcat.org/issn/1046-1310) . S2CID 145497196 (https://ap
i.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:145497196) .
35. Allen, Michael (2011). "Theory-led confirmation bias and experimental persona". Research in
Science & Technological Education. 29 (1). Informa UK Limited: 107–127.
Bibcode:2011RSTEd..29..107A (https://ui.adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/2011RSTEd..29..107A) .
doi:10.1080/02635143.2010.539973 (https://doi.org/10.1080%2F02635143.2010.539973) .
ISSN 0263-5143 (https://search.worldcat.org/issn/0263-5143) . S2CID 145706148 (https://ap
i.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:145706148) .

36. Wason, P. C. (1960). "On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task". Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology. 12 (3): 129–140. doi:10.1080/17470216008416717 (http
s://doi.org/10.1080%2F17470216008416717) . S2CID 19237642 (https://api.semanticschola
r.org/CorpusID:19237642) .

37. Luchins, Abraham S. (1942). "Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung".
Psychological Monographs. 54 (248): i-95. doi:10.1037/h0093502 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2F
h0093502) .

38. Öllinger, Michael; Jones, Gary; Knoblich, Günther (2008). "Investigating the Effect of Mental Set
on Insight Problem Solving" (http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/23048/1/193183_1563%20Jones%
20Postprint.pdf) (PDF). Experimental Psychology. 55 (4). Hogrefe Publishing Group: 269–
282. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.55.4.269 (https://doi.org/10.1027%2F1618-3169.55.4.269) .
ISSN 1618-3169 (https://search.worldcat.org/issn/1618-3169) . PMID 18683624 (https://pub
med.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/18683624) . Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20230316064717/
http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/23048/1/193183_1563%20Jones%20Postprint.pdf) (PDF)
from the original on 2023-03-16. Retrieved 2023-01-31.

39. Wiley, Jennifer (1998). "Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative
problem solving" (https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fbf03211392) . Memory & Cognition. 24 (4):
716–730. doi:10.3758/bf03211392 (https://doi.org/10.3758%2Fbf03211392) .
PMID 9701964 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/9701964) .

40. Cottam, Martha L.; Dietz-Uhler, Beth; Mastors, Elena; Preston, Thomas (2010). Introduction to
Political Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.

41. German, Tim P.; Barrett, H. Clark (2005). "Functional Fixedness in a Technologically Sparse
Culture". Psychological Science. 16 (1). SAGE Publications: 1–5. doi:10.1111/j.0956-
7976.2005.00771.x (https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.0956-7976.2005.00771.x) . ISSN 0956-7976
(https://search.worldcat.org/issn/0956-7976) . PMID 15660843 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.ni
h.gov/15660843) . S2CID 1833823 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:1833823) .
42. German, Tim P.; Defeyter, Margaret A. (2000). "Immunity to functional fixedness in young
children" (https://doi.org/10.3758%2FBF03213010) . Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 7 (4):
707–712. doi:10.3758/BF03213010 (https://doi.org/10.3758%2FBF03213010) .
PMID 11206213 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11206213) .

43. Furio, C.; Calatayud, M. L.; Baracenas, S.; Padilla, O. (2000). "Functional fixedness and
functional reduction as common sense reasonings in chemical equilibrium and in geometry
and polarity of molecules". Science Education. 84 (5): 545–565. Bibcode:2000SciEd..84..545F
(https://ui.adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/2000SciEd..84..545F) . doi:10.1002/1098-
237X(200009)84:5<545::AID-SCE1>3.0.CO;2-1 (https://doi.org/10.1002%2F1098-237X%28200
009%2984%3A5%3C545%3A%3AAID-SCE1%3E3.0.CO%3B2-1) .

44. Adamson, Robert E (1952). "Functional fixedness as related to problem solving: A repetition of
three experiments". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 44 (4): 288–291.
doi:10.1037/h0062487 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2Fh0062487) . PMID 13000071 (https://pub
med.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/13000071) .

45. Kellogg, R. T. (2003). Cognitive psychology (2nd ed.). California: Sage Publications, Inc.

46. Meloy, J. R. (1998). The Psychology of Stalking, Clinical and Forensic Perspectives (2nd ed.).
London, England: Academic Press.

47. MacGregor, J.N.; Ormerod, T.C.; Chronicle, E.P. (2001). "Information-processing and insight: A
process model of performance on the nine-dot and related problems". Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 27 (1): 176–201. doi:10.1037/0278-
7393.27.1.176 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0278-7393.27.1.176) . PMID 11204097 (https://pu
bmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11204097) .

48. Weiten, Wayne (2011). Psychology: themes and variations (8th ed.). California: Wadsworth.

49. Novick, L. R.; Bassok, M. (2005). "Problem solving". In Holyoak, K. J.; Morrison, R. G. (eds.).
Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. New York, N.Y.: Cambridge University Press.
pp. 321–349.

50. Walinga, Jennifer (2010). "From walls to windows: Using barriers as pathways to insightful
solutions". The Journal of Creative Behavior. 44 (3): 143–167. doi:10.1002/j.2162-
6057.2010.tb01331.x (https://doi.org/10.1002%2Fj.2162-6057.2010.tb01331.x) .

51. Walinga, Jennifer; Cunningham, J. Barton; MacGregor, James N. (2011). "Training insight
problem solving through focus on barriers and assumptions". The Journal of Creative Behavior.
45: 47–58. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2011.tb01084.x (https://doi.org/10.1002%2Fj.2162-6057.
2011.tb01084.x) .
52. Vlamings, Petra H. J. M.; Hare, Brian; Call, Joseph (2009). "Reaching around barriers: The
performance of great apes and 3–5-year-old children" (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/artic
les/PMC2822225) . Animal Cognition. 13 (2): 273–285. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0265-5 (http
s://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs10071-009-0265-5) . PMC 2822225 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/p
mc/articles/PMC2822225) . PMID 19653018 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/19653018) .

53. Gupta, Sujata (7 April 2021). "People add by default even when subtraction makes more sense"
(https://www.sciencenews.org/article/psychology-numbers-people-add-default-subtract-bette
r) . Science News. Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20210521134851/https://www.scie
ncenews.org/article/psychology-numbers-people-add-default-subtract-better) from the
original on 21 May 2021. Retrieved 10 May 2021.

Adams, Gabrielle S.; Converse, Benjamin A.; Hales, Andrew H.; Klotz, Leidy E. (April 2021).
"People systematically overlook subtractive changes" (https://www.nature.com/articles/s4158
6-021-03380-y) . Nature. 592 (7853): 258–261. Bibcode:2021Natur.592..258A (https://ui.adsa
bs.harvard.edu/abs/2021Natur.592..258A) . doi:10.1038/s41586-021-03380-y (https://doi.or
g/10.1038%2Fs41586-021-03380-y) . ISSN 1476-4687 (https://search.worldcat.org/issn/1476
-4687) . PMID 33828317 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33828317) . S2CID 233185662
(https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:233185662) . Archived (https://web.archive.org/w
eb/20210510130853/https://www.nature.com/articles/s41586-021-03380-y) from the
original on 10 May 2021. Retrieved 10 May 2021.

54. Kaempffert, Waldemar B. (1924). A Popular History of American Invention. Vol. 2. New York:
Charles Scribner's Sons. p. 385 (https://archive.org/details/popularhistoryof02kaem/page/38
5/mode/1up) .

55. Kekulé, August (1890). "Benzolfest-Rede". Berichte der Deutschen Chemischen Gesellschaft. 23:
1302–1311.

Benfey, O. (1958). "Kekulé and the birth of the structural theory of organic chemistry in 1858".
Journal of Chemical Education. 35 (1): 21–23. Bibcode:1958JChEd..35...21B (https://ui.adsabs.
harvard.edu/abs/1958JChEd..35...21B) . doi:10.1021/ed035p21 (https://doi.org/10.1021%2F
ed035p21) .

56. Dement, W.C. (1972). Some Must Watch While Some Just Sleep. New York: Freeman.

57. Fromm, Erika O. (1998). "Lost and found half a century later: Letters by Freud and Einstein".
American Psychologist. 53 (11): 1195–1198. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.53.11.1195 (https://doi.or
g/10.1037%2F0003-066x.53.11.1195) .

58. Einstein, Albert (1954). "A Mathematician's Mind". Ideas and Opinions. New York: Bonanza
Books. p. 25.
59. Sternberg, R. J. (1995). "Conceptions of expertise in complex problem solving: A comparison
of alternative conceptions". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The
European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 295–321.

60. Funke, J. (1991). "Solving complex problems: Human identification and control of complex
systems". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and
mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 185–222. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4.
OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/oclc/23254443) .

61. Broadbent, Donald E. (1977). "Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control" (https://journals.sag
epub.com/doi/abs/10.1080/14640747708400596) . Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology. 29 (2): 181–201. doi:10.1080/14640747708400596 (https://doi.org/10.1080%2F1
4640747708400596) . S2CID 144328372 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:1443283
72) . Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20200806214714/https://journals.sagepub.com/
doi/abs/10.1080/14640747708400596) from the original on 2020-08-06. Retrieved
2019-06-09.

Berry, Dianne C.; Broadbent, Donald E. (1995). "Implicit learning in the control of complex
systems: A reconsideration of some of the earlier claims" (https://www.researchgate.net/publi
cation/200134353) . In Frensch, P.A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European
Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 131–150.

62. Dörner, Dietrich (1975). "Wie Menschen eine Welt verbessern wollten" [How people wanted to
improve the world]. Bild der Wissenschaft (in German). 12: 48–53.

Dörner, Dietrich (1985). "Verhalten, Denken und Emotionen" [Behavior, thinking, and emotions].
In Eckensberger, L. H.; Lantermann, E. D. (eds.). Emotion und Reflexivität (in German). München,
Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg. pp. 157–181.

Dörner, Dietrich; Wearing, Alex J. (1995). "Complex problem solving: Toward a (computer-
simulated) theory" (https://www.researchgate.net/publication/200134353) . In Frensch, P.A.;
Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates. pp. 65–99.
63. Buchner, A. (1995). "Theories of complex problem solving". In Frensch, P.A.; Funke, J. (eds.).
Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 27–63.

Dörner, D.; Kreuzig, H. W.; Reither, F.; Stäudel, T., eds. (1983). Lohhausen. Vom Umgang mit
Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität [Lohhausen. On dealing with uncertainty and complexity] (in
German). Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.

Ringelband, O. J.; Misiak, C.; Kluwe, R. H. (1990). "Mental models and strategies in the control
of a complex system". In Ackermann, D.; Tauber, M. J. (eds.). Mental models and human-
computer interaction. Vol. 1. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. pp. 151–164.

64. Anzai, K.; Simon, H. A. (1979). "The theory of learning by doing". Psychological Review. 86 (2):
124–140. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.86.2.124 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0033-295X.86.2.12
4) . PMID 493441 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/493441) .

Bhaskar, R.; Simon, Herbert A. (1977). "Problem Solving in Semantically Rich Domains: An
Example from Engineering Thermodynamics" (https://doi.org/10.1207%2Fs15516709cog0102
_3) . Cognitive Science. 1 (2). Wiley: 193–215. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0102_3 (https://doi.
org/10.1207%2Fs15516709cog0102_3) . ISSN 0364-0213 (https://search.worldcat.org/issn/0
364-0213) .

65. e.g., Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A., eds. (1991). Complex problem solving: Principles and
mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4.
OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/oclc/23254443) .

66. Chase, W. G.; Simon, H. A. (1973). "Perception in chess". Cognitive Psychology. 4: 55–81.
doi:10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2F0010-0285%2873%2990004-
2) .

Chi, M. T. H.; Feltovich, P. J.; Glaser, R. (1981). "Categorization and representation of physics
problems by experts and novices" (https://doi.org/10.1207%2Fs15516709cog0502_2) .
Cognitive Science. 5 (2): 121–152. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0502_2 (https://doi.org/10.120
7%2Fs15516709cog0502_2) .

Anderson, J. R.; Boyle, C. B.; Reiser, B. J. (1985). "Intelligent tutoring systems". Science. 228
(4698): 456–462. Bibcode:1985Sci...228..456A (https://ui.adsabs.harvard.edu/abs/1985Sci...2
28..456A) . doi:10.1126/science.228.4698.456 (https://doi.org/10.1126%2Fscience.228.469
8.456) . PMID 17746875 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17746875) . S2CID 62403455 (ht
tps://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:62403455) .
67. Sokol, S. M.; McCloskey, M. (1991). "Cognitive mechanisms in calculation" (https://books.googl
e.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA85) . In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.).
Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 85–116. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/oc
lc/23254443) .

68. Kay, D. S. (1991). "Computer interaction: Debugging the problems" (https://books.google.com/


books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA317) . In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex
problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
pp. 317–340. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/oclc/232544
43) . Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20221204055601/https://books.google.com/bo
oks?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA317) from the original on 2022-12-04. Retrieved 2022-12-04.

69. Frensch, P. A.; Sternberg, R. J. (1991). "Skill-related differences in game playing" (https://books.
google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA343) . In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.).
Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J .: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 343–381. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/o
clc/23254443) .

70. Amsel, E.; Langer, R.; Loutzenhiser, L. (1991). "Do lawyers reason differently from
psychologists? A comparative design for studying expertise". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A.
(eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 223–250. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/o
clc/23254443) .

71. Wagner, R. K. (1991). "Managerial problem solving". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.).
Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 159–183. PsycNET: 1991-98396-005 (https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1991-98
396-005) .

72. Pólya, George (1945). How to Solve It. Princeton University Press.

Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985). Mathematical Problem Solving (https://books.google.com/books?id=


0cbSBQAAQBAJ) . Orlando, Fla.: Academic Press. ISBN 978-1-4832-9548-0. Archived (https://
web.archive.org/web/20231023053840/https://books.google.com/books?id=0cbSBQAAQBA
J) from the original on 2023-10-23. Retrieved 2019-06-09.
73. Hegarty, M. (1991). "Knowledge and processes in mechanical problem solving" (https://books.
google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA253) . In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.).
Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 253–285. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/o
clc/23254443) . Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20221204055603/https://books.goog
le.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA253) from the original on 2022-12-04. Retrieved
2022-12-04.

74. Heppner, P. P.; Krauskopf, C. J. (1987). "An information-processing approach to personal


problem solving". The Counseling Psychologist. 15 (3): 371–447.
doi:10.1177/0011000087153001 (https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0011000087153001) .
S2CID 146180007 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:146180007) .

75. Voss, J. F.; Wolfe, C. R.; Lawrence, J. A.; Engle, R. A. (1991). "From representation to decision:
An analysis of problem solving in international relations". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A.
(eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 119–158. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/o
clc/23254443) . PsycNET: 1991-98396-004 (https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1991-98396-00
4) .

76. Lesgold, A.; Lajoie, S. (1991). "Complex problem solving in electronics" (https://books.google.c
om/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA287) . In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex
problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
pp. 287–316. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/oclc/232544
43) . Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20221204055601/https://books.google.com/bo
oks?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA287) from the original on 2022-12-04. Retrieved 2022-12-04.

77. Altshuller, Genrich (1994). And Suddenly the Inventor Appeared. Translated by Lev Shulyak.
Worcester, Mass.: Technical Innovation Center. ISBN 978-0-9640740-1-9.

78. Stanovich, K. E.; Cunningham, A. E. (1991). "Reading as constrained reasoning" (https://books.


google.com/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA3) . In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.).
Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 3–60. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/oclc/
23254443) . Archived (https://web.archive.org/web/20230903202339/https://books.google.c
om/books?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA3) from the original on 2023-09-03. Retrieved
2022-12-04.
79. Bryson, M.; Bereiter, C.; Scardamalia, M.; Joram, E. (1991). "Going beyond the problem as given:
Problem solving in expert and novice writers". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex
problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
pp. 61–84. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/oclc/2325444
3) .

80. Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A., eds. (1991). Complex problem solving: Principles and
mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4.
OCLC 23254443 (https://search.worldcat.org/oclc/23254443) .

81. Hung, Woei (2013). "Team-based complex problem solving: a collective cognition perspective".
Educational Technology Research and Development. 61 (3): 365–384. doi:10.1007/s11423-013-
9296-3 (https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs11423-013-9296-3) . S2CID 62663840 (https://api.sema
nticscholar.org/CorpusID:62663840) .

82. Jewett, Pamela; MacPhee, Deborah (2012). "Adding Collaborative Peer Coaching to Our
Teaching Identities". The Reading Teacher. 66 (2): 105–110. doi:10.1002/TRTR.01089 (https://d
oi.org/10.1002%2FTRTR.01089) .

83. Wang, Qiyun (2009). "Design and Evaluation of a Collaborative Learning Environment".
Computers and Education. 53 (4): 1138–1146. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2009.05.023 (https://do
i.org/10.1016%2Fj.compedu.2009.05.023) .

84. Wang, Qiyan (2010). "Using online shared workspaces to support group collaborative learning".
Computers and Education. 55 (3): 1270–1276. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2010.05.023 (https://do
i.org/10.1016%2Fj.compedu.2010.05.023) .

85. Kai-Wai Chu, Samuel; Kennedy, David M. (2011). "Using Online Collaborative tools for groups to
Co-Construct Knowledge". Online Information Review. 35 (4): 581–597.
doi:10.1108/14684521111161945 (https://doi.org/10.1108%2F14684521111161945) .
ISSN 1468-4527 (https://search.worldcat.org/issn/1468-4527) . S2CID 206388086 (https://ap
i.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:206388086) .

86. Legare, Cristine; Mills, Candice; Souza, Andre; Plummer, Leigh; Yasskin, Rebecca (2013). "The
use of questions as problem-solving strategies during early childhood". Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology. 114 (1): 63–7. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.002 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.j
ecp.2012.07.002) . PMID 23044374 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23044374) .

87. Engelbart, Douglas (1962). "Team Cooperation" (https://www.dougengelbart.org/pubs/augmen


t-3906.html#3b9) . Augmenting Human Intellect: A Conceptual Framework. Vol. AFOSR-3223.
Stanford Research Institute.
88. Flew, Terry (2008). New Media: an introduction. Melbourne: Oxford University Press.

89. Henry, Jenkins. "Interactive audiences? The 'collective intelligence' of media fans" (https://web.
archive.org/web/20180426232104/https://labweb.education.wisc.edu/curric606/readings/Je
nkins2002.pdf) (PDF). Archived from the original (http://labweb.education.wisc.edu/curric60
6/readings/Jenkins2002.pdf) (PDF) on April 26, 2018. Retrieved December 11, 2016.

90. Finger, Matthias (2008-03-27). "Which governance for sustainable development? An


organizational and institutional perspective". In Park, Jacob; Conca, Ken; Finger, Matthias
(eds.). The Crisis of Global Environmental Governance: Towards a New Political Economy of
Sustainability. Routledge. p. 48 (https://books.google.com/books?id=lrr3K50r144C&pg=PA4
8) . ISBN 978-1-134-05982-9.

91. Guazzini, Andrea; Vilone, Daniele; Donati, Camillo; Nardi, Annalisa; Levnajić, Zoran (10
November 2015). "Modeling crowdsourcing as collective problem solving" (https://www.ncbi.nl
m.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4639727) . Scientific Reports. 5: 16557. arXiv:1506.09155 (http
s://arxiv.org/abs/1506.09155) . Bibcode:2015NatSR...516557G (https://ui.adsabs.harvard.ed
u/abs/2015NatSR...516557G) . doi:10.1038/srep16557 (https://doi.org/10.1038%2Fsrep1655
7) . PMC 4639727 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4639727) .
PMID 26552943 (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/26552943) .

Boroomand, A.; Smaldino, P.E. (2021). "Hard Work, Risk-Taking, and Diversity in a Model of
Collective Problem Solving" (https://doi.org/10.18564%2Fjasss.4704) . Journal of Artificial
Societies and Social Simulation. 24 (4). doi:10.18564/jasss.4704 (https://doi.org/10.18564%2Fj
asss.4704) . S2CID 240483312 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:240483312) .

92. Stefanovitch, Nicolas; Alshamsi, Aamena; Cebrian, Manuel; Rahwan, Iyad (30 September 2014).
"Error and attack tolerance of collective problem solving: The DARPA Shredder Challenge" (http
s://doi.org/10.1140%2Fepjds%2Fs13688-014-0013-1) . EPJ Data Science. 3 (1).
doi:10.1140/epjds/s13688-014-0013-1 (https://doi.org/10.1140%2Fepjds%2Fs13688-014-0013
-1) . hdl:21.11116/0000-0002-D39F-D (https://hdl.handle.net/21.11116%2F0000-0002-D39F-
D) .

Further reading

Beckmann, Jens F.; Guthke, Jürgen (1995). "Complex problem solving, intelligence, and learning
ability" (https://www.researchgate.net/publication/200134353) . In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J.
(eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 177–200.
Brehmer, Berndt (1995). "Feedback delays in dynamic decision making". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J.
(eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates. pp. 103–130.

Brehmer, Berndt; Dörner, D. (1993). "Experiments with computer-simulated microworlds: Escaping


both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study". Computers in
Human Behavior. 9 (2–3): 171–184. doi:10.1016/0747-5632(93)90005-D (https://doi.org/10.101
6%2F0747-5632%2893%2990005-D) .

Dörner, D. (1992). "Über die Philosophie der Verwendung von Mikrowelten oder
'Computerszenarios' in der psychologischen Forschung" [On the proper use of microworlds or
"computer scenarios" in psychological research]. In Gundlach, H. (ed.). Psychologische Forschung
und Methode: Das Versprechen des Experiments. Festschrift für Werner Traxel (in German). Passau,
Germany: Passavia-Universitäts-Verlag. pp. 53–87.

Eyferth, K.; Schömann, M.; Widowski, D. (1986). "Der Umgang von Psychologen mit Komplexität"
[On how psychologists deal with complexity]. Sprache & Kognition (in German). 5: 11–26.

Funke, Joachim (1993). "Microworlds based on linear equation systems: A new approach to
complex problem solving and experimental results" (https://web-archive.southampton.ac.uk/cogp
rints.org/1679/2/Funke_1993_micro.pdf) (PDF). In Strube, G.; Wender, K.-F. (eds.). The cognitive
psychology of knowledge. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. pp. 313–330.

Funke, Joachim (1995). "Experimental research on complex problem solving" (https://web-archiv


e.southampton.ac.uk/cogprints.org/3003/1/Funke_1995_CPS.pdf) (PDF). In Frensch, P. A.;
Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates. pp. 243–268.

Funke, U. (1995). "Complex problem solving in personnel selection and training". In Frensch, P. A.;
Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates. pp. 219–240.

Groner, M.; Groner, R.; Bischof, W. F. (1983). "Approaches to heuristics: A historical review". In
Groner, R.; Groner, M.; Bischof, W. F. (eds.). Methods of heuristics. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates. pp. 1–18.

Hayes, J. (1980). The complete problem solver. Philadelphia: The Franklin Institute Press.

Huber, O. (1995). "Complex problem solving as multistage decision making". In Frensch, P. A.;
Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates. pp. 151–173.
Hübner, Ronald (1989). "Methoden zur Analyse und Konstruktion von Aufgaben zur kognitiven
Steuerung dynamischer Systeme" (https://www.cogpsych.uni-konstanz.de/pdf/Hubner_1989_Met
hoden.pdf) [Methods for the analysis and construction of dynamic system control tasks] (PDF).
Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie (in German). 36: 221–238.

Hunt, Earl (1991). "Some comments on the study of complexity" (https://books.google.com/book


s?id=ZECYAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA383) . In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem
solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 383–395.
ISBN 978-1-317-78386-2.

Hussy, W. (1985). "Komplexes Problemlösen—Eine Sackgasse?" [Complex problem solving—a


dead end?]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie (in German). 32: 55–77.

Kluwe, R. H. (1993). "Chapter 19 Knowledge and Performance in Complex Problem Solving". The
Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge. Advances in Psychology. Vol. 101. pp. 401–423.
doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62668-0 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2FS0166-4115%2808%2962668-
0) . ISBN 978-0-444-89942-2.

Kluwe, R. H. (1995). "Single case studies and models of complex problem solving". In Frensch, P.
A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates. pp. 269–291.

Kolb, S.; Petzing, F.; Stumpf, S. (1992). "Komplexes Problemlösen: Bestimmung der
Problemlösegüte von Probanden mittels Verfahren des Operations Research—ein
interdisziplinärer Ansatz" [Complex problem solving: determining the quality of human problem
solving by operations research tools—an interdisciplinary approach]. Sprache & Kognition (in
German). 11: 115–128.

Krems, Josef F. (1995). "Cognitive flexibility and complex problem solving" (https://www.research
gate.net/publication/200134353) . In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving:
The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 201–218.

Melzak, Z. (1983). Bypasses: A Simple Approach to Complexity. London, UK: Wiley.

Müller, H. (1993). Komplexes Problemlösen: Reliabilität und Wissen [Complex problem solving:
Reliability and knowledge] (in German). Bonn, Germany: Holos.

Paradies, M.W.; Unger, L. W. (2000). TapRooT—The System for Root Cause Analysis, Problem
Investigation, and Proactive Improvement. Knoxville, Tenn.: System Improvements.

Putz-Osterloh, Wiebke (1993). "Chapter 15 Strategies for Knowledge Acquisition and Transfer of
Knowledge in Dynamic Tasks". The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge. Advances in Psychology.
Vol. 101. pp. 331–350. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62664-3 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2FS0166-
4115%2808%2962664-3) . ISBN 978-0-444-89942-2.

Riefer, David M.; Batchelder, William H. (1988). "Multinomial modeling and the measurement of
cognitive processes" (https://web.archive.org/web/20181125115507/https://pdfs.semanticschol
ar.org/9bf7/c2bb2a621983c0e590ea1f019bd1eb8029d4.pdf) (PDF). Psychological Review. 95
(3): 318–339. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.95.3.318 (https://doi.org/10.1037%2F0033-295x.95.3.31
8) . S2CID 14994393 (https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:14994393) . Archived from the
original (https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/9bf7/c2bb2a621983c0e590ea1f019bd1eb8029d4.pd
f) (PDF) on 2018-11-25.

Schaub, H. (1993). Modellierung der Handlungsorganisation (in German). Bern, Switzerland: Hans
Huber.

Strauß, B. (1993). Konfundierungen beim Komplexen Problemlösen. Zum Einfluß des Anteils der
richtigen Lösungen (ArL) auf das Problemlöseverhalten in komplexen Situationen [Confoundations in
complex problem solving. On the influence of the degree of correct solutions on problem solving in
complex situations] (in German). Bonn, Germany: Holos.

Strohschneider, S. (1991). "Kein System von Systemen! Kommentar zu dem Aufsatz


'Systemmerkmale als Determinanten des Umgangs mit dynamischen Systemen' von Joachim
Funke" [No system of systems! Reply to the paper 'System features as determinants of behavior
in dynamic task environments' by Joachim Funke]. Sprache & Kognition (in German). 10: 109–113.

Tonelli, Marcello (2011). Unstructured Processes of Strategic Decision-Making. Saarbrücken,


Germany: Lambert Academic Publishing. ISBN 978-3-8465-5598-9.

Van Lehn, Kurt (1989). "Problem solving and cognitive skill acquisition". In Posner, M. I. (ed.).
Foundations of cognitive science (https://apps.dtic.mil/dtic/tr/fulltext/u2/a218905.pdf) (PDF).
Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. pp. 527–579.

Wisconsin Educational Media Association (1993), Information literacy: A position paper on


information problem-solving (https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED376817) , WEMA Publications, vol. ED
376 817, Madison, Wis. (Portions adapted from Michigan State Board of Education's Position
Paper on Information Processing Skills, 1992.)

External links

Learning materials related to Solving Problems at Wikiversity

You might also like