Module 1 Psychology
Module 1 Psychology
Developmental psychology is the scientific study of how and why humans change over their
lifetimes. It focuses on the processes of growth, adaptation, and change that occur from
conception through old age . Early work in the field emphasized infancy and childhood, but
the scope has broadened to encompass adolescence, adulthood, and aging . Developmental
psychologists examine changes across physical, cognitive, and social-emotional domains. In
other words, they study what changes occur in people (such as intellectual or emotional
development), how those changes happen, and why they unfold as they do . The field asks
questions like how a child’s thought processes develop, how personality and social
relationships evolve, and how biological and environmental factors jointly influence
development. The emphasis on change over time distinguishes developmental psychology
from other subfields that focus on static traits or behaviors.
The formal study of human development has roots in philosophy and early science, but it
coalesced as a distinct discipline in the late 19th and early 20th centuries. Psychologists
began to examine children and adolescents scientifically around this time . For example, the
British philosopher John Locke (1632–1704) famously proposed that the newborn mind is a
“tabula rasa” or blank slate, shaped entirely by experience . In contrast, Jean-Jacques
Rousseau (1712–1778) argued that development is guided by innate stages (infancy,
childhood, adolescence) unfolding naturally . Charles Darwin’s evolutionary theory also
influenced early psychologists; Darwin’s notion of developmental “recapitulation” (that
human development replays evolutionary history) spurred interest in how early stages of life
shape the whole organism . These thinkers set the stage for modern theories and highlighted
the nature-versus-nurture problem (innate vs. learned influences).
During the 1930s–1950s, Jean Piaget’s theory of cognitive development became hugely
influential (see Module II), and the field increasingly integrated insights from biology,
sociology, and education. By the mid-20th century, developmental psychology had matured
into a broad field. Organizations like the Society for Research in Child Development (founded
1933) and numerous research journals were established. Since the 1970s, researchers have
extended developmental perspectives to older age and specialized topics (e.g., emotional
regulation, cultural variations) . In summary, developmental psychology evolved from a
narrow focus on childhood into a rich interdisciplinary science of lifespan development,
driven by a succession of seminal theories and methodological advances .
Development is rooted in biology, with heredity providing the basic blueprint. Each person
inherits chromosomes (23 pairs in humans) containing DNA from their parents. These genes
code for proteins that guide neural development, bodily growth, and all physiological
processes. From conception onward, genetic instructions influence development. For
example, genes regulate the formation of the brain’s neurons, the migration of cells to
different brain regions, and the timing of processes like myelination (insulating brain axons).
Although most genes are fixed at conception, their expression can be altered by environment
and development (as noted, the bioecological framework incorporates biology into
Bronfenbrenner’s model ).
Heredity, however, is not destiny. Genetic potential creates a range of possible outcomes,
and whether or how these potentials are realized depends on environmental factors. As one
review explains, “It’s as if one’s genetic make-up sets up a range of possibilities, which may
or may not be realized depending upon one’s environmental experiences” . For instance, a
child may inherit genes favoring tall stature, but nutrition in childhood will determine the
actual height reached. Similarly, someone might have genes linked to depression, but a
nurturing childhood or supportive relationships might mitigate that risk.
Behavioral genetics research methods, such as twin studies and adoption studies, illustrate
the contributions of heredity. These studies show that traits like temperament, intelligence,
and even patterns of social behavior have substantial genetic components, while also being
sensitive to environmental influences. A classic finding is that identical twins (sharing all
genes) tend to be more similar on many traits than fraternal twins (sharing ~50% of genes) .
Genetic disorders and chromosomal abnormalities further illustrate hereditary influences
(e.g. Down syndrome from extra chromosome 21 leads to intellectual disability).
Neuroscience also contributes to understanding biological bases: brain-imaging studies
reveal how specific brain regions and neural circuits underlie cognitive and emotional
development. For example, in adolescence, the limbic system (involved in emotion) matures
earlier than the prefrontal cortex (involved in reasoning), helping explain typical teenage
behavior patterns. Hormonal changes provide another biological influence—puberty’s
cascade of sex hormones triggers growth and reproductive maturity, while stress hormones
like cortisol affect temperament and anxiety.
In sum, developmental psychology integrates biology by recognizing that both our genetic
inheritance and physiological growth processes underlie development. Genes provide the
raw material (the genotype), while ongoing biological processes (brain maturation, hormone
levels, neural connectivity) shape how phenotypes (observable traits and behaviors)
emerge . Modern research emphasizes that these biological foundations always operate in
an environmental context, creating the dynamic interplay of nature and nurture discussed
above.
Theories of Development
Developmental psychology has been shaped by several influential theories, each offering a
framework for understanding how and why people change over time. We briefly review four
major approaches: psychoanalytic (Freud and Erikson), behaviorist (Watson and Skinner),
social learning (Bandura), and ecological (Bronfenbrenner).
Sigmund Freud’s psychoanalytic theory was one of the first systematic accounts of human
development . Freud proposed that personality and psychological development are driven by
unconscious motives and conflicts, rooted in early childhood experiences. According to
Freud, children progress through five psychosexual stages in which sexual energy (libido) is
focused on different erogenous zones of the body. For example, in the oral stage (birth to ~1
year), the mouth is the primary source of pleasure (through feeding and sucking); in the anal
stage (~1–3 years), control over bowel movements becomes central; the phallic stage (~3–6
years) involves attraction to the opposite-sex parent (the Oedipus/Electra complex); latency
(~6 to puberty) is a calm period of social learning; and the genital stage (puberty onward)
reawakens mature sexuality. Freud believed that successful resolution of each stage’s
conflict was essential for healthy personality development, whereas “fixation” (excessive or
insufficient gratification) could lead to lasting traits in adulthood. As one explanation notes,
Freud held that the id’s pleasure-seeking energies become focused on successive erogenous
areas and that problems (fixations) at a stage can later cause emotional difficulties . For
instance, an adult who was fixated at the oral stage might develop habits like overeating or
nail-biting, reflecting unresolved needs for oral stimulation.
Freud’s theory also introduces three structures of personality: the id (primitive desires), ego
(rational self), and superego (moral conscience). According to psychoanalysis, the ego must
navigate conflicts between the id and superego throughout development. Freud’s emphasis
on early childhood laid the groundwork for viewing development as a sequence of
qualitatively different periods. While many aspects of Freud’s theory have been criticized or
revised (for example, his emphasis on sexual motivation and his sample of clients), his notion
that early experiences shape later behavior is widely accepted.
(Freud’s psychosexual stages are often illustrated in a diagram, but in text we simply note
their order and significance . According to one source: “Freud believed that personality
developed through a series of childhood stages in which the id’s pleasure-seeking energies
become focused on certain erogenous areas” . Fixations in any stage can lead to personality
traits or neuroses linked to that stage.)
Erikson’s key contributions are twofold. First, he extended developmental theory into
adulthood, emphasizing that challenges and personality growth occur at all ages . Second,
he highlighted the role of social and cultural factors (family, peers, society) in development.
According to Erikson, a person’s social interactions and relationships critically influence their
identity formation and well-being . He also believed that successful resolution of a stage’s
crisis leads to psychological strengths: “If people successfully deal with the conflict, they
emerge with psychological strengths that will serve them for the rest of their lives” .
Conversely, failing to resolve a crisis can hamper development in that domain. For instance, a
teenager who does not achieve a coherent identity in Erikson’s “identity vs. role confusion”
stage may struggle with self-confidence and relationships.
Erikson’s theory is often summarized in tabular form (see sources ). In essence, it stresses
that development is a lifelong process of negotiating social demands and personal goals.
This perspective underscored the importance of studying adolescents, adults, and the
elderly within developmental psychology, and it linked psychological development to broader
cultural and interpersonal contexts .
The behaviorist approach, championed by John B. Watson and B.F. Skinner, asserts that
development results from learning via environmental experiences. Watson (often called the
father of behaviorism) argued in the early 20th century that psychology should focus on
observable behavior rather than the unconscious mind. He famously maintained that “give
me a dozen healthy infants…” and he could shape any into a specialist through conditioning
(emphasizing nurture) . In Watson’s view, all behavior (including emotional reactions and
personality traits) is learned through associations and reinforcement. For example, Watson’s
“Little Albert” experiment demonstrated that a child could be conditioned to fear a white rat
by pairing the rat with a loud noise. Watson’s dictum, based on Locke’s tabula rasa
philosophy, was that human behavior can be predicted and controlled by understanding the
stimuli that elicit responses .
Skinner later expanded behaviorist theory with his work on operant conditioning. He focused
on how the consequences of behavior (reinforcements or punishments) shape future
behavior. Skinner developed the “Skinner box” to rigorously study how rats or pigeons learn
to press levers or peck disks to receive food (positive reinforcement) or avoid shock
(negative reinforcement). He argued that development, much like learning in general, is a
matter of accumulating reinforced behaviors. In Skinner’s words, reinforcement and
punishment are major factors in driving behavior . For children, this implies that behaviors
parents reward (e.g. compliance, sharing) increase over time, while punished behaviors
decrease. The behaviorist perspective emphasizes environmental control: by systematically
rewarding desired behaviors, adults can promote desired developmental outcomes.
Both Watson and Skinner downplayed internal drives or stages, proposing instead that
general learning principles applied across all ages. In developmental terms, behaviorists
would encourage using reinforcement techniques to shape children’s development, and they
would study development empirically in laboratory settings. Today, pure behaviorism is less
prominent in developmental theory, but its legacy persists in practices like behavior
modification and in recognizing the power of learning environments (especially in early years)
for shaping development.
Albert Bandura’s social learning theory builds on behaviorism but adds a cognitive and social
dimension. Bandura proposed that learning (and by extension development) occurs not only
through direct reinforcement but also through observation and imitation of others. In other
words, children learn by modeling the behaviors of adults and peers. A classic example is
Bandura’s Bobo doll experiment: children who watched an adult act aggressively toward a
Bobo doll later imitated that aggressive behavior . This demonstrated that seeing a behavior
rewarded (or at least enacted) could teach children new behaviors without direct
reinforcement.
Bandura emphasized that attention, memory, and motivation are crucial. His theory suggests
that people selectively attend to others’ behaviors, remember them, and are motivated to
reproduce them if they believe it will lead to desirable outcomes. For instance, children who
see a model being praised for sharing will be more likely to imitate sharing than if the model
were punished for it. Bandura described his approach as a social cognitive theory because it
acknowledges internal thought processes. As one source explains, social learning theory
“accounts for the interaction of environmental and cognitive elements” in learning .
In developmental terms, Bandura’s theory highlights how social context shapes learning.
Children are not passive recipients; they actively encode and enact behaviors observed in
family, school, and media. Internal factors like self-efficacy (belief in one’s abilities) also
emerge from social learning processes. Thus, Bandura’s theory provides a bridge between
strict behaviorism and cognitive-developmental theories, underscoring that development is
partly learned through cultural and social modeling as well as personal experience .
For example, a child’s development is directly influenced by her family relationships and
school environment (microsystem). But if her parents work long hours (an exosystem factor),
this might affect the child’s home life. Cultural norms about parenting (macrosystem) also
shape how parents interact with the child. Over time, changes like parental divorce or the
advent of social media (chronosystem) alter the developmental context. Bronfenbrenner later
incorporated biology into his model (calling it bioecological) to acknowledge that genetics
and physiology also shape and are shaped by these environments .
Self-report methods involve asking people (or their proxies, such as parents) to describe
behaviors, thoughts, or feelings. Common tools are interviews, questionnaires, and
standardized surveys. For example, parents might fill out a check-list about a child’s
temperament, or older children might rate their own emotions on a survey. Self-reports allow
researchers to gather information from many participants quickly and directly access
subjective experiences. However, they depend on participants’ honesty and self-awareness.
One caveat is that what people say they do is not always what they actually do . Issues like
social desirability bias, memory errors, or misunderstanding questions can affect accuracy.
Researchers must therefore craft questions carefully and often supplement self-report with
other data. In developmental work, self-reports are valuable for understanding internal states
(e.g. children’s attitudes or perceptions) but are often combined with observations for a fuller
picture.
However, the trade-off is that findings from case studies cannot be generalized to all children
or populations . Because a case study focuses on non-random, idiosyncratic subjects, it is
hard to know whether the observed behaviors are typical. For instance, a case study of a
prodigious child who learned to read at age 2 provides fascinating data on early literacy, but
we cannot assume all toddlers will follow that pattern. In summary, case studies offer depth
and detail, but their conclusions must be interpreted cautiously. They are often used in
clinical or exploratory research, rather than to draw broad developmental laws .
Experiments are powerful because they can demonstrate causal links. As one summary
notes, the experimental method “looks at cause-and-effect relationships, and what happens
when a single variable is manipulated” . The Stanford Prison Experiment and Bandura’s Bobo
doll study are famous examples (though Bandura’s was quasi-experimental). In
developmental contexts, experiments have been used to study memory, reasoning, social
interactions, and more.
Nevertheless, experiments can be limited in realism. Highly controlled settings may not
reflect children’s natural environments, and some developmental questions (like ethical
implications of depriving nutrition) cannot be studied experimentally. Also, children often
behave differently under observation. Despite these limits, experimental methods remain a
cornerstone of developmental science when causal inference is needed .
However, longitudinal studies are time-consuming and costly. They risk participant attrition
(people dropping out over time), and the findings may be influenced by historical events
occurring during the study (cohort effects) . Still, when feasible, longitudinal research
provides invaluable insight into continuity and change, and can test developmental
hypotheses directly.
The major drawback of cross-sectional design is that age and cohort are confounded.
Differences between age groups might reflect generational differences rather than
development. As one source notes, cross-sectional data “does not allow the researcher to
distinguish between the impact of age and the cohort effect” . For example, if older children
perform differently, is it due to maturation or because they grew up in a different cultural
environment? Nevertheless, cross-sectional studies remain widely used for their practicality.
In summary, longitudinal designs shine in revealing how individuals change, while cross-
sectional designs excel at identifying age-related patterns quickly . Developmental
psychologists choose the design that best answers their question, often balancing depth
(longitudinal) against breadth and convenience (cross-sectional).