Теоретична Фонетика PDF
Теоретична Фонетика PDF
4. Phoneme – the shortest functional unit of a language. Each phoneme exists in speech in
the form of mutually non-distinctive speech sounds, its allophones. Each speech sound is
an allophone of some phoneme.
5. Sound – a material unit, produced by speech organs. A sound can be viewed from the
articulatory, acoustic, auditory and functional points of view.
6. Allophones – variants or members of one and the same phoneme, which never occur in
identical positions, but are said to be in complementary distribution, they are actual
speech sounds.
8. Diaphone – allophone of one and the same phoneme, pronounced by different people.
9. Diphthong – a vowel phoneme which consists of two elements: a nucleus and a glide.
The first element of a diphthong is more loud and distinct, the formation of the second
element of a diphthong is not accomplished. English diphthongs can be normal and
centring.
11.Triphthong – a vowel sound that consists of three elements. The first element is a
diphthong and the second is a neutral vowel |a|.
12.Glide – that part of a diphthong which constitutes its additional element, the full
articulation of which is not accomplished.
13.Enclitic – unstressed word or syllable, which refers to the preceding stressed word or
syllable.
15.Lexical stress – the relative prominence given to a specific syllable within a word.
16.Sentence stress – the greater degree of prominence given to certain words in a sentence.
23.Pitch – the degree of highness or lowness varying with the number of vibrations of a
note.
24.Assimilation – the result of coarticulation, when one sound is made similar to its
neighbor sound.
25.Types of assimilations:
Progressive – the process when the first of the two neighbouring sounds
influences the second and makes it similar to itself.
27.Liaison – connecting of the final sound of one word or syllable to the initial sound of the
next.
30.Glottal stop – a sound which reminds a slight cough and is articulated by the vocal
cords, before a vowel sound is heard in cases of emphatic speech.
31.Nasal plosion – plosion formed when the soft palate is separated from the back wall of
the nasal pharynx and the air quickly escapes through the nasal cavity.
TOPICS
1. Phonetics as a Branch of Linguistics
Phonetics studies the human sounds that express thought and their functions in relation to
meaning.
It is divided into practical (normative) and theoretical phonetics, with the latter focusing on
the functioning of phonetic units in language.
The course will primarily focus on Received Pronunciation and the pronunciation norms of
English, which vary in interpretation among scholars.
Phonetics emphasizes the expression level of language while also considering how sounds
affect meaning; understanding phonetics is essential for any linguistic study.
Articulatory phonetics: Examines how speech sounds are produced by the vocal
organs and air motion.
Auditory phonetics: Focuses on how speech sounds are perceived by the listener's
auditory system, emphasizing sensations of sound rather than psychological processes.
Phonology, on the other hand, studies how phonemes and prosodic features systematically
express meaning and compares different sound systems within languages.
Phonology identifies phoneme patterns, addresses phonological universals, and solves
problems related to phoneme identification and distribution within words.
Introduction to Pronunciation
Pronunciation encompasses both articulation features and broader discourse-related
features.
It relates to several components, including the sound system, syllabic structure, word stress,
and intonation.
The notion of speech is distinguished from language; speech is the practical act of
communication, while language represents the cognitive understanding and system used for
transmitting messages.
1. Segmental/Phonemic Component
The segmental component refers to the smallest units of sound in a language, known as
phonemes. These phonemes are critical as they can differentiate meanings in morphemes,
words, and sentences.
2. Syllabic Structure
A syllable is a unit of spoken language that is larger than a single sound but smaller than a
word. It plays a crucial role in both speech production and perception.
Word stress refers to the emphasis placed on certain syllables within words, which can
affect meaning and comprehension.
Lexical Stress: The amount of effort used in producing a syllable, which can change
the meaning of words.
Patterns of Stress: Different languages have varying rules for stress placement,
influencing rhythm and flow in speech.
4. Intonation
Intonation involves the variation in pitch while speaking, which can convey different
meanings or emotions.
Prosodic Features: These include pitch, loudness, and tempo, which add layers of
meaning beyond the individual sounds.
The use of pitch contours can differentiate between statements and questions, reflecting the
speaker's intent.
Sentence stress refers to the perceptual prominence given to certain words, influenced by
grammatical and situational context.
Speech tempo also affects meaning, with variations conveying urgency or emotional
involvement.
Phonetics studies the human sounds that express thought and their functions in relation to
meaning.
It is divided into practical (normative) and theoretical phonetics, with the latter focusing on
the functioning of phonetic units in language.
The course will primarily focus on Received Pronunciation and the pronunciation norms of
English, which vary in interpretation among scholars.
Phonetics emphasizes the expression level of language while also considering how sounds
affect meaning; understanding phonetics is essential for any linguistic study.
Articulatory phonetics: Examines how speech sounds are produced by the vocal
organs and air motion.
Auditory phonetics: Focuses on how speech sounds are perceived by the listener's
auditory system, emphasizing sensations of sound rather than psychological processes.
Phonology, on the other hand, studies how phonemes and prosodic features systematically
express meaning and compares different sound systems within languages.
Introduction to Pronunciation
Pronunciation encompasses both articulation features and broader discourse-related
features.
It relates to several components, including the sound system, syllabic structure, word stress,
and intonation.
The notion of speech is distinguished from language; speech is the practical act of
communication, while language represents the cognitive understanding and system used for
transmitting messages.
1. Segmental/Phonemic Component
The segmental component refers to the smallest units of sound in a language, known as
phonemes. These phonemes are critical as they can differentiate meanings in morphemes,
words, and sentences.
2. Syllabic Structure
A syllable is a unit of spoken language that is larger than a single sound but smaller than a
word. It plays a crucial role in both speech production and perception.
Word stress refers to the emphasis placed on certain syllables within words, which can
affect meaning and comprehension.
Lexical Stress: The amount of effort used in producing a syllable, which can change
the meaning of words.
Patterns of Stress: Different languages have varying rules for stress placement,
influencing rhythm and flow in speech.
4. Intonation
Intonation involves the variation in pitch while speaking, which can convey different
meanings or emotions.
Prosodic Features: These include pitch, loudness, and tempo, which add layers of
meaning beyond the individual sounds.
The use of pitch contours can differentiate between statements and questions, reflecting the
speaker's intent.
Sentence stress refers to the perceptual prominence given to certain words, influenced by
grammatical and situational context.
Speech tempo also affects meaning, with variations conveying urgency or emotional
involvement.
The term "sound" can be interpreted in two distinct ways when discussing a language.
One interpretation involves the phonemic aspect, as seen in the examples of [t] and
[d], where they create meaningful distinctions (e.g., "tie" vs. "die" and "seat" vs.
"seed").
The phoneme comprises three key aspects: material, abstract, and functional, demonstrating
its multifaceted nature in language.
Definition of Phonemes
Phoneme: The smallest indivisible unit of sound that can differentiate meaning in speech.
Allophone: Variants of a phoneme that do not create meaningful distinctions and occur in
different environments.
Example: Distinction between [d] in "bread" (alveolar) and [d] in "breadth" (dental) may
not be consciously perceived.
Invariant of the Phoneme: The core set of articulatory features that must remain
unchanged to preserve meaning.
Transcription of Phonemes
Broad (Phonemic) Transcription: Represents phonemes with symbols, focusing on
functional aspects.
4. Classification of consonants
Two main classes of consonants are distinguished: occlusive (complete obstruction) and
constrictive (incomplete obstruction).
Each class (occlusive and constrictive) further divides into noise consonants and sonorants
based on whether the sound features a dominant noise or tone component.
Noise consonants are further divided into plosives (stops) and affricates.
Some British phoneticians classify certain sonorants like [r], [j], and [w] as semivowels due
to their vowel-like characteristics.
While functionally, [r], [j], and [w] are considered consonantal based on their position in
syllables, they are phonetically described as vowel glides.
Place of Articulation
The place of articulation classifies English consonants based on the active organ of speech
against the point of obstruction, resulting in categories such as labial, lingual, and glottal.
In articulatory terms, voiced consonants are noted for their weak (lenis) quality, while
voiceless consonants are considered strong (fortis).
Additionally, consonants can be classified as oral or nasal based on the position of the soft
palate. Nasal occlusive sonorants, such as [m], [n], and [ŋ], allow airflow into the nasal cavity
due to the lowered position of the soft palate.
Affricates in English
The phonological status and classification of affricates, specifically [ʧ] (as in "chop") and
[ʤ] (as in "judge"), raise questions about whether they are single phonemic entities or
biphonemic combinations.
There is some debate regarding the number of affricates in English. According to standard
views, English has two: [ʧ] and [ʤ].
D. Jones suggests six, including [ts, ʤ] and [tr, dr], while A.C. Gimson adds [tθ, dð] to this
list.
N.S. Trubetskoy states that a sound complex can be nonphonemic if it meets three criteria:
the elements are in the same syllable, produced with one effort, and have a duration that doesn't
exceed normal limits for each element.
Assimilation occurs when a sound changes to become more like a neighboring sound,
primarily affecting plosives and nasals.
Examples include the dental sounds [t] and [d] influenced by interdental sounds [θ] and [ð]
(e.g., "eighth," "at the," "said that"), and post-alveolar sounds [t] and [d] influenced by [r] (e.g.,
"free," "that right word").
Loss of plosion in sequences of plosive consonants (e.g., "glad to see you"), where
one sound loses its plosion.
Lateral plosion occurs when a plosive is followed by a lateral sonorant [l], changing
its sound production (e.g., "settle," "table").
Voicing Changes During Assimilation
Voicing features may shift during assimilation, where lenis voiced sounds can become
voiceless fortis when followed by voiceless consonants.
In informal speech, voicing changes might be heard in phrases like "have to do it"
becoming ['hæf tə 'du:] and "five past two" ['faif past 'tu:].
Weak forms of "is" and "has" merge with preceding voiceless consonants (e.g., "What’s
your name?" and "Your aunt's coming.").
Spread lip positions occur with front vowels, while slight nasalization in vowels may occur
due to neighboring sonorants (e.g., "and," "morning").
The loss of [h] in "he," "her," and auxiliary verbs like "have."
Alveolar plosives being omitted, as seen in "next day" ['neks 'dei'] or "just one" ['ʤʌs
'wʌn'].
Linking "r" occurs commonly, seen in words like "clearer" and in phrases where word-final
vowels are followed by vowels.
Insertion may include sounds [j] or [w] for diphthongs gliding, like "saying" ['seijiŋ'] and
"going" ['gəuwiŋ'].
Such insertions serve to make articulation easier rather than provide clarity for the listener.
The term "accent" is often used in phonetic literature alongside stress, specifically relating
to the pitch component of syllable prominence. However, stress encompasses a broader range
of attributes beyond just pitch.
Even within single words, pitch changes can create an accent effect, which is also present in
utterances or phrases, highlighting the connection between accent and intonation.
The study of word stress can be approached from both production and perception
perspectives, with stressed syllables requiring more muscular energy, resulting in higher
subglottal pressure and increased intensity, duration, and frequency.
Dynamic stress, observed in languages such as English, German, and Russian, relies on
greater force of pronunciation, leading to notable intensity and duration.
Tonic (musical) stress, found in languages like Chinese and Japanese, is characterized by
changes in pitch rather than forceful pronunciation.
The balance of stress components (intensity, duration, pitch) may vary across different
languages, influencing how stress is perceived and produced.
Loudness is a significant factor in stress perception; stressed syllables are often heard as
louder than unstressed ones.
Pitch change plays a crucial role in identifying stressed syllables; a high pitch can signal
stress, regardless of whether it rises or falls.
Length is another critical element, as stressed syllables generally possess greater length due
to increased energy in production compared to unstressed syllables.
Vowel quality impacts stress perception; stressed syllables typically contain vowels that are
less reduced than those in unstressed syllables, enhancing their prominence.
Prosody is viewed as a complex whole formed by variations in pitch, loudness, tempo, and
rhythm, with speech timbre sometimes included by linguists.
The primary elements affecting intonation are pitch (fundamental frequency), loudness
(amplitude of vibrations), and tempo (duration of speech units and pauses).
Each syllable in speech has a unique pitch, with variations in tone creating an intonation
pattern, a fundamental unit that consists of a nucleus and may include other syllables.
Intonation patterns help to emphasize syntagms, which are groups of words that are
complete both semantically and syntactically.
Roger Kingdon's classification of nuclear tones includes Kinetic (moving) tones, where
pitch changes, and Static tones, where pitch remains constant. Examples include Low Fall,
High Fall, Low Rise, High Rise, Fall-Rise, Rise-Fall, and Mid-Level tones.
The Low Fall indicates calm finality, the High Fall expresses warmth and interest, while the
Low Rise conveys hesitation and incompleteness.
The High Rise is often used in questions indicating a search for information, while the Fall-
Rise tone can suggest contradiction or correction of earlier statements.
Each tone has specific emotional implications, e.g., the Rise-Fall suggests deep impression,
and the Mid-Level tone expresses non-finality without expectation.
Pauses serve to separate phonetic wholes, phrases, and intonation groups, with short,
longer, and very long pauses differentiating between them.
Syntactic pauses organize speech units; emphatic pauses highlight specific utterances, while
hesitation pauses are used in spontaneous speech to allow the speaker to gather thoughts.
Each method has advantages and disadvantages, with some being more cumbersome than
others; for instance, the method favored by Bolinger requires a specially designed print model.
8. Definition of Intonation
Intonation in Language
Intonation is a significant aspect of suprasegmental phonetics, which involves more than
just individual speech sounds; it encompasses other phonetic means that characterize sequences
of speech.
Every language exhibits intonation, and none are spoken in a monotone; however, the
function of intonation can vary across different languages.
Intonation can be analyzed at three levels: acoustic (characteristics like frequency and
intensity), perception (human interpretation), and linguistic (meanings conveyed through
intonation).
An apt analogy for intonation's complexity is its similarity to ocean waves, as described by
Bolinger (1972).
Approaches to Intonation
There are multiple approaches to defining and describing intonation, reflecting its intricate
nature.
In contrast, the British and American traditions primarily associate intonation with pitch
variation, noting its prominence in communication, as evidenced by definitions from British
phoneticians like D. Jones (1976) and A. Cruttenden (1986).
The term "prosody" encompasses the variations in pitch, loudness, and tempo alongside
intonation and is a frequent term in linguistic literature.
British phoneticians see prosody as a more comprehensive term than intonation, including
rhythm as a key feature alongside the other prosodic characteristics established by linguists like
D. Crystal and J. Wells.
For consistency in teaching practice, the term "intonation" will be primarily used, while
also acknowledging relevant prosodic features where appropriate.
Key Concepts
Kinetic Tones: Pitch varies during the tone's duration, moving upwards or
downwards.
Static Tones: Pitch remains constant throughout the tone, including High Level
Tones and Low Level Tones.
Tone Meanings
Falling Tone: Conveys certainty, completeness, and independence.
• Example: Questions commonly use a rising tone due to uncertainty about the
answer.
Types of Tones
1. Low Fall
Tone Mark: ↘
2. High Fall
Tone Mark: ↘
3. Low Rise
Tone Mark: ↗
Conveys: Non-finality and encouragement.
4. High Rise
Tone Mark: ↗
5. Fall-Rise
Tone Mark: ↘↗
6. Rise-Fall
Tone Mark: ↗↘
7. Mid-Level
There are two views among phoneticians regarding how stressed and unstressed syllables
are grouped: one view emphasizes the semantic principle, where unstressed syllables associate
with the stressed syllable of the same word or a semantically related lexical unit.
The alternate view is the enclitic principle, suggesting that unstressed syllables attach to the
preceding stressed syllable, resulting in the same phrase being divided into rhythmic groups
differently.
The enclitic tendency is more typical in rapid colloquial speech, while the semantic
grouping becomes more prevalent during careful, slow speech.
The rhythmic group is recognized as the fundamental unit of rhythm across all text types,
though descriptions of rhythm extend beyond just this unit.
This hierarchy includes various levels of rhythmic units such as rhythmic groups, intonation
groups, phrases, and phonopassages.
Rhythm in speech results from the interaction between lexical, syntactic, and prosodic
elements, which are critical to marking every text segment.
Each type of text may exhibit different rhythmic characteristics due to the integration of
these elements across the hierarchy of units.
The broader understanding of rhythm enhances the analysis of speech beyond just the
concept of rhythmic groups, emphasizing the complexity and dynamics of spoken language.
Local accents may share many pronunciation features, leading to groupings like the
"Northern accent," which includes accents from Yorkshire, Lancashire, and Cheshire in Britain.
Many phonetics textbooks still use "dialect" to refer to regional pronunciation, although
recent editions distinguish between "dialects" and "accents."
There is significant overlap between the terms "dialects" and "accents," and one dialect can
become the standard language due to various factors. The London dialect's accent became the
"RP" (Received Pronunciation) associated with Britain.
Individuals may use an imitation of RP when interacting with teachers and revert to their
native accent among peers, demonstrating a state known as diglossia.
Diglossia involves using the standard language and a regional dialect in different social
situations, differentiated from bilingualism, which entails command over two distinct
languages.
The social differentiation of language correlates with the social differentiation of society,
although it is essential to note that linguistic facts cannot be directly attributed to class
structure.
Every linguistic community, whether small or large, possesses its own social dialect and
corresponding social accent, reflecting the community's unique linguistic identity.
Although final consonants are devoiced when following a vowel, they are not identical to
their fully voiceless counterparts, which are articulated with greater breath-force.
The voiced/voiceless distinction in minimal pairs like [sed] vs. [set] and [dog] vs. [dok]
may seem absent; however, the weak consonants do not convert to their voiceless forms and
remain devoiced without becoming aspirated.
There is a close interrelation between final consonants and the preceding stressed vowels,
illustrated by the pairs "cap" and "cab."
A. C. Gimson classifies initial lenis stops [b, d, g] as partially devoiced, while final stops
are categorized as voiceless. In intervocalic positions, the sound [t] is realized as voiced, as
seen in "better" ['betə] compared to "letter" ['letǝ].
Loss of [h]
The initial [h] sound is commonly lost in rapid speech, particularly in function words, and
may be entirely omitted even by educated speakers. For example, "She wants her to come" may
be pronounced as [i: wonts 3 tə kam].
The loss of [h] is particularly noticeable in unstressed syllables, while its omission in
stressed syllables is generally perceived as incorrect.
Initial "hw"
Some conservative RP speakers maintain an initial breathy [h] sound in words like "why,"
"when," and "which." However, the general trend is to articulate these with [w].
Vocalization of final [l] is occasionally observed, especially in words like "people," but this
trend is not expected to establish itself as a norm.
Glottal Stop
In RP, the glottal stop can be produced in two contexts: as a realization of syllable-final [t]
before another consonant, e.g., "batman" ['bætman] becoming ['bæ?mn'], and in specific
consonant clusters.
English accents are categorized into "rhotic" (those that pronounce [r]) and "non-rhotic"
(those that do not, such as RP). However, RP can exhibit linking [r] when it appears before a
vowel, leading to phrases like "It is a faraway country."
The intrusive [r] phenomenon involves inserting “r” before a vowel in certain contexts,
despite its absence in spelling, such as “idea of” and “China and.”
There is a noticeable trend towards elision, reduction, and assimilation in colloquial speech
among younger RP speakers. Examples include "tutor" ['tfu:tə] and "second year" ['sekəndzıə].
Combinative changes occur where sound combinations like [tj] and [dj] are articulated as [t]
and [dʒ], respectively. This is evident in words like "actual" ['æktjuəl] and "graduate"
['grædjuǝıt].
The loss of [j] frequently occurs after sounds like [l, s, z] as in "suit" [su:t] and after [n] as
in "news" [nu:z].